visual search

视觉搜索
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人们在搜索方面有所不同。我们测试了两种认知能力的贡献:视觉工作记忆(VWM)能力和物体识别能力。参与者完成了三项任务:一项困难的低效视觉搜索任务,他们在偏斜的L干扰者中搜索目标字母T;VWM任务,他们记忆了一个颜色数组,然后确定被探测的颜色是否属于前一个数组;以及新颖的对象记忆测试(NOMT),在那里他们学习了复杂的新奇物体,然后在与它们非常相似的物体中识别出它们。探索性和验证性因素分析显示,有两个潜在因素可以解释这三个任务之间的共同差异:一个因素表明参与者在具有挑战性的视觉搜索任务中行使的谨慎程度,也是代表他们视觉认知能力的因素。谨慎搜索得分高的人倾向于执行更准确但较慢的搜索。在视觉认知能力因子上得分高的人往往具有较高的VWM能力,更好的物体识别能力,更快的搜索速度。结果反映了两点:(1)视觉搜索任务与视觉工作记忆和对象识别任务共享组件。(2)搜索性能不仅受搜索显示属性的影响,还受个人偏好(如谨慎和一般视觉能力)的影响。这项研究引入了解释视觉搜索行为变化时要考虑的新因素。
    People differ in how well they search. What are the factors that might contribute to this variability? We tested the contribution of two cognitive abilities: visual working memory (VWM) capacity and object recognition ability. Participants completed three tasks: a difficult inefficient visual search task, where they searched for a target letter T among skewed L distractors; a VWM task, where they memorized a color array and then identified whether a probed color belonged to the previous array; and the Novel Object Memory Test (NOMT), where they learnt complex novel objects and then identified them amongst objects that closely resembled them. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses revealed that there are two latent factors that explain the shared variance among these three tasks: a factor indicative of the level of caution participants exercised during the challenging visual search task, and a factor representing their visual cognitive abilities. People who score high on the search cautiousness tend to perform a more accurate but slower search. People who score high on the visual cognitive ability factor tend to have a higher VWM capacity, a better object recognition ability, and a faster search speed. The results reflect two points: (1) Visual search tasks share components with visual working memory and object recognition tasks. (2) Search performance is influenced not only by the search display\'s properties but also by individual predispositions such as caution and general visual abilities. This study introduces new factors for consideration when interpreting variations in visual search behaviors.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    上下文提示是一种现象,其中重复遇到的项目数组可以增强对目标项目的视觉搜索。这广泛归因于在视觉搜索过程中获得的上下文记忆所驱动的注意力指导。一些研究表明,与成年人相比,儿童使用语境线索的能力可能不成熟,而其他人则认为不同年龄的情境学习能力相似。为了测试情境引导注意力的发展,这项研究比较了三个年龄组的情境提示效果:成年人(18-33岁,N=32),青少年(15-17岁,N=41),和年幼的儿童(8-9岁,N=43)。此外,这项研究引入了响应时间可变性的测量,该测量跟踪整个实验过程中响应时间的波动,除了常规的响应时间分析。结果表明,在重复搜索环境中,所有年龄组的反应都明显快于非重复搜索环境。值得注意的是,成年人和青少年在重复环境中表现出比在非重复环境中更小的响应时间变异性,而年幼的孩子没有。这意味着儿童在将上下文信息整合到稳定的记忆表示中时效率较低,这可能会导致视觉搜索过程中注意力引导不稳定。
