Social dominance

社会支配地位
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    支配观念在社会交往中起着重要作用。尽管许多研究人员提出,塑造男性气质是支配知觉的重要面部线索,这种说法的证据几乎完全来自使用强制选择范式评估实验操纵面孔感知的研究。因此,我们调查了男性形状特征在男性面部优势感知中的作用(1)当形状操纵刺激以强制选择范式呈现时,以及(2)当未经操纵的面部图像被评定为优势并从面部图像测量形状男性气质时。尽管当我们使用强制选择方法时,我们观察到男性气质对支配感的巨大影响(科恩的ds=2.51和3.28),当对未处理的面部图像进行评级并从面部图像测量男性气概时,男性气概对优势知觉的影响要小得多(科恩的ds=0.44和0.62)。当面部被分别评定为身体优势时,观察到了这种模式,社会支配地位,和男子气概,并观察到两组不同的刺激。总的来说,这些结果表明,当面孔在多个维度上同时变化时,男性气质可能不是支配知觉的特别重要的线索,在日常社交互动中也是如此。
    Dominance perceptions play an important role in social interactions. Although many researchers have proposed that shape masculinity is an important facial cue for dominance perceptions, evidence for this claim has come almost exclusively from studies that assessed perceptions of experimentally manipulated faces using forced-choice paradigms. Consequently, we investigated the role of masculine shape characteristics in perceptions of men\'s facial dominance (1) when shape-manipulated stimuli were presented in a forced-choice paradigm and (2) when unmanipulated face images were rated for dominance and shape masculinity was measured from face images. Although we observed large effects of masculinity on dominance perceptions when we used the forced-choice method (Cohen\'s ds = 2.51 and 3.28), the effect of masculinity on dominance perceptions was considerably smaller when unmanipulated face images were rated and shape masculinity measured from face images (Cohen\'s ds = 0.44 and 0.62). This pattern was observed when faces were rated separately for physical dominance, social dominance, and masculinity, and was seen for two different sets of stimuli. Collectively, these results suggest that shape masculinity may not be a particularly important cue for dominance perceptions when faces vary simultaneously on multiple dimensions, as is the case during everyday social interactions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在我们当前的全球化中,多元文化的世界,了解土著人民和移民背景的人之间的相互互动的前提是一个主要问题,因为群体间的接触在建设包容性社会中起着至关重要的作用。在这种情况下,使用每日日记数据,我们研究了白人英国多数(N=744)和亚裔英国少数群体(N=582)与各自外群成员的每日积极和消极互动之间的关系,RWA,SDO,感知群体规范,邻里多样性和语境剥夺。结果显示,对于大多数群体来说,有利于群体间接触的群体内规范与积极的群体间接触呈正相关,而右翼威权主义(RWA)与负向群体接触呈正相关。邻里多样性与积极和消极的群体间相遇呈正相关。此外,RWA缓和了邻里多样性与白人英国人的积极和消极接触之间的关系。对于少数群体来说,群体内规范与群体间积极接触呈正相关,群体内规范与负接触之间的关系受到SDO的调节。总的来说,不同的因素影响着多数群体和少数群体的积极和消极的群体间接触。我们讨论了这些发现对未来研究和干预的影响。
    In our current globalised, multicultural world, understanding antecedents of reciprocal interactions between native people and people of immigrant background is a major issue, because intergroup contact plays a crucial role in building inclusive societies. In this vein, using daily diary data, we examined the relation between the number of daily positive and negative interactions of White British majority (N = 744) and Asian British minority people (N = 582) with members of the respective outgroup, with RWA, SDO, perceived ingroup norms, neighbourhood diversity and contextual deprivation. Results showed that for the majority group, ingroup norms in favour of intergroup contact were positively associated with positive intergroup encounters, whereas Right Wing Authoritarianism (RWA) was positively associated with negative intergroup contact. Neighbourhood diversity was positively associated with positive and negative intergroup encounters. Moreover, RWA moderated the relationship between neighbourhood diversity and both positive and negative contact of White British people. For the minority group, ingroup norms were positively associated with positive intergroup contact, and the relationship between ingroup norms and negative contact was moderated by SDO. Overall, different factors affect positive and negative intergroup contact of majority and minority groups. We discuss the implications of the findings for future research and interventions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    一个世纪前,ThorleifSchjelderup-Ebbe的基础工作描述了鸡社会中的“啄食顺序”,可以根据个人对其同伴施加影响力的能力来命令个人。现在被称为支配等级制度,这些结构已被证明会影响过多的个体特征和结果,作为现代社会生态学和进化研究的支柱。这里,我们首先回顾了过去100年中有关优势等级的一些主要问题。接下来,我们介绍了对这个主题问题的贡献,并总结了它们如何在认识论中提供持续的见解,生理学和神经生物学,层次结构,和主导地位的动态。这些贡献采用了现代生物学家可用的全方位研究方法。这些贡献产生的交叉主题包括关注支配地位的认知基础,网络分析方法的应用,以及实验等级操作用于揭示因果关系的效用。对过去100年的优势研究的反思揭示了Schjelderup-Ebbe的早期思想和随后的研究如何帮助推动从物种特征的本质论观点转变为对社会中丰富的个体间变异的现代认识,行为和生理表型。本文是主题问题“啄食顺序的一百周年:优势等级研究的现状和未来前景”的一部分。
