visual search

视觉搜索
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:研究使用全大写的单词识别效果(例如,瓦伦西亚)或标题案例(例如,瓦伦西亚)交通标志中的城市名称,控制单词大小,并比较固定和动态观看情况。
    背景:先前的研究提供了关于单词大小写对交通标志中城市名称识别的影响的混合证据。此外,关于视觉运动对这些影响的潜在影响的证据很少。
    方法:我们使用模拟交通标志进行了实验研究。任务是指出,对于每个标志,是否包含给定的城市名称(单词搜索任务,50%阳性试验)。标志的视觉运动被操纵为参与者之间的因素:静止(标志是静止的)与动态的(标志扩展,好像参与者正在接近它)。单词大小写是作为参与者内部因素进行操作的:全大写与两个标题大小写条件的字体大小不同:宽度匹配的标题大小写和点大小匹配的标题大小写。
    结果:在静态和动态条件下,与宽度匹配的标题大小写相比,全大写导致更多的错误响应和更慢的延迟。与点大小匹配的标题大小写相比,全大写在静止状态下产生较慢的正确响应,而在动态条件下更快。
    结论:其他因素相同,在交通标志中,全大写的城市名称将被识别为比其标题形式更糟糕的名称,在静止和动态情况下。
    结论:当前实验研究的结果将对交通标志的设计和其他文本呈现在运动中的情况感兴趣。
    OBJECTIVE: To investigate the word recognition effects of the use of all-uppercase (e.g., VALENCIA) or titled-case (e.g., Valencia) for city names in traffic signs, controlling for word size, and comparing stationary and dynamic viewing situations.
    BACKGROUND: Prior studies provide mixed evidence regarding the effects of word case on the recognition of city names in traffic signs. Moreover, the evidence on the potential impact of visual motion on these effects is scarce.
    METHODS: We carried out an experimental study using simulated traffic signs. The task was to indicate, for each sign, whether it contained a given city name or not (word search task, 50% positive trials). Visual motion of signs was manipulated as a between-participants factor: stationary (the sign was still) versus dynamic (the sign expanded as if the participant was approaching to it). Word case was manipulated as a within-participants factor: all-uppercase versus two titled-case conditions varying in font size: width-matched titled-case and point size-matched titled-case.
    RESULTS: In both the stationary and dynamic conditions, all-uppercase resulted in more incorrect responses and slower latencies than width-matched titled-case. When compared to point size-matched titled-case, all-uppercase produced slower correct responses in the stationary condition, whereas faster in the dynamic condition.
    CONCLUSIONS: Other factors being equal, all-uppercase city names will be recognized worse than their titled-case versions in traffic signs, both in stationary and dynamic situations.
    CONCLUSIONS: Results in the current experimental study would be of interest in the design of traffic signs and other circumstances in which text is presented in motion.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    UNASSIGNED:结构化模板放射学报告的一个可能的限制是放射科医师在查看和听写监视器之间来回查看,从而影响查看图像和生成报告所花费的时间长度。我们假设,与自由文本格式相比,使用结构化模板生成报告时,查看案例图像所花费的总时间减少和/或创建报告所花费的总时间延长。
    UNASSIGNED:三名神经放射科医生和三名高级居民观察了五例脑部磁共振成像病例,这些病例具有独特的发现,同时记录了眼位。参与者使用结构化模板和自由文本听写为每个案例生成报告。将观看图像所花费的时间与观看听写屏幕所花费的时间进行比较。
    UNASSIGNED:确认了两个主要假设:使用模板与自由文本听写相比,观看图像的总时间减少,使用模板创建报告的总时间延长。作为报告类型的函数,在“图像”感兴趣区域上花费的平均时间(s)接近统计显著性[免费:出席=236.79(154.43),居民=223.55(77.79);模板:出席人数=163.40(73.42),居民=182.48(77.47)],并且在出席者和居民的模板报告中总体较低(F=3.77,p=0.0623),但它作为资历的函数没有差异(F=0.017,p=0.8977)。
    UNASSIGNED:基于模板的放射学报告在改变放射科医师查看和报告图像的方式方面具有巨大的潜力,与自由文本听写相比,花更多的时间查看报告监视器而不是诊断图像。许多放射科医生更喜欢模板报告,因为结构化格式可能有助于进行更系统或彻底的搜索结果。尽管先前关于这一假设的工作好坏参半。未来的眼动追踪研究可以进一步阐明模板和免费报告是否以及如何影响射线照相结果的检测和分类。
    UNASSIGNED: One possible limitation of structured template radiology reports is that radiologists look back and forth between viewing and dictation monitors, thereby impacting the length of time spent reviewing images and generating a report. We hypothesize that the total time spent viewing case images is diminished and/or the total time spent creating a report is prolonged when the report is generated using a structured template compared with free text format.
