Vocalization

发声
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    记录给定分类单元的分布和丰度的变化需要历史数据。在没有收集整个分类单元范围内的长期监测数据的情况下,保护生物学家通常依靠保存的博物馆标本来确定过去或现在,假定的地理分布。休斯顿蟾蜍(Anaxyrushoustonensis)的分布数据一直被与同胞同类物的相似性所混淆,矮人美国蟾蜍(A.Americanuscharlesmithi),无论是在监控来自合唱调查的数据中,以及通过博物馆标本获得的历史数据。在这种情况下,错误识别可能会对保护工作产生意想不到的影响,休斯顿蟾蜍濒临灭绝,矮美国蟾蜍是最不关心的。先前发表的报告根据其男性广告称呼和形态外观对这两个分类单元进行了比较,通常目的是在DNA测序技术出现之前使用这些字符来证实其分类地位。然而,许多研究报告的发现相互矛盾,无法就真正的差异或相似之处达成共识。这里,我们使用每个分类单元的野生种群的当代记录来测试男性广告呼叫的可量化差异。此外,我们定量检查了代表每个分类单元的有证博物馆标本的子集,以测试先前报告的用于区分德克萨斯州中东部其他Bufonids的形态计量学特征,美国。最后,我们收集并定性评估一个代表每个分类单元的博物馆分类券的照片数据库,以确定其先前记录的历史范围是否可能大于目前接受的范围。我们的发现揭示了两种异形同源物之间在男性广告呼叫方面的可量化差异,而我们发现它们的详细形态相似。此外,我们报告了额外的存在,历史上被忽视的,休斯顿蟾蜍在其假定的历史范围内的博物馆记录,并讨论与这些标本的鉴定和命名法随着时间的推移并不一致相关的错误。这些结果结束了几十年关于形态学的分歧,声音,这些类群的历史分布,并提醒从业人员注意休斯顿蟾蜍的保护工作,以确保以前未报告的发生地点。
    Documenting changes in the distribution and abundance of a given taxon requires historical data. In the absence of long-term monitoring data collected throughout the range of a taxon, conservation biologists often rely on preserved museum specimens to determine the past or present, putative geographic distribution. Distributional data for the Houston Toad (Anaxyrus houstonensis) has consistently been confounded by similarities with a sympatric congener, the Dwarf American Toad (A. americanus charlesmithi), both in monitoring data derived from chorusing surveys, and in historical data via museum specimens. In this case, misidentification can have unintended impacts on conservation efforts, where the Houston Toad is federally endangered, and the Dwarf American Toad is of least concern. Previously published reports have compared these two taxon on the basis of their male advertisement call and morphological appearance, often with the goal of using these characters to substantiate their taxonomic status prior to the advent of DNA sequencing technology. However, numerous studies report findings that contradict one another, and no consensus on the true differences or similarities can be drawn. Here, we use contemporary recordings of wild populations of each taxon to test for quantifiable differences in male advertisement call. Additionally, we quantitatively examine a subset of vouchered museum specimens representing each taxon to test previously reported differentiating morphometric characters used to distinguish among other Bufonids of East-Central Texas, USA. Finally, we assemble and qualitatively evaluate a database of photographs representing catalogued museum vouchers for each taxon to determine if their previously documented historic ranges may be larger than are currently accepted. Our findings reveal quantifiable differences between two allopatric congeners with respect to their male advertisement call, whereas we found similarities among their detailed morphology. Additionally, we report on the existence of additional, historically overlooked, museum records for the Houston Toad in the context of its putative historic range, and discuss errors associated with the curation of these specimens whose identity and nomenclature have not been consistent through time. These results bookend decades of disagreement regarding the morphology, voice, and historic distribution of these taxa, and alert practitioners of conservation efforts for the Houston Toad to previously unreported locations of occurrence.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    语音中的语调是对声音音调的控制,以分层表达对交流的意义,比如增加音调来表示问题。此外,音高的刻板模式用于创建具有不同外延的不同声音,像音调语言,也许,鼠类词典中的10种声音。基本的音调是通过收缩的喉部语音盒呼气产生的,人们认为,更复杂的话语仅由喉部张力的动态变化产生。但也许,音高的变化也是由呼气速度的变化引起的。与后一种模式一致,我们描述了大多数发声类型中的语调都是在呼气偏差之后出现的,这似乎是由于主呼吸肌肉的重新激活而产生的。我们还展示了脑干发声中枢模式发生器,iRO,可以创造这种呼吸模式。因此,iRO的异位激活不仅诱导发声,还有构成鼠类词典中大部分发声的音高模式。这些结果揭示了一种新的脑干语调机制。