    Contextual cueing is a phenomenon in which repeatedly encountered arrays of items can enhance the visual search for a target item. This is widely attributed to attentional guidance driven by contextual memory acquired during visual search. Some studies suggest that children may have an immature ability to use contextual cues compared to adults, while others argue that contextual learning capacity is similar across ages. To test the development of context-guided attention, this study compared contextual cueing effects among three age groups: adults (aged 18-33 years, N = 32), teenagers (aged 15-17 years, N = 41), and younger children (aged 8-9 years, N = 43). Moreover, this study introduced a measure of response time variability that tracks fluctuations in response time throughout the experiment, in addition to the conventional analysis of response times. The results showed that all age groups demonstrated significantly faster responses in repeated than non-repeated search contexts. Notably, adults and teenagers exhibited smaller response time variability in repeated contexts than in non-repeated ones, while younger children did not. This implies that children are less efficient at consolidating contextual information into a stable memory representation, which may lead to less stable attentional guidance during visual search.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    放射科医师的任务是在视觉上仔细检查由3D体积成像模式产生的大量数据。在3D搜索过程中,小信号可能会被忽视,因为它们很难在视觉外围检测到。机器学习和计算机视觉的最新进展导致了有效的计算机辅助检测(CADe)支持系统,具有减轻感知错误的潜力。
    16名非专家观察者通过数字乳房断层合成(DBT)体模和DBT体模的单个横截面切片进行了搜索。3D/2D搜索在有和没有基于卷积神经网络(CNN)的CADe支持系统的情况下发生。该模型为观察者提供了叠加在图像刺激上的边界框,同时他们寻找小的微钙化信号和大的质量信号。记录眼睛注视位置,并与ROC曲线下面积(AUC)的变化相关。
    CNN-CADe改进了对小的微钙化信号的3D搜索(ΔAUC=0.098,p=0.0002)和2D搜索大质量信号(ΔAUC=0.076,p=0.002)。对于小信号,3D中的CNN-CADe益处明显大于2D中的(ΔΔAUC=0.066,p=0.035)。对个体差异的分析表明,那些探索眼球运动最少的人从CNN-CADe中受益最多(r=-0.528,p=0.036)。然而,对于大信号,2D效益并不显著大于3D效益(ΔΔAUC=0.033,p=0.133)。
    CNN-CADe为小信号的3D(相对于2D)搜索带来了独特的性能优势,它减少了体积数据不足造成的误差。
    UNASSIGNED: Radiologists are tasked with visually scrutinizing large amounts of data produced by 3D volumetric imaging modalities. Small signals can go unnoticed during the 3D search because they are hard to detect in the visual periphery. Recent advances in machine learning and computer vision have led to effective computer-aided detection (CADe) support systems with the potential to mitigate perceptual errors.
    UNASSIGNED: Sixteen nonexpert observers searched through digital breast tomosynthesis (DBT) phantoms and single cross-sectional slices of the DBT phantoms. The 3D/2D searches occurred with and without a convolutional neural network (CNN)-based CADe support system. The model provided observers with bounding boxes superimposed on the image stimuli while they looked for a small microcalcification signal and a large mass signal. Eye gaze positions were recorded and correlated with changes in the area under the ROC curve (AUC).
    UNASSIGNED: The CNN-CADe improved the 3D search for the small microcalcification signal ( Δ   AUC = 0.098 , p = 0.0002 ) and the 2D search for the large mass signal ( Δ   AUC = 0.076 , p = 0.002 ). The CNN-CADe benefit in 3D for the small signal was markedly greater than in 2D ( Δ Δ   AUC = 0.066 , p = 0.035 ). Analysis of individual differences suggests that those who explored the least with eye movements benefited the most from the CNN-CADe ( r = - 0.528 , p = 0.036 ). However, for the large signal, the 2D benefit was not significantly greater than the 3D benefit ( Δ Δ   AUC = 0.033 , p = 0.133 ).