    UNASSIGNED: Three neuroradiologists and three senior residents viewed five brain magnetic resonance imaging cases with unique findings while eye position was recorded. Participants generated reports for each case utilizing both structured templates and free text dictation. The time spent viewing images was compared with the time spent looking at the dictation screen.
    UNASSIGNED: The two main hypotheses were confirmed: the total time viewing images diminished with templates versus free text dictation and the total time to create a report was prolonged with templates. The mean time (s) spent on the \"image\" region of interest approached statistical significance as a function of the report type [free: attendings = 236.79 (154.43), residents = 223.55 (77.79); template: attendings = 163.40 (73.42), residents = 182.48 (77.47)] and was overall lower with the template reporting for both attendings and residents ( F = 3.77 , p = 0.0623 ), but it did not differ as a function of seniority ( F = 0.017 , p = 0.8977 ).
    UNASSIGNED: Template-based radiology reports have significant potential to alter the way radiologists view images and report on them, spending more time viewing the report monitor rather than diagnostic images compared with free text dictation. Many radiologists prefer templates for reporting as the structured format may aid in conducting a more systematic or thorough search for findings, although prior work on this assumption is mixed. Future eye-tracking studies could further elucidate whether and how templates and free reports impact the detection and classification of radiographic findings.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    The standard visual search task is integral to the study of selective attention and in search tasks target present slopes are the primary index of attentional demand. However, there are times when similarities in slopes may obscure important differences between conditions. To demonstrate this point, we used the case of line-ending illusory contours, building on a study by Li, Cave, and Wolfe (2008) where orientation-based search for figures defined by line-ending illusory contours was compared to that for the corresponding real-contour controls. Consistent with Li et al. (2008), we found search to be efficient for both illusory contour figures and the corresponding real-contour controls, with no significant differences between them. However, major differences between illusory contours and the real-contour controls emerged in selective enumeration, a task where participants enumerated targets in a display of distractors, with the number of targets and distractors manipulated. When looking at the distractor slopes, the increase in RT to enumerate a single target as a function of the number of distractors (a direct analogue to target present trials, with identical displays), we found distractor costs for illusory contour figures to be over 100 ms/distractor higher than for the corresponding real-contour controls. Furthermore, the discrepancies in RT slope between 1-3 and 6-8 targets associated with subitizing were only seen in the real-contour controls. These results show that similarities in RT slopes in search may mask important differences between conditions that emerge in other tasks.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Visual search tasks have often been used to investigate how cognitive processes change with expertise. Several studies have shown visual experts\' advantages in detecting objects related to their expertise. Here, we tried to extend these findings by investigating whether professional search experience could boost top-down monitoring processes involved in visual search, independently of advantages specific to objects of expertise. To this aim, we recruited a group of quality-control workers employed in citrus farms. Given the specific features of this type of job, we expected that the extensive employment of monitoring mechanisms during orange selection could enhance these mechanisms even in search situations in which orange-related expertise is not suitable. To test this hypothesis, we compared performance of our experimental group and of a well-matched control group on a computerized visual search task. In one block the target was an orange (expertise target) while in the other block the target was a Smurfette doll (neutral target). The a priori hypothesis was to find an advantage for quality-controllers in those situations in which monitoring was especially involved, that is, when deciding the presence/absence of the target required a more extensive inspection of the search array. Results were consistent with our hypothesis. Quality-controllers were faster in those conditions that extensively required monitoring processes, specifically, the Smurfette-present and both target-absent conditions. No differences emerged in the orange-present condition, which resulted to mainly rely on bottom-up processes. These results suggest that top-down processes in visual search can be enhanced through immersive real-life experience beyond visual expertise advantages.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Individuals with Parkinson\'s disease (PD) commonly demonstrate lateralized spatial biases, which affect daily functioning. Those with PD with initial motor symptoms on the left body side (LPD) have reduced leftward attention, whereas PD with initial motor symptoms on the right side (RPD) may display reduced rightward attention. We investigated whether a sustained attention training program could help reduce these spatial biases. Four non-demented individuals with PD (2 LPD, 2 RPD) performed a visual search task before and after 1 month of computer training. Before training, all participants showed a significant spatial bias and after training, all participants\' spatial bias was eliminated.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

公众号