    Intonation in speech is the control of vocal pitch to layer expressive meaning to communication, like increasing pitch to indicate a question. Also, stereotyped patterns of pitch are used to create distinct sounds with different denotations, like in tonal languages and, perhaps, the 10 sounds in the murine lexicon. A basic tone is created by exhalation through a constricted laryngeal voice box, and it is thought that more complex utterances are produced solely by dynamic changes in laryngeal tension. But perhaps, the shifting pitch also results from altering the swiftness of exhalation. Consistent with the latter model, we describe that intonation in most vocalization types follows deviations in exhalation that appear to be generated by the re-activation of the cardinal breathing muscle for inspiration. We also show that the brainstem vocalization central pattern generator, the iRO, can create this breath pattern. Consequently, ectopic activation of the iRO not only induces phonation, but also the pitch patterns that compose most of the vocalizations in the murine lexicon. These results reveal a novel brainstem mechanism for intonation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    北京鸭暴露于应激源,如热应力,肠道病原体,霉菌毒素,和其他环境压力。我们从野生鸟类文献中知道,鸟类通过发声进行交流。我们假设北京鸭会有不同的曲目,受到性别的影响,社会群体,和特定的刺激。我们利用成年北京鸭开发了声乐曲目。我们将1到4只不同性别的鸭子放入声音室中,并使用各种刺激来鼓励新的发声。记录鸟类20分钟,鸭子的数量和性别有几种变化。一旦记录了鸭子,就会根据预定的命名系统对每个被剪辑的发声进行命名。我们以4种方式描述了鸭子在每种刺激和社会待遇下的声音系统:总体呼叫率,呼叫多样性,调用曲目,并调用光谱属性。在所有情况下,使用Proc单变量(SASv9.4)确认了GLM和ANOVA模型的残差正态和方差的均一性,其中p≤0.05被认为是显著的.我们发现北京鸭产生多达16种不同的发声。处理对鸭子发出的呼叫的总速率具有显著影响(ANOVA:F6,31=8.55,p<0.0001)。到目前为止,当有人和鸭子一起坐在房间里时,鸭子发出的声音最多(30.04±4.45个电话/分钟)。为了呼叫多样性,我们发现母鸡数量有显着的主效应(F218=12.21,p=0.0004),但德雷克数量没有主效应(F3,18=3.04,p=0.0555)。聚类分析表明,在特定条件下给出了某些类型的呼叫。通常有6个主要的声带簇(R平方=0.899,立方聚类标准=9.30)。我们的结果表明,北京鸭受到刺激类型和社会环境的影响,它们发声的程度以及使用的通话特性。此外,男性和女性在他们使用的通话曲目上有所不同,以及它们呼叫的光谱特性。
    Pekin ducks are exposed to stressors such as heat stress, enteric pathogens, mycotoxins, and other environmental stressors. We know from wild bird literature that birds communicate through vocalizations. We hypothesized that Pekin ducks would have a diverse repertoire that is affected by the sex, social group, and specific stimuli. We utilized adult Pekin ducks to develop a vocal repertoire. We placed 1 to 4 ducks of varying sexes into a sound chamber with various stimuli used to encourage new vocalizations. Birds were recorded for 20 min with several variations of number and sexes of ducks. Once the ducks were recorded each vocalization that was clipped was named based on a predetermined naming system. We characterized the vocal system of the ducks under each stimulus and social treatment in 4 ways: overall call rates, call diversity, call repertoire, and call spectral properties. In all cases, normality of residuals and homogeneity of variances for GLM and ANOVA models were confirmed using Proc Univariate (SAS v9.4) where a p ≤ 0.05 was considered significant. We found that Pekin ducks produce up to 16 different vocalizations. The treatments had a significant effect on the overall rate of calls given by the ducks (ANOVA: F6,31 = 8.55, p < 0.0001). Ducks produced the most calls by far when someone was sitting in the chamber with them (30.04 ± 4.45 calls/min). For call diversity, we found that there was a significant main effect of hen number (F218 = 12.21, p = 0.0004) but no main effect of drake number (F3,18 = 3.04, p = 0.0555). Cluster analyses indicated that certain types of calls were given under specific conditions. There were generally 6 major clusters of vocal repertoires (R-square = 0.899, Cubic Clustering Criterion = 9.30). Our results suggest that Pekin ducks are affected by the types of stimuli and social environment in how much they vocalize and in the properties of the calls they use. In addition, males and females differ somewhat in the repertoire of the calls they use, and in the spectral properties of their calls.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Published Erratum
    [这更正了文章DOI:10.3389/fvets.2024.1366933。].
