Emotion expression

情感表达
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尽管儿童在情感表达上表现出强烈的个体差异,他们还与互动伙伴进行情感同步或互惠。为了理解性状样影响和二元影响之间的悖论,本研究的目的是使用社会关系模型(SRM)方法调查儿童的情绪表达。通常由四个同性陌生的9岁儿童(N=202)组成的游戏组以循环形式(每组6个二元组)进行互动。每个二元完成两个5分钟的任务,具有挑战性的挫折任务和合作计划任务。观察者在任务中编码孩子的情绪(快乐,悲伤,生气,焦虑,中性)。SRM分析提供了儿童情绪表达的特质样性质的实质性证据(通过对演员方差的显著影响,多变量行动者-行动者相关性,和多变量内部相关性)及其情绪表达的二重性质(通过对伴侣方差的显著影响,关系方差,二进互惠相关,和多元人际相关性)。
    Although children display strong individual differences in emotion expression, they also engage in emotional synchrony or reciprocity with interaction partners. To understand this paradox between trait-like and dyadic influences, the goal of the current study was to investigate children\'s emotion expression using a Social Relations Model (SRM) approach. Playgroups consisting typically of four same-sex unfamiliar nine-year-old children (N = 202) interacted in a round-robin format (6 dyads per group). Each dyad completed two 5-minute tasks, a challenging frustration task and a cooperative planning task. Observers coded children\'s emotions during the tasks (happy, sad, angry, anxious, neutral) on a second-by-second basis. SRM analyses provided substantial evidence of both the trait-like nature of children\'s emotion expression (through significant effects for actor variance, multivariate actor-actor correlations, and multivariate intrapersonal correlations) and the dyadic nature of their emotion expression (through significant effects for partner variance, relationship variance, dyadic reciprocity correlations, and multivariate interpersonal correlations).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    就像接受情感能力一样,富有成效的情感能力对于社会交往至关重要。尽管接受情绪能力存在个体差异,比如感知和识别情绪,经过充分调查,生产情绪能力的个体差异,比如面部表达情绪的能力,在很大程度上被忽视了。因此,关于情感表达能力如何适应相关能力和典型行为的法理网络,人们知之甚少。我们开发了一种多任务电池,用于测量面部表情识别软件评分的情绪表情的能力。通过三个多变量研究(n1=237;n2=141;n3=123),我们测试了情绪姿势的竞争性测量模型,并将这种结构与其他社会情绪特征和认知能力联系起来。我们复制了测量模型,其中包括情绪构成的一般因素,嵌套的特定于任务的因素,和特定于情绪的因素。情绪构成能力因子与接受社会情绪能力中等到强相关,与一般认知能力弱相关,和外向性弱相关。这有力地证明了情绪摆姿势是一种认知人际交往能力。这种对情感交流能力的新理解为研究社交互动中的个体差异打开了大门。
    Just as receptive emotional abilities, productive emotional abilities are essential for social communication. Although individual differences in receptive emotional abilities, such as perceiving and recognizing emotions, are well-investigated, individual differences in productive emotional abilities, such as the ability to express emotions in the face, are largely neglected. Consequently, little is known about how emotion expression abilities fit in a nomological network of related abilities and typical behavior. We developed a multitask battery for measuring the ability to pose emotional expressions scored with facial expression recognition software. With three multivariate studies (n1 = 237; n2 = 141; n3 = 123), we test competing measurement models of emotion posing and relate this construct with other socio-emotional traits and cognitive abilities. We replicate the measurement model that includes a general factor of emotion posing, a nested task-specific factor, and emotion-specific factors. The emotion-posing ability factor is moderately to strongly related to receptive socio-emotional abilities, weakly related to general cognitive abilities, and weakly related to extraversion. This is strong evidence that emotion posing is a cognitive interpersonal ability. This new understanding of abilities in emotion communication opens a gateway for studying individual differences in social interaction.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    管理负面情绪对于自闭症患者和他们的家庭来说,从年轻的时候起就具有挑战性。父母通过安慰或支持的方式帮助幼儿管理负面情绪。很少有研究研究了自闭症儿童的负面情绪和父母对负面情绪的反应如何不同。这项研究使用了18个月大的幼儿和父母在家的录像带。我们研究了幼儿在日常生活中表达负面情绪的程度和程度,以及父母的反应。参与者是自闭症儿童的弟弟妹妹,我们比较了三组儿童:(1)后来接受自闭症诊断的儿童;(2)有语言延迟,但没有自闭症;(3)没有延迟或自闭症。我们发现,与没有延误或自闭症的儿童相比,自闭症幼儿的负面情绪更可能是强烈的,并且一旦开始就会持续下去。语言延迟的幼儿也表现出一些,但并非所有这些差异。父母对所有组的负面情绪的反应相似。当父母使用策略来帮助时,它减少了负面情绪持续的机会,虽然它可能对自闭症幼儿没有帮助。这项研究表明,自闭症儿童可能会从小就表现出更强烈和持久的负面情绪。这也表明自闭症儿童的父母对孩子的负面情绪非常敏感,但是这些反应可能对自闭症儿童没有帮助。虽然需要更多的研究,这项研究帮助我们了解自闭症幼儿如何以不同的方式表达和体验情绪。
