Social dominance

社会支配地位
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    研究表明,婴儿代表合法的领导,并预测继续服从权威,但是他们使用哪种线索仍然未知。在八个预先注册的实验中,所提供的线索各不相同,我们测试了挪威21个月大的孩子(N=128)是否期望三个主角即使在她不在的情况下也服从角色。我们评估了是否为角色鞠躬,接受主角的致敬或给予主角的利益,对他们施加成本(强行获取资源或打击他们),或相对的身体大小被用作线索,以产生持续服从的期望,这标志着合法的领导。而鞠躬足以产生这样的期望,我们发现了积极的贝叶斯证据,表明所有其他线索都没有。挪威婴儿不太可能在日常生活中目睹鞠躬。因此,鞠躬/虚脱作为持续服从的线索可能是早期发展能力的一部分,以代表进化建立的领导力。
    Research has shown that infants represent legitimate leadership and predict continued obedience to authority, but which cues they use to do so remains unknown. Across eight pre-registered experiments varying the cue provided, we tested if Norwegian 21-month-olds (N=128) expected three protagonists to obey a character even in her absence. We assessed whether bowing for the character, receiving a tribute from or conferring a benefit to the protagonists, imposing a cost on them (forcefully taking a resource or hitting them), or relative physical size were used as cues to generate the expectation of continued obedience that marks legitimate leadership. Whereas bowing sufficed in generating such an expectation, we found positive Bayesian evidence that all the other cues did not. Norwegian infants unlikely have witnessed bowing in their everyday life. Hence, bowing/prostration as cue for continued obedience may form part of an early-developing capacity to represent leadership built by evolution.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    社会交往有时会成为压力的来源,但是社交伙伴也可以改善和缓冲压力。压力和新陈代谢紧密相连,但是,社会同伴在压力情况下调节代谢反应的程度以及这种影响是否因社会等级而异,人们对此知之甚少。为了调查这个问题,我们研究了新金丝雀,坦any尼喀湖特有的群居丽鱼鱼,测量了显性和下级个体在可见或相互隐藏时的代谢反应。当个人可以看到对方时,与优势人群相比,次级人群的最大代谢率较低,并且在进行详尽的追逐后恢复所需的时间更长。相比之下,当个体无法看到彼此时,支配者和下属的代谢反应没有差异。这些发现表明,显性个体的存在对下属有负面的代谢后果,即使在亲社会关系牢固的稳定社会群体中。
    Social interactions can sometimes be a source of stress, but social companions can also ameliorate and buffer against stress. Stress and metabolism are closely linked, but the degree to which social companions modulate metabolic responses during stressful situations-and whether such effects differ depending on social rank-is poorly understood. To investigate this question, we studied Neolamprologus pulcher, a group-living cichlid fish endemic to Lake Tanganyika and measured the metabolic responses of dominant and subordinate individuals when they were either visible or concealed from one another. When individuals could see each other, subordinates had lower maximum metabolic rates and tended to take longer to recover following an exhaustive chase compared with dominants. In contrast, metabolic responses of dominants and subordinates did not differ when individuals could not see one another. These findings suggest that the presence of a dominant individual has negative metabolic consequences for subordinates, even in stable social groups with strong prosocial relationships.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    侵略性和延伸,支配,是确定获取配偶或领土等资源的重要机制。虽然对比赛结果和优势的预测因素的研究越来越多,我们对它们在不同人群中的差异了解甚少。这里,我用的是分布广泛的澳大利亚阿米拉蜥蜴,Jacky龙(Amphibolurusmuricatus),量化预测不同人群男性比赛结果的特征变化。我测量了物理属性,最大生理表现能力(冲刺速度,耐力,咬合力)和分阶段相遇期间的视觉显示。我发现形态学,在激动性相互作用中使用的表现能力以及视觉显示的类型和频率在人群中差异很大。来自CannRiverStateForest的比赛获胜者偏爱甩尾和俯卧撑/身体岩石,而来自皇家国家公园的人更有可能追逐,而来自亚拉特州立森林的人比其他人群表现出更多的冲刺。比赛的失败者在展示上也有所不同。根据行为属性,来自坎恩河人口的个体比其他人占优势(即,侵略性的视觉显示,追逐和咬伤)。我建议,在环境快速变化的时代,随着人口接触,信号形式和功能的人口差异可能会对范围动态产生影响。