    [This corrects the article DOI: 10.3389/fvets.2024.1366933.].
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    动物行为可以提供有关动物福利的有用信息,但是用于收集行为数据和数据处理的方法和工具会影响结果。因此,这项研究是对奶牛(Bostaurus)行为和与小牛产后早期的相互作用进行的,旨在比较两种采样规则,即以30s的扫描间隔进行连续和瞬时采样,1、2、3、4、5和10分钟,和两种方法来处理看不见的动物。该研究基于三个假设:(1)连续抽样提供了最完整和准确的数据,允许观察很少的行为和短事件;(2)瞬时采样可以提供频率和持续时间的准确测量,特别是在短扫描间隔;(3)行为结果的差异可能发生取决于是否应用了对视线以外的时间的校正。因此,在分娩后2小时的视频中观察到10个水坝。沉思,刻板印象,在观察期间没有记录到咬小腿和对接小腿。仅在连续采样或以30s扫描间隔连续且瞬时的情况下观察到其他行为。当应用较长的扫描间隔时,几种行为的重新编码不太准确。以30s扫描间隔进行连续和瞬时采样的数据与Wilcoxon检验进行比较。结果显示姿势没有显着差异,笔中的位置和所有行为(p>0.05),除了发声(p=0.003)。由于处理两种采样规则的视线外的方法,相同的测试没有突出显着差异(p>0.05)。连续采样和瞬时采样之间的相关性普遍以30s的间隔很高,并且随着大多数行为的扫描间隔长度的增加而降低。结果证实了前两个假设,表明连续抽样更准确,特别是对于短暂和罕见的行为,并警告以分钟的扫描间隔使用瞬时采样收集的大坝行为数据的适用性。第三个假设没有被这项研究证明。应根据新技术的发展来考虑结果,这些新技术依赖于通过传感器和成像获取的数据来监测母牛的福利和分娩后的行为。
    Animal behavior can provide useful information about animal welfare, but methods and tools used to gather behavioral data and data treatment can influence the results. Therefore, this study was carried out on dairy cow (Bos taurus) behavior and interaction with calves early post-partum aiming at comparing two sampling rules, namely continuous and instantaneous sampling at scan intervals of 30 s, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 10 min, and of two methods to deal with out of sight animals. The study was based on three assumptions: (1) continuous sampling provides the most complete and accurate data, allowing the observation of seldom behaviors and short events; (2) instantaneous sampling can provide accurate measurements of frequency and duration, especially at short scan intervals; (3) differences in behavioral results may occur depending on whether a correction for time out of sight is applied or not. Thus, 10 dams were observed from videos in the 2 h post-parturition. Ruminating, stereotypies, calf-biting and calf-butting were not recorded during the observation period. Other behaviors were observed only with continuous sampling or with continuous and instantaneous at 30-s scan intervals. The recoding of several behaviors was less accurate when applying longer scan intervals. Data from continuous and instantaneous sampling at 30-s scan intervals were compared with Wilcoxon test. Results showed no significant differences for posture, position in the pen and all behaviors (p > 0.05) except vocalizing (p = 0.003). The same test did not highlight significant differences due to method of dealing with out of sight for both sampling rules (p > 0.05). Correlation between continuous and instantaneous sampling were prevalently high at 30-s intervals and they decreased as the length of scan intervals increased for most behaviors. Results confirmed the first two assumptions suggesting that continuous sampling is more accurate, in particular for short and rare behaviors, and caution against the suitability of dam behavioral data collected using instantaneous sampling at scan intervals of minutes. The third assumption was not proven by this study. Results should be considered in light of the development of new technologies that relies on data acquired by sensors and imaging to monitor cow-calf welfare and behavior post-parturition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这篇评论讨论了福利科学家如何检查发声以深入了解单个动物的情感状态。近年来,在专业管理环境中工作的研究人员已经认识到监控类型的价值,rates,和呼叫的声学结构,这可能反映了福利的各个方面。幸运的是,生物声学领域的最新技术进步允许用麦克风记录人声活动,水听器,和动物附着设备(例如,项圈),以及自动呼叫识别。我们考虑如何将声音行为用作情感状态的指标,对情绪的效价特别感兴趣。虽然大多数研究都调查了在负面环境下产生的声乐活动(例如,经历疼痛,社会孤立,环境干扰),我们强调表达积极情感状态的发声。例如,有些物种在觅食时会发声,玩耍,从事美容,或与特定物种有联系地互动。这篇综述概述了非人类哺乳动物情感状态的声音指标的结构有效性的证据。此外,我们讨论了可以用来调查声音行为的非侵入性方法,以及对这一研究领域的潜在限制。在未来,福利科学家应该试图找出可靠的,反映情感效价的有效物种特异性呼叫,这可以通过采用维度方法来实现。维度方法通过比较负面和正面环境中发出的发声来考虑唤醒和效价。最终,可以连续跟踪声学活动,以检测福利状况的变化或评估动物转移的影响,介绍,以及畜牧业常规或环境的变化。我们鼓励福利科学家通过将声音活动与其他行为措施和生理生物标志物相结合来扩展其福利监测工具包。