    UNASSIGNED: The CNN-CADe brings unique performance benefits to the 3D (versus 2D) search of small signals by reducing errors caused by the underexploration of the volumetric data.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    传统的视觉搜索任务不能直接解决注意力,它们对“设置大小”(显示项目的数量)的核心操纵引入了阻碍解释的刺激混淆。然而,替代方法尚未被广泛采用,也许反映了它们的复杂性,假设,或间接关注抽样。这里,一个新的程序,“注意位置和大小”(“ATLAS”)任务使用探针显示来跟踪注意位置,广度,搜索过程中的指导。尽管大多数探测器显示包含六个项目,参与者只报告了他们认为自己感觉最清楚的一个项目-索引注意力\“峰值\”。通过跨变量\'选择集\'采样峰值,搜索过程中注意窗口的大小和位置进行了分析。这些指数似乎与广泛的关注区分开来,发出了对出现的项目的关注信号,并随着时间的推移跟踪了不断发展的关注指导。ATLAS旨在区分五种关键搜索模式:串行无引导,顺序引导,在当地指导下,无指导地关注“团块”,以及有或没有指导的广泛平行关注。最初的调查仅使用了一组高度规则的刺激,但其更广泛的潜力应该被调查。
    Conventional visual search tasks do not address attention directly and their core manipulation of \'set size\' - the number of displayed items - introduces stimulus confounds that hinder interpretation. However, alternative approaches have not been widely adopted, perhaps reflecting their complexity, assumptions, or indirect attention-sampling. Here, a new procedure, the ATtention Location And Size (\'ATLAS\') task used probe displays to track attention\'s location, breadth, and guidance during search. Though most probe displays comprised six items, participants reported only the single item they judged themselves to have perceived most clearly - indexing the attention \'peak\'. By sampling peaks across variable \'choice sets\', the size and position of the attention window during search was profiled. These indices appeared to distinguish narrow- from broad attention, signalled attention to pairs of items where it arose and tracked evolving attention-guidance over time. ATLAS is designed to discriminate five key search modes: serial-unguided, sequential-guided, unguided attention to \'clumps\' with local guidance, and broad parallel-attention with or without guidance. This initial investigation used only an example set of highly regular stimuli, but its broader potential should be investigated.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    类别搜索涉及根据来自长期记忆的类别信息来查找对象。先前的研究表明,分类搜索中的搜索效率受目标/干扰者相似性和类别可变性的影响(即,异质性)。然而,这些因素之间的相互作用及其对搜索的不同子过程的影响尚不清楚.这项研究考察了目标/干扰物相似性和类别可变性对分类搜索过程的影响。使用多维缩放,我们操纵了目标/干扰物相似性,并测量了参与者搜索的目标类别的类别变异性.收集眼动追踪数据以检查注意指导和目标验证。结果表明,类别变异性对响应时间(RTs)的影响取决于目标/干扰物相似性的水平。具体来说,当干扰物与目标类别高度相似时,RTs和变异性之间存在负相关关系,低变异性类别比高变异性类别产生更长的RT。令人惊讶的是,这种趋势仅存在于目标验证的眼动追踪措施中,而不存在于注意力引导中.我们的结果表明,与高变异性类别相比,搜索者更有效地将注意力引导到低变异性类别,无论目标和干扰物之间的相似程度如何。然而,当干扰因素与类别高度相似时,低类别可变性会干扰目标匹配决策,因此,低类别可变性提供给搜索者的优势在搜索过程中是不相等的。
    Categorical search involves looking for objects based on category information from long-term memory. Previous research has shown that search efficiency in categorical search is influenced by target/distractor similarity and category variability (i.e., heterogeneity). However, the interaction between these factors and their impact on different subprocesses of search remains unclear. This study examined the effects of target/distractor similarity and category variability on processes of categorical search. Using multidimensional scaling, we manipulated target/distractor similarity and measured category variability for target categories that participants searched for. Eye-tracking data were collected to examine attentional guidance and target verification. The results demonstrated that the effect of category variability on response times (RTs) was dependent on the level of target/distractor similarity. Specifically, when distractors were highly similar to target categories, there was a negative relation between RTs and variability, with low variability categories producing longer