    Being aggressive and by extension, dominant, is an important mechanism for determining access to resources such as mates or territories. While predictors of contest outcome and dominance are increasingly studied, we have a poor understanding of how they vary across populations. Here, I use the widely distributed Australian agamid lizard, the Jacky dragon (Amphibolurus muricatus), to quantify variation in features predicting contest outcome among males of different populations. I measured physical attributes, maximal physiological performance capacity (sprint speed, endurance, bite force) and visual displays during staged encounters. I found that morphology, performance capacity and the type and frequency of visual displays used during agonistic interactions varied significantly across populations. Contest winners from the Cann River State Forest population favored tail-flicks and push-up/body-rocks, while those from Royal National Park were more likely to chase and individuals from Yarratt State Forest performed more bite-lunges than other populations. The losers of contests also differed in their displays. Individuals from the Cann River population were dominant over the others based on behavioral attributes (i.e., aggressive visual displays, chases and bite-lunges). I suggest that population differences in signal form and function could have implications for range dynamics as populations come into contact in an era of rapid environmental change.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    优势是社会性动物社会动态和资源获取的主要决定因素。最近的研究表明,优势也反映在外周免疫细胞的基因调节谱中。然而,这种关系的强度和方向在所研究的物种和性别组合中有所不同,可能是由于预测因素的变化和优势地位的能量后果。这里,我们调查了野生猫猫(Suricatasuricatta;n=113个人)的血液中的社会地位和基因表达之间的关联,包括对脂多糖的反应,Gardiquimod(TLR7的激动剂,体内检测单链RNA)和糖皮质激素刺激。猫鼬是合作繁殖的社会食肉动物,其中繁殖的雌性在身体上胜过其他雌性以抑制繁殖,导致高生殖偏斜。因此,它们提供了一个机会,可以将社会支配地位的影响与性别本身的影响分开。我们确定了特定性别的优势标志,包括1045个女性差异表达基因,但男性没有差异表达基因。显性雌性在先天免疫途径中表现出升高的活性和对LPS攻击的较大倍数变化响应。根据这些结果和与其他哺乳动物的初步比较,我们推测免疫系统中社会地位的基因调控特征取决于社会支配地位的决定因素和能量成本,因此,在身体竞争很重要且生殖偏斜很大的等级制度中,这种现象最为明显。这种模式有可能调解生殖投资与躯体维持之间的生活史权衡。
    Dominance is a primary determinant of social dynamics and resource access in social animals. Recent studies show that dominance is also reflected in the gene regulatory profiles of peripheral immune cells. However, the strength and direction of this relationship differs across the species and sex combinations investigated, potentially due to variation in the predictors and energetic consequences of dominance status. Here, we investigated the association between social status and gene expression in the blood of wild meerkats (Suricata suricatta; n = 113 individuals), including in response to lipopolysaccharide, Gardiquimod (an agonist of TLR7, which detects single-stranded RNA in vivo) and glucocorticoid stimulation. Meerkats are cooperatively breeding social carnivores in which breeding females physically outcompete other females to suppress reproduction, resulting in high reproductive skew. They therefore present an opportunity to disentangle the effects of social dominance from those of sex per se. We identify a sex-specific signature of dominance, including 1045 differentially expressed genes in females but none in males. Dominant females exhibit elevated activity in innate immune pathways and a larger fold-change response to LPS challenge. Based on these results and a preliminary comparison to other mammals, we speculate that the gene regulatory signature of social status in the immune system depends on the determinants and energetic costs of social dominance, such that it is most pronounced in hierarchies where physical competition is important and reproductive skew is large. Such a pattern has the potential to mediate life history trade-offs between investment in reproduction versus somatic maintenance.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    社会生活为灵长类动物(包括人类)提供了许多好处。沟通被认为是连接社会关系的关键机制,这可以解释为什么灵长类动物进化出如此富有表现力的面孔。我们评估了9组圈养恒河猴(猕猴)中占主导地位的男性的面部表现力(从基于解剖学的面部运动编码中量化)是否与社交网络属性(基于社交接近度和修饰)有关在统一的物理和社会环境中。更具面部表现力的雄性猕猴与社会的联系更紧密,社会群体的凝聚力也更强。这些发现表明,面部表现力的个体间差异与个人和群体层面的不同社会结果有关。更具表现力的个人占据更多有益的社会地位,这可能有助于解释灵长类动物复杂面部交流的选择。