    A century ago, foundational work by Thorleif Schjelderup-Ebbe described a \'pecking order\' in chicken societies, where individuals could be ordered according to their ability to exert their influence over their group-mates. Now known as dominance hierarchies, these structures have been shown to influence a plethora of individual characteristics and outcomes, situating dominance research as a pillar of the study of modern social ecology and evolution. Here, we first review some of the major questions that have been answered about dominance hierarchies in the last 100 years. Next, we introduce the contributions to this theme issue and summarize how they provide ongoing insight in the epistemology, physiology and neurobiology, hierarchical structure, and dynamics of dominance. These contributions employ the full range of research approaches available to modern biologists. Cross-cutting themes emerging from these contributions include a focus on cognitive underpinnings of dominance, the application of network-analytical approaches, and the utility of experimental rank manipulations for revealing causal relationships. Reflection on the last 100 years of dominance research reveals how Schjelderup-Ebbe\'s early ideas and the subsequent research helped drive a shift from an essentialist view of species characteristics to the modern recognition of rich inter-individual variation in social, behavioural and physiological phenotypes. This article is part of the theme issue \'The centennial of the pecking order: current state and future prospects for the study of dominance hierarchies\'.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Personality is not a uniquely human characteristic and it has been documented in a wide range of organisms, from mammals to birds, reptiles, fish, and invertebrates. However, personality is still poorly understood in Cervids. Therefore, our study aimed to fill this gap by i) investigating personality and ii) exploring its links to dominance hierarchy, assessed by behavioral observations in 11 captive and tame male red deer (Cervus elaphus). Using questionnaires to assess personality, three trained volunteers rated these animals in 15 behaviorally composed adjectives with detailed descriptions, based on their overall impression at the end of the observation period. Behavioral data from animals were collected across three different situations, namely \"feeding\" (i.e., high competition for a scarce resource), \"normal\" (i.e., no external stimuli) in a group setting, and \"handling\" (i.e., stressful situation due to human manipulation) in an individual setting. We estimated dominance hierarchies between the individuals based on situations of average and high competition (i.e., \"normal\" and \"feeding\") via the Clutton-Brock Index (CBI). Using Fleiss\' Kappa for inter-rater reliability, only five of our 15 behavioral adjectives showed acceptable reliability. Using principal component analysis, four of these adjectives formed one personality component labelled \"Confidence/Aggressiveness\". We found that although \"Confidence/Aggressiveness\" did not correlate with CBI, ratings of two adjectives loading onto this component, namely \"Confident\" and \"Submissive\", significantly correlated with the CBI, indicating that the questionnaire ratings reflect real behavioral variation in red deer males. Our study provides the first assessment of personality in male red deer and adds to the growing literature on Cervid personality, offering the basis for future personality research in ungulates.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    This ethnographic study was designed to explore living conditions among a group of aboriginal families residing in an urban context, who participated in a recreational social program. The aim of the study was to explore life satisfaction by analyzing their living conditions and cultural capital inscribed in established attitudes, perceptions, and lay knowledge. Fieldwork was carried out in two complementary phases: phase 1, between May 2008 and December 2010, began when the first listed author initiated volunteer work with the organization responsible for the program; in phase 2, which extended from January 2011 to February 2013, participant observation was intensified and semi-structured interviews were carried out. The results indicate that the process of cultural uprooting underscores the experience of social inequality, and suggest a cohort effect that continues to this day. This process of cultural (in)consonance must be taken into account as a key factor when analyzing the living conditions and well-being of ethnic minorities, as well as when developing programs and interventions.