RTs than higher variability categories. Surprisingly, this trend was only present in the eye-tracking measures of target verification but not attentional guidance. Our results suggest that searchers more effectively guide attention to low-variability categories compared to high-variability categories, regardless of the degree of similarity between targets and distractors. However, low category variability interferes with target match decisions when distractors are highly similar to the category, thus the advantage that low category variability provides to searchers is not equal across processes of search.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究的目的是系统地比较和评估专家和非专家体育官员之间在感知-认知技能方面的差异,并进一步探讨不同类型体育官员造成的潜在差异,以便更全面地了解体育官员的感知认知能力。
    在四个英文数据库中搜索了2022年12月31日之前发布的相关文献。使用ReviewManager5.4和Stata12.0软件进行荟萃分析和偏倚检验。
    专家体育官员的决策要比非专家体育官员准确得多,并表现出较大的效应量大小(SMD=1.09;95CI:0.52,1.66;P<0.05)。专家体育官员的注视次数明显少于非专家体育官员,并且效果大小适中(SMD=0.71;95CI:1.25,0.17;P<0.05)。专家体育官员的固定时间(SMD=0.23;95CI:0.25,0.71;P=0.35)与非专家体育官员没有显着差异。
    可以看出,专家和非专家体育官员的感知-认知技能存在差异。决策准确性可以作为区分专家和非专家体育官员的感知认知技能的重要指标。注视次数可以作为区分监护仪感知认知技能的重要指标。
    https://www.crd.约克。AC.uk/PROSPERO/display_record。php?RecordID=418594,标识符:CRD42023418594。
    UNASSIGNED: The purpose of this study is to systematically compare and assess the differences in perceptual-cognitive skills between expert and non-expert sports officials, and further explore the potential differences caused by different types of sports officials, in order to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the perceptual-cognitive skills of sports officials.
    UNASSIGNED: Relevant literature published before 31 December 2022 was searched in four English databases. Review Manager 5.4 and Stata 12.0 software were used for meta-analysis and bias test.
    UNASSIGNED: Expert sports officials are significantly more accurate in their decision-making than non-expert sports officials, and exhibit a large amount of effect size (SMD = 1.09; 95%CI: 0.52, 1.66; P < 0.05). Expert sports officials had significantly fewer number of fixations than non-expert sports officials and showed a moderate amount of effect size (SMD = 0.71; 95%CI: 1.25, 0.17; P < 0.05). Expert sports officials\' duration of fixation (SMD = 0.23; 95%CI: 0.25, 0.71; P = 0.35) were not significantly different from non-expert sports officials.
    UNASSIGNED: It can be seen that there are differences in the Perceptual-cognitive skills of expert and non-expert sports officials. Decision-making accuracy can serve as an important indicator for distinguishing the perceptual-cognitive skills of expert and non-expert sports officials. Number of fixations can serve as important indicators to differentiate the perceptual-cognitive skills of monitors.
    UNASSIGNED: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/display_record.php?RecordID=418594, identifier: CRD42023418594.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    视觉搜索(VS)包括一类任务,我们通常在一天中执行几次,并且需要在干扰因素刺激中有意扫描(有或没有移动眼睛)特定目标(无论是对象还是特征)的环境。在基于实验室或现实世界的环境中进行的实验研究从一个名义的角度提供了对其潜在神经认知机制的见解。鲜为人知但快速增长的准实验和相关研究机构探索了个体差异与VS表现之间的联系。这结合了不同的研究传统,涵盖了部署大量VS任务的研究中广泛的个体差异。因此,在考虑更广泛的文献时,确定单项研究中强调的任何关联是否稳健是一个挑战.然而,系统和全面地阐明这种关系将有助于建立更准确的VS模型,它将突出未来研究的有希望的方向。本系统综述提供了对现有文献的最新和全面综合,这些文献调查了VS任务中常见的性能指标与映射到四类认知能力(短期工作记忆,流体推理,视觉处理和处理速度)和七类性状(五大性状,特质焦虑和自闭症特征)。这两个特征的一致关联(特别是,尽责,自闭症特征和特质焦虑-后者仅限于情绪刺激)和认知能力(尤其是视觉处理)被确定。总的来说,然而,未来研究的信息将受益于检查和报告所有测量工具的可靠性,应用多重性校正,使用互补技术,研究预注册和测试为什么,而不仅仅是如果,某些个体差异与VS表现之间存在稳健的关系。
