Vision, Monocular

愿景,单眼
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    单眼(单眼)人类是否与具有完整双眼的人类使用单眼深度线索来执行与模仿日常生活复杂活动的深度相关的视觉运动任务?如果是这样,表现是否取决于参与者的年龄,单眼和头部运动的持续时间?
    45例单眼病例(年龄范围6-37岁;2.4个月-31.0年的单眼)和46例年龄相似的双目控制执行了一项任务,要求他们多次通过环绕深度弯曲的电线的环,同时避免听觉反馈所指示的接触。在有和没有头枕的情况下执行任务,以随机顺序。错误率和速度是根据环和线之间的接触频率和总任务持续时间(调整错误时间)计算的,分别,全部从任务的视频记录中确定。使用面部跟踪软件从视频中分析头部运动。
    错误率随着年龄的增长而下降(P<0.001),直到青少年后期,而速度则没有这种趋势。在所有年龄段,在没有双眼的情况下,错误率增加,速度降低(P<0.001)。随着单眼持续时间的增加,误差没有减少(P=0.16)。头部运动对任务表现没有优势,尽管产生视差差异可与双目观看相媲美。
    动态性能,在没有双目观察的情况下,深度相关的视觉运动任务会减少,与年龄相关的绩效水平无关。这项研究没有发现任何证据表明,单眼深度线索的长期经验对于此类任务比双眼性的短暂丧失更有利。
    UNASSIGNED: Do one-eyed (uniocular) humans use monocular depth cues differently from those with intact binocularity to perform depth-related visuomotor tasks that emulate complex activities of daily living? If so, does performance depend on the participant\'s age, duration of uniocularity and head movements?
    UNASSIGNED: Forty-five uniocular cases (age range 6-37 years; 2.4 months-31.0 years of uniocularity) and 46 age-similar binocular controls performed a task that required them to pass a hoop around an electrified wire convoluted in depth multiple times, while avoiding contact as indicated by auditory feedback. The task was performed with and without head restraint, in random order. The error rate and speed were calculated from the frequency of contact between the hoop and wire and the total task duration (adjusting for error time), respectively, all determined from video recordings of the task. Head movements were analyzed from the videos using face-tracking software.
    UNASSIGNED: Error rate decreased with age (P < 0.001) until the late teen years while speed revealed no such trend. Across all ages, the error rate increased and speed decreased in the absence of binocularity (P < 0.001). There was no additional error reduction with duration of uniocularity (P = 0.16). Head movements provided no advantage to task performance, despite generating parallax disparities comparable to binocular viewing.
    UNASSIGNED: Performance in a dynamic, depth-related visuomotor task is reduced in the absence of binocular viewing, independent of age-related performance level. This study finds no evidence for a prolonged experience with monocular depth cues being advantageous for such tasks over transient loss of binocularity.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在高水平的双目模糊下,运动表现相对稳健。然而,调查单眼损伤的有限研究表明,单眼损伤对运动表现有较大影响。这项研究与视力障碍运动员的运动分类有关(VI),在分类过程中使用来自较好眼睛的视敏度(VA)。在两个实验中,我们旨在通过模拟现役足球运动员中不同严重程度的VA退化和对比敏感度(CS),确定双眼和单眼损伤影响足球点球(PK)表现的点。在实验一,25名足球运动员进行了PKs,因为两只眼睛的VA和CS都系统地减少了,在一种情况下,视野(VF)降低。最严重的VA/CS状况和VF降低显著影响结局,球速度和位置(球踢得更靠近球门中心)(p<0.05)。在实验二,29名不同的足球运动员进行了PKs,因为只有优势眼的VA和CS被系统地减少,并且在一种情况下优势眼被遮挡,参与者通过非优势眼(单眼观看)观看他们的环境。在评估单眼损伤对结果的影响时,没有观察到差异。速度和球的放置。PK对VI有很高的弹性,但是双眼损伤有更直接的影响,建议在足球分类过程中应使用双目测量。
    Sports performance is relatively robust under high levels of binocular blur. However, the limited research studies investigating monocular impairments has shown it has a larger impact on sport performance. This research study is relevant for classification in sports for athletes with vision impairment (VI), where visual acuity (VA) from the better eye is used during classification. Across two experiments, we aimed to establish the point at which binocular and monocular impairments affected performance in a football penalty kick (PK) through simulating varying severities of degraded VA and contrast sensitivity (CS) in active football players. In experiment one, 25 footballers performed PKs as VA and CS were systematically decreased in both eyes, and