    UNASSIGNED: Managing negative emotion can be challenging for autistic individuals and their families from a young age. Parents help young children manage negative emotions by responding in comforting or supportive ways. Not much research has examined how negative emotions and parent responses to negative emotions are different in very young autistic children. This study used videotapes of 18-month-old toddlers and parents at home. We examined how much and how intensely toddlers expressed negative emotion in everyday situations, and how parents responded. Participants were younger siblings of autistic children, and we compared three groups-children that (1) later received an autism diagnosis; (2) had language delays but not autism; and (3) had no delays or autism. We found that autistic toddlers\' negative emotion was more likely to be intense and to continue once it started compared with children without delays or autism. Language-delayed toddlers also showed some, but not all these differences. Parents responded similarly to negative emotions in all groups. When parents used strategies to help, it reduced the chances of the negative emotions continuing, although it may have been less helpful for autistic toddlers. This study shows that autistic children may express more intense and long-lasting negative emotions from an early age. It also shows that parents of autistic children are very responsive to their children\'s negative emotions, but these responses may not be as helpful to autistic children. While more research is needed, this study helps us understand how autistic toddlers may express and experience emotions differently.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:情感体验和表达的差异长期以来被认为在自闭症的表现中很常见,然而,研究儿童早期情绪表达的研究是有限的,结果喜忧参半。了解自闭症早期的情绪反应和表达是揭示这些风险机制和确定干预目标的重要一步。
    方法:本研究检查了自闭症(N=17)和非自闭症(N=20)幼儿(平均年龄=25.27;SD=1.88)在情感激发任务中的情感表达,目的是激发喜悦,挫败感,和不安。视频记录的任务以十秒的间隔进行编码,以确定情绪效价和强度,并计算了以下变量:时间比例为正,中性,和负面影响;正面和负面影响的最大强度;和影响范围(即,最消极到最积极的强度)。
    结果:自闭症幼儿花更多时间在中性的面部表情上,显示积极影响的时间较少,与非自闭症的同龄人相比,积极的情感表达有所减弱。负面影响表达的强度差异很小,而在负面影响中花费的时间没有差异。
    结论:研究结果强调,差异在持续时间上可能更明显,而不是情感表达的强度,检查自闭症儿童的“中性”影响时期可能特别重要。未来的研究应该考虑到他们的独特兴趣,考虑到他们的情绪反应性,考虑到最好的方法。挑战,和沟通风格。
    OBJECTIVE: Differences in emotional experience and expression have long been recognized as common in the presentation of autism, yet research examining emotional expression in early childhood is limited, with mixed findings. Understanding emotional reactivity and expression in autism in early life is an essential step towards uncovering the mechanisms of these risks and identifying targets for intervention.
    METHODS: The present study examined emotional expression in autistic (N = 17) and non-autistic (N = 20) toddlers (mean age = 25.27; SD = 1.88) during emotion elicitation tasks aimed at eliciting joy, frustration, and unease. Video recorded tasks were coded in ten second intervals for emotional valence and intensity, and the following variables were computed: proportion of time in positive, neutral, and negative affect; maximum intensity of positive and negative affect; and range of affect (i.e., most negative to most positive intensity).
    RESULTS: Autistic toddlers spent more time in neutral facial expressions, less time displaying positive affect, and had somewhat less intense positive emotional expression than non-autistic peers. Small differences were apparent in intensity of negative affect expression, while no differences emerged in duration of time spent in negative affect.