    Visual search (VS) comprises a class of tasks that we typically perform several times during a day and requires intentionally scanning (with or without moving the eyes) the environment for a specific target (be it an object or a feature) among distractor stimuli. Experimental research in lab-based or real-world settings has offered insight into its underlying neurocognitive mechanisms from a nomothetic point of view. A lesser-known but rapidly growing body of quasi-experimental and correlational research has explored the link between individual differences and VS performance. This combines different research traditions and covers a wide range of individual differences in studies deploying a vast array of VS tasks. As such, it is a challenge to determine whether any associations highlighted in single studies are robust when considering the wider literature. However, clarifying such relationships systematically and comprehensively would help build more accurate models of VS, and it would highlight promising directions for future research. This systematic review provides an up to date and comprehensive synthesis of the existing literature investigating associations between common indices of performance in VS tasks and measures of individual differences mapped onto four categories of cognitive abilities (short-term working memory, fluid reasoning, visual processing and processing speed) and seven categories of traits (Big Five traits, trait anxiety and autistic traits). Consistent associations for both traits (in particular, conscientiousness, autistic traits and trait anxiety - the latter limited to emotional stimuli) and cognitive abilities (particularly visual processing) were identified. Overall, however, informativeness of future studies would benefit from checking and reporting the reliability of all measurement tools, applying multiplicity correction, using complementary techniques, study preregistration and testing why, rather than only if, a robust relation between certain individual differences and VS performance exists.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这篇文章,从结构特征的角度来看,重点介绍车载用户界面图标,探讨不同图标结构特征对视觉搜索效率的影响。最初,我们根据结构特征将图标分为四组:个体结构图标(ISI),封闭结构图标(ESI),水平结构图标(HSI)和垂直结构图标(VSI)。随后,我们以结构为唯一变量进行了视觉搜索实验,记录参与者的行为和眼动追踪数据。最后,数据分析采用方差分析和逻辑回归分析.结果表明,图标结构特征的差异显著影响视觉搜索效率,显示显著的组间差异。恒生指数表现出最高的视觉搜索效率,而ESI显示效率最低。ISI的响应时间较短,但匹配精度最低。VSI的性能仅优于ESI。这些发现对优化图标设计和提高视觉搜索效率具有重要意义。
    图标的视觉搜索效率对于人机交互至关重要。我们研究了图标的结构特征如何影响视觉搜索效率。水平图标是最有效的,封闭的图标最少。单个图标是快速的,但不太准确。垂直图标优于封闭的图标。在设计中应考虑结构特征。
    This article, from the perspective of structural features, focuses on in-car user interface icons and explores the impact of different icon structural features on visual search efficiency. Initially, we categorised the icons into four groups based on structural features: individual structure icons (ISI), enclosed structure icons (ESI), horizontal structure icons (HSI) and vertical structure icons (VSI). Subsequently, we conducted a visual search experiment with structure as the sole variable, recording participants\' behaviours and eye-tracking data. Finally, data analysis was conducted using methods including analysis of variance and logistic regression. The results indicate that differences in icon structural features significantly affect visual search efficiency, showcasing significant intergroup differences. HSI exhibit the highest visual search efficiency, while ESI show the lowest efficiency. ISI have shorter response times but the lowest matching accuracy. VSI only perform better than ESI. These findings hold significant implications for optimising icon design and enhancing visual search efficiency.