    This review discusses how welfare scientists can examine vocalizations to gain insight into the affective states of individual animals. In recent years, researchers working in professionally managed settings have recognized the value of monitoring the types, rates, and acoustic structures of calls, which may reflect various aspects of welfare. Fortunately, recent technological advances in the field of bioacoustics allow for vocal activity to be recorded with microphones, hydrophones, and animal-attached devices (e.g., collars), as well as automated call recognition. We consider how vocal behavior can be used as an indicator of affective state, with particular interest in the valence of emotions. While most studies have investigated vocal activity produced in negative contexts (e.g., experiencing pain, social isolation, environmental disturbances), we highlight vocalizations that express positive affective states. For instance, some species produce vocalizations while foraging, playing, engaging in grooming, or interacting affiliatively with conspecifics. This review provides an overview of the evidence that exists for the construct validity of vocal indicators of affective state in non-human mammals. Furthermore, we discuss non-invasive methods that can be utilized to investigate vocal behavior, as well as potential limitations to this line of research. In the future, welfare scientists should attempt to identify reliable, valid species-specific calls that reflect emotional valence, which may be possible by adopting a dimensional approach. The dimensional approach considers both arousal and valence by comparing vocalizations emitted in negative and positive contexts. Ultimately, acoustic activity can be tracked continuously to detect shifts in welfare status or to evaluate the impact of animal transfers, introductions, and changes to the husbandry routine or environment. We encourage welfare scientists to expand their welfare monitoring toolkits by combining vocal activity with other behavioral measures and physiological biomarkers.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    喉癌切除术或严重吞咽困难的防误吸手术后患者失去声音。为了帮助这些病人,我们使用称为语音检索器(VR)的设备开发并验证了一种新颖的发声方法的实用性,其中声源放在嘴里。
    我们调查了VR在患者中的有效性。VR由带有内置扬声器的话筒和用作声源的专用应用程序组成。我们首次比较了使用VR和电喉部(EL)的正常参与者的语音清晰度和自然性,以及使用VR前后的发声障碍患者的语音相关生活质量(V-RQOL)。
    VR产生了显着更高的100音节考试成绩以及流畅性,额外的噪音,语调,首次使用VR和EL用户的清晰度和整体长阅读测试等级。此外,VR的使用显著改善了发声障碍参与者的V-RQOL.
    与EL相比,VR可以在没有使用替代发声方法经验的情况下对参与者进行更有效的语音改善,并改善了发声障碍患者的V-RQOL。
    步骤4.
    UNASSIGNED: Patients lose their voice after laryngectomy for laryngeal cancer or aspiration prevention surgery for severe dysphagia. To assist such patients, we developed and verified the utility of a novel vocalization method using a device termed the voice retriever (VR), in which the sound source is placed in the mouth.
    UNASSIGNED: We investigated the effectiveness of the VR in patients. The VR consists of a mouthpiece with a built-in speaker and a dedicated application that serves as the sound source. We compared the speech intelligibility and naturalness in normal participants using VR and an electrolarynx (EL) for the first time as well as the voice-related quality of life (V-RQOL) in patients with dysphonia before and after using the VR.
    UNASSIGNED: The VR produced significantly higher 100-syllable test scores as well as fluency, amount of additional noise, intonation, intelligibility and overall long reading test ratings in the first-time VR and EL users. Furthermore, the VR use significantly improved the V-RQOL of participants with dysphonia.
    UNASSIGNED: Compared to EL, VR allows more effective speech improvement in participants without experience using an alternative vocalization method and improves the V-RQOL in patients with dysphonia.