    Social living affords primates (including humans) many benefits. Communication has been proposed to be the key mechanism used to bond social connections, which could explain why primates have evolved such expressive faces. We assessed whether the facial expressivity of the dominant male (quantified from the coding of anatomically based facial movement) was related to social network properties (based on social proximity and grooming) in nine groups of captive rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) housed in uniform physical and social environments. More facially expressive dominant male macaques were more socially connected and had more cohesive social groups. These findings show that inter-individual differences in facial expressivity are related to differential social outcomes at both an individual and group level. More expressive individuals occupy more beneficial social positions, which could help explain the selection for complex facial communication in primates.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    马使用者和看护人必须意识到混合社会群体的风险。当前的研究调查了八名马从业者是否可以评估20匹马的社会优势等级。在饲料对抗试验中观察到马的进食时间和激动/侵略和顺从行为,并计算了优势度指数(DI)。肯德尔的W,斯皮尔曼相关性和因子分析被用来检验评估者的一致性,优势等级与行为变量之间的关系,并确定聚集行为。在支配顺序的分类中,所有评估者之间的一致性从中等到完美不等。每个评分者的排名与饲料对抗测试中的进食时间和DI呈强烈负相关,证明更顺从的马的喂食时间更短。受到威胁时马的退出是行为变量,最常与评估者排名相关。目前的研究证实了从业者根据他们的社交互动对他们照顾下的马进行分类的能力。此外,当拒绝访问Feed时滚动被提议为挫折释放(重定向)行为。
    Horse users and caretakers must be aware of the risks of mixing social groups. The current study investigated whether eight equine practitioners can assess the social dominance rank of 20 horses. The horses\' feeding time and agonistic/aggressive and submissive behaviours were observed during the feed confrontation test, and the dominance index (DI) was calculated. Kendal\'s W, Spearman correlations and factor analysis were applied to test the raters\' agreement, the relationship between dominance ranks and the behavioural variables, and to determine the clustered behaviours. The agreement between all raters in the classification of dominance order ranged from moderate to perfect. The ranking by every rater was strongly and negatively correlated with the time of eating in feed confrontation tests and with the DI, evidencing shorter feeding times for more submissive horses. The withdrawal of the horse when threatened was the behavioural variable that was most often correlated with raters\' ranking. The current study confirmed the abilities of practitioners to categorise the horses under their care according to their social interactions. Additionally, rolling when denied access to feed was proposed as frustration-releasing (redirected) behaviour.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:在数字时代,欺凌表现为两种不同的形式:传统欺凌和网络欺凌。儿童的同伴关系是欺凌的重要预测因素,欺凌反过来预测同伴关系。然而,很少有研究人员注意到同伴关系和欺凌之间的双向关系。
    方法:本研究采用了两波交叉滞后纵向设计来填补这一空白。在这种关系中也检查了潜在的性别差异。样本包括527名8至12岁的中国儿童(M=9.69,SD=.96;53.5%为女性)。参与者完成了同行提名以获得同行认可,同伴排斥和社会支配地位,以及传统欺凌和网络欺凌的自我报告。
    结果:结果表明,在第一时间点(T1)的同伴拒绝显着并且积极地预测了传统的欺凌行为,第二时间点(T2)的网络欺凌行为和网络欺凌受害。T1时的传统欺凌受害显着,并负面地预测了T2时的同伴接受和社会支配地位。结果还显示了显着的男性和女性差异。例如,在男孩中,T1时的同伴接受度显着,并负面地预测了T2时的网络欺凌受害。相比之下,在女孩中没有观察到这种关系。本研究结果对于理解同伴关系和欺凌之间的周期性关系,并为改善同伴关系和减少欺凌提供实践指导具有重要意义。
    BACKGROUND: In the digital age, bullying manifests in two distinct forms: traditional bullying and cyberbullying. Children\'s peer relationships are important predictors of bullying, and bullying in turn predicts peer relationships. However, few researchers have noted the bidirectional relationship between peer relationships and bullying.