    CONCLUSIONS: Findings emphasize that differences may be more apparent in duration, rather than intensity of emotional expression, and that it may be particularly important to examine periods of \"neutral\" affect in young autistic children. Future research should consider the best ways to understand emotional reactivity in this population considering their unique interests, challenges, and communication styles.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    孩子们在学龄前对男性与女性的某些情绪的适当性形成了刻板的期望,基于他们社会环境的线索。尽管大量研究已经检查了儿童性别陈规定型观念的发展,对儿童性别定型情绪处理的神经反应知之甚少。因此,当前的研究检查了3岁儿童在违反性别刻板印象的情绪刺激的神经处理方面是否存在差异(即,带有恐惧或快乐表情的男性面孔)或确认性别刻板印象(即,带有恐惧或快乐表情的女性面孔),以及男孩和女孩在违反和确认性别定型观念的神经处理方面是否存在差异。来自72名3岁儿童的数据(±6个月,43%的男孩)来自YOUth队列研究。当儿童被动地查看显示中性的男性和女性面孔时,获得了脑电图数据,快乐,或者可怕的面部表情。这项研究提供了第一个迹象,表明学龄前儿童中快乐的男性面孔比快乐的女性面孔引起更大的P1幅度,这可能反映出违反性别刻板印象的刺激的注意力处理增加。此外,有初步证据表明女孩有更大的负中央(Nc)反应,与显著性处理相关,女性快乐的面孔比男性快乐的面孔,而男孩对男性快乐面孔的Nc反应要大于女性快乐面孔。在处理中性和恐惧的面部表情时没有发现性别差异。我们的结果表明,脑电图测量可以提供对学龄前儿童关于情绪的性别刻板印象知识的见解,可能通过观察枕骨早期和前脑中央晚期的反应。
    Children form stereotyped expectations about the appropriateness of certain emotions for men versus women during the preschool years, based on cues from their social environments. Although ample research has examined the development of gender stereotypes in children, little is known about the neural responses that underlie the processing of gender-stereotyped emotions in children. Therefore, the current study examined whether 3-year-olds differ in the neural processing of emotional stimuli that violate gender stereotypes (i.e., male faces with fearful or happy expressions) or confirm gender stereotypes (i.e., female faces with fearful or happy expressions), and whether boys and girls differ in their neural processing of the violation and confirmation of gender stereotypes. Data from 72 3-year-olds (±6 months, 43% boy) were obtained from the YOUth Cohort Study. Electroencephalography data were obtained when children passively viewed male and female faces displaying neutral, happy, or fearful facial expressions. This study provided first indications that happy male faces elicited larger P1 amplitudes than happy female faces in preschool children, which might reflect increased attentional processing of stimuli that violate gender stereotypes. Moreover, there was preliminary evidence that girls had larger negative central (Nc) responses, associated with salience processing, toward female happy faces than male happy faces, whereas boys had larger Nc responses toward male happy faces than female happy faces. No gender differences were found in the processing of neutral and fearful facial expressions. Our results indicate that electroencephalography measurements can provide insights into preschoolers\' gender-stereotype knowledge about emotions, potentially by looking at the early occipital and late fronto-central responses.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Systematic Review
    酒精与两者都呈阳性(例如,社交能力)和消极(例如,侵略)社会结果,研究人员提出,酒精引起的情绪识别变化可能部分解释了这些影响。这里,我们系统地回顾了酒精管理研究,以阐明酒精对情绪识别的急性影响。我们还研究了各种主持人变量(即,性别,学习质量,研究设计,酒精用量,情绪识别任务和结果测量)。PsycINFO,根据预先注册的PROSPERO协议(CRD42021225392)和PRISMA方法搜索PubMed和GoogleScholar。分析的重点是消费酒精和/或非酒精的参与者之间的情绪识别差异(即,安慰剂或无酒精控制)饮料。19个独特样本(N=1271名参与者)来自17篇文章(两篇文章包括两项研究,每个都在一个独特的样本上进行)。提取样本特征的数据,酒精管理方法和情绪识别任务和结果。所有研究都将酒精饮料与安慰剂饮料进行了比较,并使用了要求参与者从面部表情的图像或视频中识别情绪的任务。否则,不同的研究方法差异很大,包括测试的酒精剂量,使用的特定情绪识别任务和评估的结果变量。对于任何情绪,酒精对情绪识别没有一致的影响。主持人变量都不会影响调查结果,除了一些迹象表明酒精可能比女性更影响男性的情绪识别能力。酒精似乎不会持续影响面部表情的正面或负面情绪识别,至少与当前在该领域使用的任务有关。