in one condition, visual field (VF) was reduced. The most severe VA/CS condition and reduced VF significantly impacted outcome, ball velocity and placement (ball kicked closer to the centre of the goal) (p < 0.05). In experiment two, 29 different footballers performed PKs as VA and CS of only the dominant eye were systematically decreased and in one condition the dominant eye was occluded, and participants viewed their environment through the non-dominant eye (monocular viewing). No differences were observed when assessing monocular impairments influence on outcome, velocity and ball placement. PKs have a high resilience to VI, but binocular impairment has a more immediate effect, suggesting binocular measures should be used in classification processes in football.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在正常视力的成年人中,短期单眼剥夺会导致眼优势的短暂转变,提升被剥夺的眼睛。这种效果已经通过知觉测试和生理记录得到了证明,但是以前的研究没有同时测量生理反应和剥夺的知觉效应。在这里,我们提出了一种将双眼竞争与瞳孔测量相结合的综合实验范式,为了介绍眼优势可塑性的客观生理指标,与感知测试同时获得。十名参与者报告了双眼竞争的感知动力学,我们测量瞳孔直径.刺激是一个白色和黑色的磁盘,每个单眼呈现。比较单眼剥夺前后2小时的竞争动力学和瞳孔大小痕迹,通过在优势眼上应用半透明贴片来实现。与先前的研究一致,我们观察到单眼剥夺增强了剥夺眼信号,因此增加了眼优势。根据以前的研究,我们还观察到瞳孔大小的微妙但系统的调制,跟踪黑色或白色圆盘的专有优势阶段之间的交替。单眼剥夺后,这些瞳孔大小调制的幅度增加,这与剥夺后眼睛的增强和眼优势的增加是一致的。这提供了证据表明剥夺会影响单眼视觉刺激的有效强度,连贯地影响感知报告和瞳孔直径的自动和无意识调节。我们的结果表明,双眼竞争和瞳孔测量的结合范例为剥夺效应的生理机制提供了新的见解。
    Short-term monocular deprivation in normally sighted adult humans produces a transient shift of ocular dominance, boosting the deprived eye. This effect has been documented with both perceptual tests and through physiological recordings, but no previous study simultaneously measured physiological responses and the perceptual effects of deprivation. Here we propose an integrated experimental paradigm that combines binocular rivalry with pupillometry, to introduce an objective physiological index of ocular dominance plasticity, acquired concurrently with perceptual testing. Ten participants reported the perceptual dynamics of binocular rivalry, while we measured pupil diameter. Stimuli were a white and a black disk, each presented monocularly. Rivalry dynamics and pupil-size traces were compared before and after 2 h of monocular deprivation, achieved by applying a translucent patch over the dominant eye. Consistent with prior research, we observed that monocular deprivation boosts the deprived-eye signal and consequently increases ocular dominance. In line with previous studies, we also observed subtle but systematic modulations of pupil size that tracked alternations between exclusive dominance phases of the black or white disk. Following monocular deprivation, the amplitude of these pupil-size modulations increased, which is consistent with the post-deprivation boost of the deprived eye and the increase of ocular dominance. This provides evidence that deprivation impacts the effective strength of monocular visual stimuli, coherently affecting perceptual reports and the automatic and unconscious regulation of pupil diameter. Our results show that a combined paradigm of binocular rivalry and pupillometry gives new insights into the physiological mechanisms underlying deprivation effects.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Letter
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    眼睛之间的图像差异会导致视觉处理速度的眼间差异。视觉处理速度的毫秒级差异会导致对运动对象的深度和三维方向的严重误解。这里,我们开发了单目和双目连续目标跟踪心理物理学范式,可以量化视觉处理速度的微小差异。人类观察者以每只眼睛中的一系列亮度水平连续地跟踪经历布朗运动的目标。适当的分析可以恢复每种情况下视觉运动反应的时间过程,视觉处理速度对亮度水平的依赖性,以及眼睛之间处理差异的时间演变。重要的是,使用直接的观察者内部比较,我们表明,连续目标跟踪和传统的强制选择心理物理方法提供了平均在几分之一毫秒内一致的两眼间延迟的估计。因此,视觉处理延迟保留在手的运动动力学中。最后,我们分析性地展示,并通过实验部分证实,两只眼睛的时间脉冲响应函数之间的差异预测了横向目标运动如何导致对深度运动和相关跟踪响应的误解。由于连续目标跟踪可以准确恢复毫秒级的视觉处理速度差异,并且相对于传统的心理物理学具有多种优势,它应该有助于将来对时间处理的研究。