    METHODS: The present study used a two-wave cross-lagged longitudinal design to fill this gap. The potential sex differences were also examined in this relationship. The sample consisted of 527 Chinese children aged 8 to 12 years (M = 9.69, SD = .96; 53.5% female). Participants completed peer nominations for peer acceptance, peer rejection and social dominance, as well as self-reports of traditional bullying and cyberbullying.
    RESULTS: Results showed that peer rejection at the first time point (T1) significantly and positively predicted traditional bullying perpetration, cyberbullying perpetration and cyberbullying victimization at the second time point (T2). Traditional bullying victimization at T1 significantly and negatively predicted peer acceptance and social dominance at T2. The results also revealed significant male and female differences. For instance, among boys, peer acceptance at T1 significantly and negatively predicted cyberbullying victimization at T2. In contrast, this relationship was not observed among girls. The present findings have important implications for understanding the cyclical relationship between peer relationships and bullying and providing practical guidance for improving peer relationships and reducing bullying.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:断奶后是大脑发育和个体生长的关键时期。作为一种重要的药物,食品和饮料,柠檬酸钠(SC)是常见的。尽管已经证明了SC暴露对个体生理的一些影响,断奶后膳食SC暴露对社会行为的潜在长期影响仍然难以捉摸.
    方法:断奶后雄性和雌性C57BL/6小鼠均通过饮用水暴露于SC,共3周。一系列的行为测试,包括社会支配能力测试(SDT),社交互动测试(SIT),床上用品偏好测试(BPT)和性偏好测试(SPT),在青春期和成年期进行。经过这些测试,检测血清催产素(OT)水平和肠道菌群。
    结果:行为结果显示,断奶后SC暴露降低了成年雄性小鼠和青春期和成年期雌性小鼠的社会优势。SC暴露也降低了男性和女性的性偏好率,虽然它对社会互动行为没有影响。ELISA结果表明,SC暴露降低了女性的血清OT水平,而不是男性。16SrRNA测序分析显示,男性和女性暴露于SC后,β多样性存在显着差异。相关系数表明社会行为之间的相关性,肠道菌群的OT水平和优势属。
    结论:我们的研究结果表明,断奶后SC暴露可能对社会行为产生持久和性别依赖的影响,这可能与血清OT水平和肠道菌群组成的改变有关。
    BACKGROUND: Postweaning is a pivotal period for brain development and individual growth. As an important chemical used in medicines, foods and beverages, sodium citrate (SC) is commonly available. Although some effects of SC exposure on individual physiology have been demonstrated, the potential long-lasting effects of postweaning dietary SC exposure on social behaviours are still elusive.
    METHODS: Both postweaning male and female C57BL/6 mice were exposed to SC through drinking water for a total of 3 weeks. A series of behavioural tests, including social dominance test (SDT), social interaction test (SIT), bedding preference test (BPT) and sexual preference test (SPT), were performed in adolescence and adulthood. After these tests, serum oxytocin (OT) levels and gut microbiota were detected.
    RESULTS: The behavioural results revealed that postweaning SC exposure decreased the social dominance of male mice in adulthood and female mice in both adolescence and adulthood. SC exposure also reduced the sexual preference rates of both males and females, while it had no effect on social interaction behaviour. ELISA results indicated that SC exposure decreased the serum OT levels of females but not males. 16S rRNA sequencing analysis revealed a significant difference in β-diversity after SC exposure in both males and females. The correlation coefficient indicated the correlation between social behaviours, OT levels and dominant genera of gut microbiota.