    Visual search efficiency of icons is crucial for human-computer interaction. We investigated how the structural features of icons influence visual search efficiency. Horizontal icons are most effective, enclosed icons the least. Individual icons are quick but less accurate. Vertical icons outperform enclosed ones. Structural features should be considered in design.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    感知生物运动(BM)对于人类生存和社会交往至关重要。许多研究报道了自闭症谱系障碍的BM感知受损,其特点是社会互动不足。患有注意力缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)的儿童通常在社交互动中表现出类似的困难。然而,很少有研究调查患有ADHD的儿童的BM感知。这里,我们比较了处理局部运动学和全局构型线索的能力差异,BM感知的两个基本能力,在典型的发展和多动症儿童之间。我们进一步调查了使用社会反应量表测量的BM感知和社交互动技能之间的关系,并检查了潜在因素的贡献(例如性别,年龄,注意,和智力)到BM感知。结果显示,患有ADHD的儿童表现出非典型的BM感知。局部和全局BM处理显示出明显的特征。本地BM处理能力与社交互动技能有关,而全球BM加工能力随着年龄的增长而显著提高。严重的,ADHD儿童的一般BM感知(即局部和全局BM处理)可能受到持续注意能力的影响。这种关系主要是由推理智力介导的。这些发现阐明了ADHD中非典型的BM感知以及与BM感知相关的潜在因素。此外,这项研究提供了新的证据,表明BM感知是社会认知的标志,并促进了我们对局部和全局处理在BM感知和社会认知障碍中的潜在作用的理解.
    Perceiving biological motion (BM) is crucial for human survival and social interaction. Many studies have reported impaired BM perception in autism spectrum disorder, which is characterised by deficits in social interaction. Children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) often exhibit similar difficulties in social interaction. However, few studies have investigated BM perception in children with ADHD. Here, we compared differences in the ability to process local kinematic and global configurational cues, two fundamental abilities of BM perception, between typically developing and ADHD children. We further investigated the relationship between BM perception and social interaction skills measured using the Social Responsiveness Scale and examined the contributions of latent factors (e.g. sex, age, attention, and intelligence) to BM perception. The results revealed that children with ADHD exhibited atypical BM perception. Local and global BM processing showed distinct features. Local BM processing ability was related to social interaction skills, whereas global BM processing ability significantly improved with age. Critically, general BM perception (i.e. both local and global BM processing) may be affected by sustained attentional ability in children with ADHD. This relationship was primarily mediated by reasoning intelligence. These findings elucidate atypical BM perception in ADHD and the latent factors related to BM perception. Moreover, this study provides new evidence that BM perception is a hallmark of social cognition and advances our understanding of the potential roles of local and global processing in BM perception and social cognitive disorders.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    微跳是基本的动眼现象和复杂的认知功能过程之间的接口,对于微妙的实验和充分的统计分析,它们也是一个挑战。在特殊主题问题的第二部分(第一部分见4),作者提出了一系列文章,这些文章表明,微跳仍然是科学研究的一个有趣和有益的领域,在许多领域的感官研究的前沿,感性的,和认知过程。.在他们的文章“复杂决策过程中对认知努力和情绪唤醒的瞳孔和微跳反应”中,44%urawska,Duchowski,&Wichary(1)研究了情感启动下多属性决策过程中瞳孔和微跳对信息处理的反应。参与者被随机分配到三个情感启动条件(中性,令人厌恶的,和色情),并指示做出歧视性决定。正如作者所假设的那样,结果表明,微小扫视速率抑制和瞳孔扩张,取决于决策前的认知努力,并通过情感启动来调节。逆向启动增加了瞳孔和微跳对信息处理工作的反应。结果表明,瞳孔反应受情感启动的影响比微扫视率更大。根据瞳孔和微视行为的神经心理学机制讨论了结果。在文章“在单眼和双眼刺激条件下的微跳速率特征相关”中,Leube,Rifai,&Wahl(2020)研究了微跳在单眼和双眼条件下的定向分布和速率。在两种刺激条件下,参与者都固定了一个Gabor贴片,该贴片在很宽的空间频率范围内以45°或135°的方向随机呈现。微扫视大多是水平取向的,而与光栅的空间频率无关。该结果在两种刺激条件之间是一致的。这项研究发现,微跳跳速率特征曲线在两种刺激条件之间相关,因此,将微跳的使用扩展到临床应用,由于参数为对比敏感度,在临床研究中经常进行单眼测量。马丁的这项研究“高速连续视觉搜索过程中的微跳”,戴维斯,Riesenhuber,&Thorpe(3)提供了对视觉搜索过程中发生的微跳的分析,针对小脸粘贴到杂乱的背景照片或简单的灰色背景。指示参与者在变化的场景中瞄准奇异的3度直立或倒置的面孔。一旦参与者的目光到达目标面部,在不同的随机位置显示了一张新的面孔。无论实验背景如何(例如背景场景,没有背景场景),或目标偏心(从4到20度的视角),作者发现,微视速率在12毫秒内降至接近零的水平。在刺激发作后和第一次扫视之前,几乎没有任何微跳。在大约20%的试验中,有一个单一的微扫视,几乎立即发生在前一个扫视的偏移。作者认为,需要通过处理刺激的视觉层次结构进行一次前馈才能实现长时间的连续视觉搜索,并提供证据证明微扫视可以发挥感知功能,例如在连续视觉搜索过程中纠正扫视或实现面向任务的目标。