    UNASSIGNED: Step 4.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    围绕声音交流的上下文可能会对声音信号的感知方式产生重大影响。5-羟色胺能系统很好地定位为根据上下文调制通信信号的感知,因为血清素能神经元对社会环境有反应,影响社会行为,和神经支配听觉区域。像实验鼠这样的动物可以成为探索血清素如何影响声音感知的主要神经系统的绝佳模型,包括像下丘(IC)这样的中央听觉区域。在IC内,血清素能活性不仅反映了一个特定的存在,也是给定社会互动的效价。为了评估血清素是否可以影响雄性小鼠对声音信号的感知,我们通过注射5-羟色胺的前体5-HTP全身操作,在IC局部输注芬氟拉明,5-羟色胺再摄取阻断剂。然后,小鼠参与了一项行为测定,其中雄性抑制其超声发声(USV),以响应雌性宽带发声(BBV)的回放,用于女性与男性互动时的防御性侵略。相对于对照,5-HTP和芬氟拉明在BBV回放期间增加了对USV的抑制。5-HTP还降低了特定类型USV和男性调查的基线产量,但是两种药物治疗都没有强烈影响男性挖掘或修饰。这些发现表明,血清素会改变小鼠对声音信号的行为反应,部分是通过作用于听觉大脑区域,并建议鼠标发声行为可以作为探索人类交流中上下文机制的有用模型。
    The context surrounding vocal communication can have a strong influence on how vocal signals are perceived. The serotonergic system is well-positioned for modulating the perception of communication signals according to context, because serotonergic neurons are responsive to social context, influence social behavior, and innervate auditory regions. Animals like lab mice can be excellent models for exploring how serotonin affects the primary neural systems involved in vocal perception, including within central auditory regions like the inferior colliculus (IC). Within the IC, serotonergic activity reflects not only the presence of a conspecific, but also the valence of a given social interaction. To assess whether serotonin can influence the perception of vocal signals in male mice, we manipulated serotonin systemically with an injection of its precursor 5-HTP, and locally in the IC with an infusion of fenfluramine, a serotonin reuptake blocker. Mice then participated in a behavioral assay in which males suppress their ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) in response to the playback of female broadband vocalizations (BBVs), used in defensive aggression by females when interacting with males. Both 5-HTP and fenfluramine increased the suppression of USVs during BBV playback relative to controls. 5-HTP additionally decreased the baseline production of a specific type of USV and male investigation, but neither drug treatment strongly affected male digging or grooming. These findings show that serotonin modifies behavioral responses to vocal signals in mice, in part by acting in auditory brain regions, and suggest that mouse vocal behavior can serve as a useful model for exploring the mechanisms of context in human communication.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近的人体临床研究表明,大麻二酚(CBD),大麻的主要非成瘾性成分,具有相当大的治疗潜力。本研究的目的是探讨CBD管理对健康家犬行为影响的可能性。众所周知,当与照顾者暂时分开时,他们可能会表现出发声的增加。这里评估了CBD给药对这种发声活动的影响。十只4至7岁的健康狗经历了两次与看护者的暂时分离,在2周内以2.0mg/kg/天的剂量施用CBD后,在相同的时间内服用与安慰剂相同量的橄榄油后。当行为评估是通过计算狗在与看护者分离之前和分离之后表现出的任何声音活动的总持续时间来进行的,研究发现,所有10只参与狗在单独离开时(分离后)的发声频率高于与照顾者在一起时(分离前)的发声频率。他们是否接受了CBD或安慰剂的管理。在CBD管理之后,然而,与安慰剂给药后相比,这种增加的程度明显减弱(p<0.01)。作为结果的可能解释之一。作者假设CBD对健康家犬的抗焦虑作用,正如在人类中报道的那样。
    Recent human clinical studies indicate that cannabidiol (CBD), the primary non-addictive component of cannabis, possesses considerable therapeutic potentials. The purpose of the present study was to explore the possibility of effects of CBD administration on behaviors of healthy domestic dogs. It is well-known that when temporarily separated from their caregivers, they may show an increase in vocalization. Here the effects of CBD administration upon such vocal activity were assessed. Ten 4- to 7-year-old healthy dogs experienced temporary separation from their caregivers twice, once following the administration of CBD at 2.0 mg/kg/day over a 2-week-period, and once following the administration of the same amount of olive oil as placebo over the same length of period. When the behavioral assessment was conducted by computing the total duration of any vocal activity exhibited by the dogs before the separation from their caregivers and after the separation, it was found that all of the 10 participant dogs vocalized more often when being left alone (after the separation) than when being with their caregivers (before the separation), whether they had received the administration of CBD or placebo. Following the CBD administration, however, the degree of such increase was significantly less robust than that following the placebo administration (p < 0.01). As one of the possible explanations for the results. the author hypothesizes anxiolytic effects of CBD in healthy domestic dogs, as has been reported in humans.