    Image differences between the eyes can cause interocular discrepancies in the speed of visual processing. Millisecond-scale differences in visual processing speed can cause dramatic misperceptions of the depth and three-dimensional direction of moving objects. Here, we develop a monocular and binocular continuous target-tracking psychophysics paradigm that can quantify such tiny differences in visual processing speed. Human observers continuously tracked a target undergoing Brownian motion with a range of luminance levels in each eye. Suitable analyses recover the time course of the visuomotor response in each condition, the dependence of visual processing speed on luminance level, and the temporal evolution of processing differences between the eyes. Importantly, using a direct within-observer comparison, we show that continuous target-tracking and traditional forced-choice psychophysical methods provide estimates of interocular delays that agree on average to within a fraction of a millisecond. Thus, visual processing delays are preserved in the movement dynamics of the hand. Finally, we show analytically, and partially confirm experimentally, that differences between the temporal impulse response functions in the two eyes predict how lateral target motion causes misperceptions of motion in depth and associated tracking responses. Because continuous target tracking can accurately recover millisecond-scale differences in visual processing speed and has multiple advantages over traditional psychophysics, it should facilitate the study of temporal processing in the future.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    双目视觉可塑性可以通过自下而上或自上而下的机制启动,但尚不清楚这两种形式的成人可塑性是否可以独立结合。在7名双眼视力正常的参与者中,感官眼睛优势是使用双目竞争任务评估的,在单眼剥夺之前和之后,并且有或没有针对一只眼睛的选择性注意。在每次审判中,参与者报告了主导单眼目标,并且系统改变了刺激之间的眼间对比差异,以获得对眼优势的估计.我们发现,单目光线和模式剥夺都改变了优势,有利于被剥夺的眼睛。然而,如果有选择地参加非剥夺眼睛的刺激,这种转变是完全抵消的。这些结果表明,眼球优势的变化,由自下而上和自上而下的选择驱动,似乎独立地调节两眼之间的相对对比度增益。
    Binocular visual plasticity can be initiated via either bottom-up or top-down mechanisms, but it is unknown if these two forms of adult plasticity can be independently combined. In seven participants with normal binocular vision, sensory eye dominance was assessed using a binocular rivalry task, before and after a period of monocular deprivation and with and without selective attention directed towards one eye. On each trial, participants reported the dominant monocular target and the inter-ocular contrast difference between the stimuli was systematically altered to obtain estimates of ocular dominance. We found that both monocular light- and pattern-deprivation shifted dominance in favour of the deprived eye. However, this shift was completely counteracted if the non-deprived eye\'s stimulus was selectively attended. These results reveal that shifts in ocular dominance, driven by bottom-up and top-down selection, appear to act independently to regulate the relative contrast gain between the two eyes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Helmholtz询问人们是否可以仅根据视网膜信号来区分哪只眼睛是感知的起源。迄今为止的研究表明,参与者分辨起源眼的能力很可能取决于上下文线索。然而,已经表明,外源性注意力可以增强单眼呈现的刺激的表现。我们质疑是否可以训练成年人区分其感知的起源眼,以及这种能力是否取决于单眼通道的强度。我们使用注意前馈训练来提高受试者的本源眼辨别能力,并提供自愿注意。在培训期间,参与者获得了双眼提示,告知他们即将到来的目标的起源眼.使用连续闪光抑制,我们还测量了单眼目标的信号强度,以查看与线索相关的任何可能的调制。我们从参与者那里收集了关于他们的起源判断的信心评级,以进一步详细研究元认知是否可以访问这些信息。我们的研究结果表明,即使自愿注意并没有改变单眼通道的强度,经过培训后,原产地眼的辨别表现有所改善。对于置信度观察到类似的模式。前馈注意训练的结果和主观信心的增加表明,高级决策机制负责起源判断。我们建议,这种高级过程是由微妙的感官线索(例如两个单眼通道中的亮度或对比度差异)提供信息的。