虽然许多研究已经描述了视觉注视过程中的眼球运动,包括微跳,在大多数情况下,只有水平和垂直分量被记录和分析。关于微跳的扭转成分知之甚少。在这项研究中,“在固定和视动刺激期间快速阶段的微跳的扭转成分”Sadeghpour&Otero-Millan(5)记录了在固定和扭转视动刺激期间围绕三个旋转轴的眼球运动。作者发现,固定过程中微扫视的扭转分量的平均幅度为0.34±0.07度,速度遵循主序列,其斜率与水平和垂直分量相当。微扫视过程中扭转位移的大小与水平分量相关,而与垂直分量无关。与固定刺激固定过程中产生的微跳相比,在存在视动刺激的情况下,诱导眼球震颤产生更频繁和更大的扭转快速阶段。快速相的扭转分量和垂直聚散分量随速度的增加而增加。在以前的研究中,微跳被解释为任务负荷的心理生理指标。到目前为止,不同类型的任务需求如何影响微扫视率仍在争论中。Schneider等人在他们的文章“任务难度与微视速率之间的相互作用:视觉负荷的关键作用的证据”中。(6)检查了视觉负荷之间的关系,心理负荷和微视速率。参与者执行了一个连续的绩效任务(n-back),其中视觉任务负载(字母与抽象数字)和心理任务负荷(1-back到4-back)作为受试者内部变量进行操纵。眼动追踪数据,记录了绩效数据和主观工作量。数据分析显示,对于高视觉需求的刺激(即抽象数字),微扫视速率增加,而精神需求(n-back-level)并不调节微视速率。作者得出结论,微小扫视率反映了任务的视觉负荷,而不是其心理负荷。这一结论符合克鲁格等人的命题。(2)“微跳区分看和看”,将感官与认知现象联系起来。当前的特殊主题为围绕微跳的研究景观增加了几个新的有趣方面。它们仍然是跨学科研究和跨学科应用的有吸引力的焦点。因此,正如在本期特别专题的第一部分中已经指出的那样,对微跳的研究不仅会持续下去,但随着知识库的扩展而不断发展。
    Microsaccades are at the interface between basic oculomotor phenomena and complex processes of cognitive functioning, and they also have been a challenge for subtle experimentation and adequate statistical analysis. In the second part of the special thematic issue (for the first part see  4) the authors present a series of articles which demonstrate that microsaccades are still an interesting and rewarding area of scientific research the forefront of research in many areas of sensory, perceptual, and cognitive processes.. In their article \"Pupillary and microsaccadic responses to cognitive effort and emotional arousal during complex decision making\" Krejtz, Żurawska, Duchowski, & Wichary (1) investigate pupillary and microsaccadic responses to information processing during multi-attribute decision making under affective priming. The participants were randomly assigned into three affective priming conditions (neutral, aversive, and erotic) and instructed to make discriminative decisions. As hypothesized by the authors, the results showed microsaccadic rate inhibition and pupillary dilation, depending on cognitive effort prior to decision and moderated by affective priming. Aversive priming increased pupillary and microsaccadic responses to information processing effort. The results indicate that pupillary response is more influenced by affective priming than microsaccadic rate. The results are discussed in the light of neuropsychological mechanisms of pupillary and microsaccadic behavior. In the article \"Microsaccadic rate signatures correlate under monocular and binocular stimulation conditions\" Essig, Leube, Rifai, & Wahl (2020) investigate microsaccades with respect to their directional distribution and rate under monocular and binocular conditions. In both stimulation conditions participants fixated a Gabor patch presented randomly in orientation of 45° or 135° over a wide range of spatial frequencies. Microsaccades were mostly horizontally oriented regardless of the spatial frequency of the grating. This outcome was consistent between both stimulation conditions. This study found that the microsaccadic rate signature curve correlates between both stimulation conditions, therefore extending the use of microsaccades to clinical applications, since parameters as contrast sensitivity, have frequently been measured monocularly in the clinical studies. The study \"Microsaccades during high speed continuous visual search\" by Martin, Davis, Riesenhuber, & Thorpe (3) provides an analysis of the microsaccades occurring during visual search, targeting to small faces pasted either into cluttered background photos or into a simple gray background.  Participants were instructed to target singular 3-degree upright or inverted faces in changing scenes.  As soon as the participant\'s gaze reached the target face, a new face was displayed in a different random location.  