    Alcohol has been linked to both positive (e.g., sociability) and negative (e.g., aggression) social outcomes, and researchers have proposed that alcohol-induced changes in emotion recognition may partially explain these effects. Here, we systematically review alcohol administration studies to clarify the acute effects of alcohol on emotion recognition. We also investigate various moderator variables (i.e., sex, study quality, study design, alcohol dosage, emotion recognition task and outcome measure). PsycINFO, PubMed and Google Scholar were searched following a pre-registered PROSPERO protocol (CRD42021225392) and PRISMA methodology. Analyses focused on differences in emotion recognition between participants consuming alcoholic and/or non-alcoholic (i.e., placebo or no-alcohol control) beverages. Nineteen unique samples (N = 1271 participants) were derived from 17 articles (two articles included two studies, each conducted on a unique sample). Data were extracted for sample characteristics, alcohol administration methods and emotion recognition tasks and outcomes. All studies compared an alcoholic beverage to a placebo beverage and used tasks that asked participants to identify emotions from images or videos of facial expressions. Otherwise, methodologies varied substantially across studies, including the alcohol dosage(s) tested, the specific emotion recognition task(s) used and the outcome variable(s) assessed. No consistent effects of alcohol on emotion recognition emerged for any emotion. None of the moderator variables affected the findings, except for some indication that alcohol may affect males\' emotion recognition abilities more so than females. Alcohol does not appear to consistently affect positive or negative emotion recognition of facial expressions, at least with the tasks currently used in the field.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    以计算机为媒介的护理正变得越来越流行,但是关于它及其对情绪相关结果的影响的研究很少。这篇系统的文献综述旨在创建一个概述,解决研究问题:“计算机介导的护理和情感表达之间是否存在关系,感知,和情绪和(长期)情绪结果?\“
    本系统文献综述是根据系统综述和荟萃分析(PRISMA)指南的首选报告项目进行的,并使用了五个合格标准,即,(1)参与者:寻求支持的成年人;(2)干预:eHealth;(3)诊断标准:诊断困难的概念识别,表达,和/或调节情绪(例如,述情障碍);(4)比较:面对面护理或无比较;(5)研究设计:定量研究或定性研究。使用QualSyst工具评估质量。
    分析包括25篇研究论文。自我节奏干预似乎对情绪调节有积极作用。视频会议干预改善了治疗前后的情绪调节,但与面对面治疗相比,情绪调节恶化。
    所研究的模态和所使用的情绪测量缺乏差异,因此很难得出负责任的结论。未来的研究应该研究不同的方式如何影响情绪的实时交流,以及非语言线索如何影响这一点。
    UNASSIGNED: Computer-mediated care is becoming increasingly popular, but little research has been done on it and its effects on emotion-related outcomes. This systematic literature review aims to create an overview that addresses the research question: \"Is there a relationship between computer-mediated care and emotional expression, perception, and emotional and (long-term) emotion outcomes?\"
    UNASSIGNED: This systematic literature review was conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines and used five eligibility criteria, namely, (1) participants: adults seeking support; (2) intervention: eHealth; (3) diagnostic criteria: transdiagnostic concept of difficulty identifying, expressing, and/or regulating emotions (e.g., alexithymia); (4) comparator: either face-to-face care or no comparator; and (5) study design: quantitative studies or qualitative studies. Quality was assessed using the QualSyst tool.
    UNASSIGNED: The analysis includes 25 research papers. Self-paced interventions appear to have a positive effect on emotion regulation. Videoconferencing interventions improved emotion regulation from before to after treatment but worsened emotion regulation compared with face-to-face treatment.