    Este estudio etnográfico fue diseñado para explorar las condiciones de vida de un grupo de familias aborígenes residentes en un contexto urbano, beneficiarias de un programa sociorecreativo en Sídney, Australia. El objetivo fue explorar la satisfacción vital mediante el análisis de sus condiciones de vida y del capital cultural inscrito en sus actitudes, percepciones y conocimientos establecidos. El trabajo de campo se estructuró en dos fases complementarias: en la fase 1, entre mayo de 2008 y diciembre de 2010, se inició con el ingreso del primer autor como voluntario en la asociación responsable del programa y, en la fase 2, entre enero de 2011 y febrero de 2013 se intensificó el proceso de observación participante y las entrevistas semiestructuradas. Los resultados muestran que el proceso de desarraigo cultural está en la base de las experiencias vitales de desigualdad social experimentadas y apunta a un efecto de cohorte que llega hasta nuestros días. Dicho proceso de (des)consonancia cultural debe ser tenido en cuenta como un factor clave a la hora de analizar las condiciones de vida y bienestar de estas minorías étnicas, así como a la hora de desarrollar programas e intervenciones.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • DOI:
    文章类型: Journal Article
    In primates, males compete for a mate, which is a non-sharable resource. This makes the conditions less conducive for males to have stable relationships. One such special kind of relationship is a bond where the interactions are reciprocated, equitable and differentiated. Bonds in macaque societies are based on the degree of within-group contest competition for mates which is dependent on the synchronization of female fertile phase and reliability of fertility signals. Species of the Fascicularis group, including Nicobar subspecies, show intermediate reliability in the signals with mild peaks, and studies have shown reciprocity but no differentiation. We conducted a study on a group of wild Nicobar long-tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis umbrosus to understand the existing patterns of male-male relationships. We examined whether there is reciprocity in affiliation among the individuals and whether the rate of affiliation is balanced. We also measured the dominance linearity and steepness in the group to understand the monopolizability of females. We used social network analysis to understand whether the relations are differentiated based on hierarchical position and whether the high-ranking individuals are the most central individuals in the distribution of grooming in the group. We found that there is reciprocity among the males although that is not equitable. There was no rankrelated differentiation of affiliation among the males of the group. Instead, the identities of individuals influenced affiliation patterns. Our results correspond to the existent strong relationships but lack of social bond otherwise found in the Fascicularis group of macaques.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    恒河猴的一些圈养繁殖群生活在大型户外,多女性社会群体。这些群体由几个母系家族组成,由明确的女性主导等级制度支配。由于社会不稳定,同一母系家庭或不同母系家庭之间的侵略会导致创伤和死亡。因此,主要管理目标是在发生重大战斗和伤害之前发现新出现的社会动荡。因此,定期监测群体的优势和联盟关系的变化以及创伤频率和严重程度的增加。食物摄入减少是对急性应激的正常生理反应;因此,关键动物或猴子群体的食欲不振可能被用作社会压力增加和出现不稳定的指标。最近在我们设施的一个繁殖小组中发生了一起内部侵略事件,并导致了相当大的战斗。因为这种化合物配备了自动喂养系统,可以跟踪单个动物的热量摄入,我们回顾性分析了喂养数据,以确定在攻击开始之前是否发生了热量消耗的显着减少,与基线值比较。在基线和之前的24h值之间,整个组和单个家庭的总热量摄入量均未显示任何显着差异;但是,受影响的家庭在侵略前的24小时内表现出总热量减少了20%。最值得注意的是,被废掉的亚科显示,在过去的24小时内,热量摄入显著减少了58%,而其余亚家族的摄入量没有显着变化。该组的高级动物,包括女性的α,β女性,和α男性,类似地,在此期间热量摄入明显减少。这些发现表明,自动饲养员可以帮助管理人员监测恒河猴繁殖菌落的社会稳定性。
    Some captive breeding colonies of rhesus macaques live in large outdoor multimale, multifemale social groups. These groups are composed of several matrilineal families, governed by a clear female dominance hierarchy. Aggression within the same or between different matrilineal families due to social instability can result in trauma and mortality. Therefore, a primary management goal is to detect emerging social unrest before the onset of significant fighting and wounding. Accordingly, groups are monitored routinely for changes in dominance and alliance relations as well as for increases in trauma frequency and severity. Decreased food intake is a normal physiologic response to acute stress; therefore, inappetence in key animals or groups of monkeys might be used as an indicator of increased social stress and emerging instability. An incident of intrafamily aggression occurred recently in a breeding group at our facility and resulted in considerable fighting. Because this compound was equipped with an automated feeding system that tracks the caloric intake of individual animals, we retrospectively analyzed feeding data to determine whether significant reduction in caloric consumption occurred prior to the onset of aggression, compared with baseline values. Neither the entire group nor individual families showed any significant differences in total caloric intake between baseline and previous 24 h values; however, the affected family exhibited a 20% reduction in total caloric during the 24 h prior to the aggression. Most notably, the deposed subfamily showed a marked 58% reduction in caloric intake during the prior 24 h, whereas remaining subfamilies showed no significant changes in intake. High-ranking animals of the group, including the α female, β female, and α male, similarly exhibited marked decreases in caloric intake during that period. These findings indicate that automated feeders can assist management staff with monitoring social stability in breeding colonies of rhesus macaque.