Regardless of the experimental context (e.g. background scene, no background scene), or target eccentricity (from 4 to 20 degrees of visual angle), The authors found that the microsaccade rate dropped to near zero levels within 12 milliseconds.  There were almost never any microsaccades after stimulus onset and before the first saccade to the face. In about 20% of the trials, there was a single microsaccade that occurred almost immediately after the preceding saccade\'s offset.  The authors argue that a single feedforward pass through the visual hierarchy of processing a stimulus is needed to effectuate prolonged continuous visual search and provide evidence that microsaccades can serve perceptual functions like correcting saccades or effectuating task-oriented goals during continuous visual search. While many studies have characterized the eye movements during visual fixation, including microsaccades, in most cases only horizontal and vertical components have been recorded and analyzed. Little is known about the torsional component of microsaccades. In the study \"Torsional component of microsaccades during fixation and quick phases during optokinetic stimulation\" Sadeghpour & Otero-Millan (5) recorded eye movements around the three axes of rotation during fixation and torsional optokinetic stimulus. The authors found that the average amplitude of the torsional component of microsaccades during fixation was 0.34 ± 0.07 degrees with velocities following a main sequence with a slope comparable to the horizontal and vertical components. The size of the torsional displacement during microsaccades was correlated with the horizontal but not the vertical component. In the presence of an optokinetic stimulus a nystagmus was induced producing  more frequent and larger torsional quick phases compared to microsaccades produced during fixation of a stationary stimulus. The torsional component and the vertical vergence component of quick phases increased with higher velocities. In previous research, microsaccades have been interpreted as psychophysiological indicators of task load. So far, it is still under debate how different types of task demands are influencing microsaccade rate. In their article \"The interplay between task difficulty and microsaccade rate: Evidence for the critical role of visual load\" Schneider et al. (6) examined the relation between visual load, mental load and microsaccade rate. The participants carried out a continuous performance task (n-back) in which visual task load (letters vs. abstract figures) and mental task load (1-back to 4-back) were manipulated as within-subjects variables. Eye tracking data, performance data as well as subjective workload were recorded. Data analysis revealed an increased level of microsaccade rate for stimuli of high visual demand (i.e. abstract figures), while mental demand (n-back-level) did not modulate microsaccade rate. The authors concluded that microsaccade rate reflects visual load of a task rather than its mental load. This conclusion is in accordance with the proposition of Krueger et al. (2) \"Microsaccades distinguish looking from seeing\", linking sensory with cognitive phenomena. The present special thematic issue adds several new interesting facets to the research landscape around microsaccades. They still remain an attractive focus of interdisciplinary research and transdisciplinary applications. Thus, as already noted in the first part of this special thematic issue, research on microsaccades will not only endure, but keep evolving as the knowledge base expands.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号