    CONCLUSIONS: Our findings suggest that postweaning SC exposure may have enduring and sex-dependent effects on social behaviours, which may be correlated with altered serum OT levels and gut microbiota composition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在马达加斯加狐猴中,雌性对雄性的统治比在其他灵长类动物中更为频繁,但是雌雄优势的模式因物种而异,一个物种特别丰富的属的数据很少,Lepilemur.我们调查了类型,时间分布,以及米尔恩-爱德华兹运动型狐猴(Lepilemuredwardsi)中男女激动冲突的结果。1998年5月至11月期间,属于五个社会单位的10名爱德华博士配备了无线电项圈,并在79个联络人之后进行了观察。我们量化了激烈的冲突,每月冲突率,并记录了所有冲突的赢家和背景。群体内男女激动性冲突(N=162)的平均发生率为0.21冲突/小时。激动性冲突在出生季节达到顶峰,主要发生在未知或婴儿接近的情况下。女性赢得了所有决定性激动性冲突的96%(N=154)。其他结果仅在女性在出生季节与婴儿在一起时才发生。在这种情况下,一名女性有时会退出她的伴侣,另一名女性退出或逃离了一名额外的男性,其袭击最终导致了杀婴。我们的结果表明,女性在成对生活的爱德华斯中占主导地位。我们假设女性攻击性的提高可能会给女性Lepilemur带来健身益处,因为它通常允许女性代表其婴儿进行快速有效的干预,以对抗非父亲男性,这些男性在将新生儿停在植被中时可能会接近新生儿。
    Female dominance over males is more frequent in Malagasy lemurs than in other primate clades, but patterns of female-male dominance vary among species, and few data are available for one particularly species-rich genus, Lepilemur. We investigated the types, temporal distribution, and outcome of female-male agonistic conflicts in the Milne-Edwards\' sportive lemur (Lepilemur edwardsi). Ten L. edwardsi belonging to five social units were equipped with radio collars and observed during 79 focal follows between May and November 1998. We quantified agonistic conflicts, monthly conflict rates, and documented the winner and context for all conflicts. Female-male agonistic conflicts (N = 162) occurred at a mean rate of 0.21 conflicts/hour within groups. Agonistic conflicts peaked during the birth season and occurred mostly in unknown or infant proximity contexts. Females won 96% of all decided agonistic conflicts (N = 154). Other outcomes occurred only when females were with infants during the birth season. In that context, one female sometimes withdrew from her pair partner, and another female withdrew or fled from an extra-group male whose attacks eventually led to infanticide. Our results suggest female dominance in pair-living L. edwardsi. We hypothesize that elevated female aggression may convey fitness benefits to female Lepilemur because it generally allows females to intervene quickly and efficiently on behalf of their infants against nonfather males who may approach the newborn infants when they are parked in vegetation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    增强的生存和繁殖与个人与群体成员的直接和间接社会联系有关。然而,这些联系的作用在许多灵长类动物中鲜为人知。我们研究了喜马拉雅中部叶猴(CHL)的女性,以调查四个特定属性(优势等级,年龄,遗传相关性,以及携带婴儿的女性的存在)以及女性的直接和间接社会关系。通过分析梳理网络,我们揭示了不同的行为策略:高级女性与许多女性形成关系(高学位),而有依赖性婴儿的女性有很强的关系(高强度和特征向量)。亚成年女性是将社交网络保持在一起的重要个体(高介数),而移民女性的策略是通过与自己拥有强大纽带(高特征向量)的女性形成强大的纽带来将自己融入群体。我们的研究揭示了行为策略如何塑造女性CHL美容网络,这可能有助于他们获得健身和生存优势。
    Enhanced survival and reproduction are associated with an individual\'s direct and indirect social connections with members of a group. Yet, the role of these connections is little known in a vast range of primate species. We studied female Central Himalayan Langur (CHL) to investigate the link between four specific attributes (dominance rank, age, genetic relatedness, and the presence of females carrying infants) and a female\'s direct and indirect social relationships. By analyzing grooming networks, we revealed different behavioral strategies: high-ranking females form relationships with many females (high degree), whereas females with dependent infants have strong relationships (high strength and eigenvector). Subadult females are important individuals that hold the social network together (high betweenness), while an immigrant female strategy is to integrate herself into the group by forming strong bonds with females who themselves have strong bonds (high eigenvector). Our study sheds light on how behavioral strategies shape female CHL grooming networks, which may help them to secure fitness and survival advantages.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号