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Cues signaling trust and dominance are crucial for social life. Previous studies on the effects of oxytocin (OT) nasal sprays on trustworthiness evaluations have been inconsistent and its influence on dominance is unknown. Vasopressin (AVP) may also influence social cue perception, but even fewer investigations have evaluated this possibility. We evaluated the effects of intranasal OT and AVP compared to placebo control during three double-blinded functional magnetic resonance imaging sessions. Twenty males received a pseudo-randomized order of nasal spray conditions and rated the trustworthiness and dominance of neutral faces. OT increased facial dominance ratings compared to placebo. Neuroimaging results revealed an inverse relationship between brain activation and face ratings for OT compared to placebo in regions involved in processing emotional expressions. Specifically, the right superior temporal gyrus was attenuated as ratings increased and the left precuneus selectively diminished with increasing dominance ratings. Additionally, OT increased functional connectivity between frontoparietal regions and the right amygdala for faces rated as highly dominant, but OT increased connectivity between the fusiform gyrus, hippocampus, and bilateral ventral tegmental area (VTA) for faces perceived as highly trustworthy. Overall, OT increased the perception of dominance but did not influence trustworthiness judgments. However, we observed regional neural effects for OT that differed between judgments of trustworthiness and dominance. AVP attenuated left temporoparietal junction activity as face ratings increased, a result consistent with AVP influencing mentalization. AVP also led to increased left amygdala and right VTA connectivity with the putamen, which is consistent with cue-driven, habitual responses.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    虽然在压力条件下长大会削弱智力,恶劣环境中的人可能会完好无损地发育,甚至增强,在高度逆境中解决问题的社会和认知能力(即“隐藏人才”)。我们研究了童年和当前暴力暴露是否与记忆(记忆项目之间的关系所需的学习回合数)和涉及暴力相关和暴力中立的传递性推理任务的推理表现(推断新关系的准确性)相关社会信息(社会优势与实际年龄)。我们假设,遭受暴力侵害的人比遭受社会支配地位的人表现更好。我们在一项预先注册的研究中对100名荷兰大学生和99名荷兰社区参与者进行了测试。我们发现,更多的暴力暴露与整体记忆表现降低有关,但没有推理性能。然而,当前(而不是童年)暴露于暴力对记忆的主要影响由显著的交互作用限定。当前更多的邻里暴力与年龄关系记忆恶化有关,但没有记忆支配关系。相比之下,当前更多的个人参与暴力与更好的支配关系记忆有关,但不是与记忆的年龄关系。这些结果表明学习和记忆能力跨内容的不完全转移。这种结果模式,这支持了赤字和隐藏才能的组合,“与更广泛的发展文学有关,它几乎只报告了恶劣条件下的赤字。本文的视频摘要可通过以下网址查看:https://youtu。是/e4ePmSzzsuc。
    Although growing up in stressful conditions can undermine mental abilities, people in harsh environments may develop intact, or even enhanced, social and cognitive abilities for solving problems in high-adversity contexts (i.e. \'hidden talents\'). We examine whether childhood and current exposure to violence are associated with memory (number of learning rounds needed to memorize relations between items) and reasoning performance (accuracy in deducing a novel relation) on transitive inference tasks involving both violence-relevant and violence-neutral social information (social dominance vs. chronological age). We hypothesized that individuals who had more exposure to violence would perform better than individuals with less exposure on the social dominance task. We tested this hypothesis in a preregistered study in 100 Dutch college students and 99 Dutch community participants. We found that more exposure to violence was associated with lower overall memory performance, but not with reasoning performance. However, the main effects of current (but not childhood) exposure to violence on memory were qualified by significant interaction effects. More current exposure to neighborhood violence was associated with worse memory for age relations, but not with memory for dominance relations. By contrast, more current personal involvement in violence was associated with better memory for dominance relations, but not with memory for age relations. These results suggest incomplete transfer of learning and memory abilities across contents. This pattern of results, which supports a combination of deficits and \'hidden talents,\' is striking in relation to the broader developmental literature, which has nearly exclusively reported deficits in people from harsh conditions. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://youtu.be/e4ePmSzZsuc.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Social judgments of faces made by Western participants are thought to be underpinned by two dimensions: valence and dominance. Because some research suggests that Western and Eastern participants process faces differently, the two-dimensional model of face evaluation may not necessarily apply to judgments of faces by Eastern participants. Here we used a data-driven approach to investigate the components underlying social judgments of Chinese faces by Chinese participants. Analyses showed that social judgments of Chinese faces by Chinese participants are partly underpinned by a general approachability dimension similar to the valence dimension previously found to underpin Western participants\' evaluations of White faces. However, we found that a general capability dimension, rather than a dominance dimension, contributed to Chinese participants\' evaluations of Chinese faces. Thus, our findings present evidence for both cultural similarities and cultural differences in social evaluations of faces. Importantly, the dimension that explained most of the variance in Chinese participants\' social judgments of faces was strikingly similar to the valence dimension previously reported for Western participants.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号