    UNASSIGNED: The lack of variation in the modalities studied and the emotion measurements used make it difficult to draw responsible conclusions. Future research should examine how different modalities affect the real-time communication of emotions and how non-verbal cues influence this.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    不可思议的山谷描述了人造实体的审美吸引力与其人类相似之间的典型非线性关系。这种效果归因于专门的(配置的)处理,该处理增加了对人类规范偏差的敏感性。我们在计算机生成中研究这种影响,像人一样的android和使用动态面部表情的人脸。带有不同程度同步的愤怒和快乐的表情被直立和倒置地呈现,并被评为他们的怪异,陌生,和人类的相似之处。对于直立的表情和倒置的面孔,发现了人类相似度和不清晰度的S形函数(“不可思议的斜率”)。虽然这个函数并不代表一个不可思议的山谷,结果支持这样的观点,即构形处理缓和了人类相似对非直性的影响,并将其作用扩展到动态面部表情。
    The uncanny valley describes the typically nonlinear relation between the esthetic appeal of artificial entities and their human likeness. The effect has been attributed to specialized (configural) processing that increases sensitivity to deviations from human norms. We investigate this effect in computer-generated, humanlike android and human faces using dynamic facial expressions. Angry and happy expressions with varying degrees of synchrony were presented upright and inverted and rated on their eeriness, strangeness, and human likeness. A sigmoidal function of human likeness and uncanniness (\"uncanny slope\") was found for upright expressions and a linear relation for inverted faces. While the function is not indicative of an uncanny valley, the results support the view that configural processing moderates the effect of human likeness on uncanniness and extend its role to dynamic facial expressions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在某一点之后,排球运动员的可见非语言行为可能表明他们的情感状态。本研究的目的是调查该点结果是否(即,胜利vs.损失)和情况的重要性(即,第一vs.第五组)影响情感状态(通过非言语行为索引)的识别程度。为此,向111名参与者展示了60个视频摘录,这些视频摘录显示了排球队在集会后立即表现出的非语言行为,并询问所显示的球队是刚刚赢了还是输了一分。结果表明,获胜的识别率(80.09%)明显高于失分(65.56%),第五组的识别率(77.84%)明显高于第一组的识别率(68.51%)。此外,探索性分析表明,第五组获胜点的识别率(89.19%)明显高于第一组(72.13%)。
    The visible non-verbal behavior of volleyball players after a point might indicate their affective state. The purpose of the present study was to investigate whether the point outcome (i.e., win vs. loss) and the importance of the situation (i.e., first vs. fifth set) influence how well the affective states (indexed through non-verbal behavior) can be recognized. For this purpose, 111 participants were presented 60 video excerpts showing the non-verbal behavior of volleyball teams immediately after a rally and asked whether the shown team had just won or lost a point. The results indicated that the recognition rate was significantly higher for won (80.09%) than for lost points (65.56%) as well as for points of the fifth set (77.84%) than for points of the first set (68.51%). Furthermore, an explorative analysis showed that the recognition rate was significantly higher for won points of the fifth set (89.19%) than for those of the first set (72.13%).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    第三方惩罚(TPP)被认为对人类合作的发展和维持至关重要。然而,这种惩罚往往得不到回报,也许是因为惩罚者的潜在动机不清楚。我们认为,道德情感的表达可以通过宣传这种动机来解决这个问题。在三个实验中(n=1711),第三方惩罚游戏之后是信任游戏。第三方表示愤怒或厌恶,而不是,或者除此之外,经济惩罚。结果表明,表达这些情绪的第三方比不表达的第三方更受信任(实验1),比那些受到经济惩罚的人更多(实验2)。此外,在经济惩罚的同时表达的第三方比在没有表达的情况下受到惩罚的第三方更信任(实验3)。研究结果表明,情感表达可能通过促进TPP在合作的发展和维持中发挥作用。
    Third party punishment (TPP) is thought to be crucial to the evolution and maintenance of human cooperation. However, this type of punishment is often not rewarded, perhaps because punishers\' underlying motives are unclear. We propose that the expression of moral emotions could solve this problem by advertising such motives. In each of three experiments (n = 1711), a third-party punishment game was followed by a trust game. Third parties expressed anger or disgust instead of, or in addition to, financial punishment. Results showed that third parties who expressed these emotions were trusted more than those who didn\'t express (Experiment 1), and more than those who financially punished (Experiment 2). Moreover, third parties who expressed while financially punishing were trusted more than those who punished without expressing (Experiment 3). Findings suggest that emotion expression might play a role in the evolution and maintenance of cooperation by facilitating TPP.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号