    Helmholtz asked whether one could discriminate which eye is the origin of one\'s perception merely based on the retinal signals. Studies to date showed that participants\' ability to tell the eye-of-origin most likely depends on contextual cues. Nevertheless, it has been shown that exogenous attention can enhance performance for monocularly presented stimuli. We questioned whether adults can be trained to discriminate the eye-of-origin of their perceptions and if this ability depends on the strength of the monocular channels. We used attentional feed-forward training to improve the subject\'s eye-of-origin discrimination performance with voluntary attention. During training, participants received a binocular cue to inform them of the eye-of-origin of an upcoming target. Using continuous flash suppression, we also measured the signal strength of the monocular targets to see any possible modulations related to the cues. We collected confidence ratings from the participants about their eye-of-origin judgements to study in further detail whether metacognition has access to this information. Our results show that, even though voluntary attention did not alter the strength of the monocular channels, eye-of-origin discrimination performance improved following the training. A similar pattern was observed for confidence. The results from the feedforward attentional training and the increase in subjective confidence point towards a high-level decisional mechanism being responsible for the eye-of-origin judgements. We propose that this high-level process is informed by subtle sensory cues such as the differences in luminance or contrast in the two monocular channels.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近的研究表明,双眼添加S和差分S通道在双眼视觉中起着重要作用。为了在双眼对比检测和双眼求和的背景下测试这种作用,我们采用了一种环绕掩蔽范例,该范例由一个由掩模环围绕的中心目标盘组成。所有刺激均为水平取向的0.5c/d正弦光栅。相关刺激在眼间空间相位相同,而反相关刺激在眼间空间相位相反。有四个目标条件:单眼左眼,单眼右眼,双眼相关和双眼反相关,和三个环绕掩模条件:无环绕,双耳相关和双耳反相关。我们观察到在两种双目环绕面罩条件下单目和双目目标的检测阈值的一致升高。此外,我们发现了环绕物类型和双目目标类型之间的相互作用:具有相反眼间相位关系的环绕物掩模和目标相对增强了检测和求和,而具有相同眼间相位关系的环绕物掩模和目标相对增强了检测和求和。数据被称为MAX(S+S-)的双目组合模型合理地解释,其中决策变量是建模的S+和S-通道响应的概率总和,自由参数确定两个通道的相对增益。我们的结果支持存在两个通道参与双眼组合,S+和S-,其相对增益可通过环绕上下文调整。
    Recent studies suggest that binocular adding S+ and differencing S- channels play an important role in binocular vision. To test for such a role in the context of binocular contrast detection and binocular summation, we employed a surround masking paradigm consisting of a central target disk surrounded by a mask annulus. All stimuli were horizontally oriented 0.5c/d sinusoidal gratings. Correlated stimuli were identical in interocular spatial phase while anticorrelated stimuli were opposite in interocular spatial phase. There were four target conditions: monocular left eye, monocular right eye, binocular correlated and binocular anticorrelated, and three surround mask conditions: no surround, binocularly correlated and binocularly anticorrelated. We observed consistent elevation of detection thresholds for monocular and binocular targets across the two binocular surround mask conditions. In addition, we found an interaction between the type of surround and the type of binocular target: both detection and summation were relatively enhanced by surround masks and targets with opposite interocular phase relationships and reduced by surround masks and targets with the same interocular phase relationships. The data were reasonably well accounted for by a model of binocular combination termed MAX (S+S-), in which the decision variable is the probability summation of modeled S+ and S- channel responses, with a free parameter determining the relative gains of the two channels. Our results support the existence of two channels involved in binocular combination, S+ and S-, whose relative gains are adjustable by surround context.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:根据单眼深度线索评估青光眼对感知三维(3D)形状的影响。
    方法:临床观察性研究。
    方法:20例青光眼患者,使用Humphrey视野分析仪进行双目视野敏感度(双目VFS)测试,和20名年龄匹配的健康志愿者,经历了两个任务:使用单目阴影(3D-SfS)识别3D形状的最近顶点,纹理(3D-SfT),或运动(3D-SfM)提示,并区分这些线索的基本一维(1D)特征。还检查了双眼VFS的视野指数(VFI)与青光眼患者的3D形状感知的关联。
    结果:与健康志愿者相比,青光眼患者在区分1D亮度亮度方面的准确性降低,3D-SfM和3D-SfS的感知深度和实际深度之间的“深度误差”更大。6例双眼VFS的VFI为100%的青光眼患者表现出与其他14例青光眼患者相似的深度误差;与健康志愿者相比,他们对3D-SfM的深度误差更大。在深度误差值和双眼VFS的VFI值之间没有观察到相关性。
    结论:青光眼患者的3D形状感知根据深度提示的特征而变化。在双眼VFS为100%VFI的青光眼患者中,1D辨别能力受损和3D-SfM阈值较大,表明3D-SfS的低级基本特征的感知缺陷更为明显,3D-SfM的3D形状的高级视觉处理更为明显。双眼视野缺陷的位置和程度对3D形状感知的影响仍有待阐明。我们的研究为青光眼的3D形状提取机制提供了见解。
    OBJECTIVE: To assess the impact of glaucoma on perceiving three-dimensional (3D) shapes based on monocular depth cues.
    METHODS: Clinical observational study.
    METHODS: Twenty glaucoma patients, subjected to binocular visual-field sensitivity (binocular-VFS) tests using a Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer, and 20 age-matched healthy volunteers, underwent two tasks: identifying the nearest vertex of a 3D shape using monocular shading (3D-SfS), texture (3D-SfT), or motion (3D-SfM) cues, and distinguishing elementary one-dimensional (1D) features of these cues. The association of the visual-field index (VFI) of binocular-VFS with 3D shape perception in glaucoma patients was also examined.
    RESULTS: Glaucoma patients demonstrated reduced accuracy in distinguishing 1D luminance brightness and a larger \"error-in-depth\" between the perceived and actual depths for 3D-SfM and 3D-SfS compared to healthy volunteers. Six glaucoma patients with a 100% VFI for binocular-VFS exhibited a similar error-in-depth to the other fourteen glaucoma patients; they had a larger error-in-depth for 3D-SfM compared to healthy volunteers. No correlation between the error-in-depth values and the VFI values of binocular-VFS was observed.
    CONCLUSIONS: The 3D shape perception in glaucoma patients varies based on the depth cue\'s characteristics. Impaired 1D discrimination and larger thresholds for 3D-SfM in glaucoma patients with a 100% VFI for binocular-VFS indicate more pronounced perceptual deficits of lower-level elementary features for 3D-SfS and higher-level visual processing of 3D shapes for 3D-SfM. The effects of the location and degree of binocular visual-field defects on 3D shape perception remain to be elucidated. Our research provides insights into the 3D shape extraction mechanism in glaucoma.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    研究单眼抬高缺陷的罕见和不寻常原因。
    对5例出现复视和抬高缺陷的患者进行了彻底检查,发现由于罕见原因而出现单眼抬高缺陷。
    所有五个都被发现具有不同的潜在病因-医源性,蝶骨翼脑膜瘤,囊虫病,结节病和中脑梗塞,并得到了适当的管理。
    单目标高缺陷可由多种原因引起。对更严重的病因有很高的怀疑指数是至关重要的。全面的临床检查和影像学检查有助于明确诊断。
    UNASSIGNED: To study the rare and unusual causes of monocular elevation deficit.
    UNASSIGNED: Five patients presenting to us with diplopia and elevation deficit were thoroughly examined and were found to have monocular elevation deficit due to rare causes.
    UNASSIGNED: All five were found to have different underlying etiologies - iatrogenic, sphenoid wing meningioma, cysticercosis, sarcoidosis and mid brain infarct, and were managed appropriately.
    UNASSIGNED: Monocular Elevation Deficit can occur due to a variety of causes. Having a high index of suspicion for the more serious etiologies is of utmost importance. Thorough clinical examination and imaging help clinch the diagnosis.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号