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    声音分析是改善精准畜牧业系统的重要研究领域。如果牲畜声音携带的信息被正确解释,它可以用来改善这一领域的管理和福利评估。因此,我们假设母牛的鼻发声可能对特定物种有镇静作用。在我们的研究中,鼻发声是从母牛(不是测试牛群的一部分)舔一天大的小牛时记录的。对原始声音进行了分析,清理干净的噪音,最具代表性的发声被延长到两分钟。从80头匈牙利灰牛中随机选择30头有小牛的母牛。选择两个试验日,相隔一周;两天的天气情况相似。牛群被收集在一个围场里,并且测试地点(带有头锁的约束箱)距离它们21m。牛群中的母牛被轻轻地移到约束板条箱上,and,安装头锁后,将Polar®心率监测器固定在动物上。RR间隔的记录进行两分钟。在测试的第一天,在心率监测过程中,每一秒母牛都会播放经过处理的鼻音。当声音结束时,心率监测器被移除.在测试的第二天,在参与的奶牛之间切换了声音和没有声音的处理。在测量结束时,头锁被打开了,自愿让动物出去,并沿5m的距离进行了飞行测试。通过5m长度所需的时间用秒表测量并除以距离。RR间期用KubiosHRV标准(ver。3.5.0)软件。记录了整个测量的以下数据:平均和最大心率;SD1和SD2;pNN50;VLF,LF,和HF。在声音分析中检测到的准周期信号几乎听不到,即使它被增强到最大。这可以被认为是可能由关节基础引起的振动,比如舌头的振动,例如。SD2/SD1比率(0.97vs.1.07对于没有声音和声音播放的动物,分别,p=0.0110)和飞行速度(0.92vs.1.08s/m对于没有声音和声音播放的动物,分别,p=0.0409)表明声音处理对受约束的母牛具有镇静作用。测试当天不影响任何测量参数;因此,没有观察到常规的效果.声音处理的是-否序列显着降低了pNN50和飞行速度值,表明与头锁和处理鼻音的有效性有更积极的联系。总之,我们已经证明,通过合理的分析,不仅可以收集有关个人和牛群的信息,经过适当的处理,获得的声音可用于改善动物福利。
    Sound analysis is an important field of research for improving precision livestock farming systems. If the information carried by livestock sounds is interpreted correctly, it could be used to improve management and welfare assessment in this field. Therefore, we hypothesized that the nasal vocalization of a mother cow could have a calming effect on conspecifics. The nasal vocalization in our study was recorded from a mother cow (not part of the test herd) while it was licking its day-old calf. The raw sound was analyzed, cleaned from noises, and the most representative vocalization was lengthened to two minutes. Thirty cows having calves were randomly selected from eighty Hungarian grey cattle cows. Two test days were selected, one week apart; the weather circumstances in both days were similar. The herd was collected in a paddock, and the test site (a restraining crate with a headlock) was 21 m away from them. The cows from the herd were gently moved to the restraining crate, and, after the installation of the headlock, Polar® heart rate monitors were fixed on the animals. The recording of the RR intervals was carried out for two minutes. On day one of the test, the processed nasal sound was played to every second cow during the heart rate monitoring. When the sound ended, the heart rate monitor was removed. On test day two, the sound and no sound treatments were switched among the participating cows. At the end of the measurement, the headlock was opened, letting the animals out voluntarily, and a flight test was performed along a 5 m distance. The time needed to pass the 5 m length was measured with a stopwatch and divided by the distance. The RR intervals were analyzed with the Kubios HRV Standard (ver. 3.5.0) software. The following data were recorded for the entire measurement: average and maximum heart rate; SD1 and SD2; pNN50; VLF, LF, and HF. The quasi-periodic signal detected in the sound analyses can hardly be heard, even when it is enhanced to the maximum. This can be considered a vibration probably caused by the basis of articulation, such as a vibration of the tongue, for example. The SD2/SD1 ratio (0.97 vs. 1.07 for the animals having no sound and sound played, respectively, p = 0.0110) and the flight speed (0.92 vs. 1.08 s/m for the animals having no sound and sound played, respectively, p = 0.0409) indicate that the sound treatment had a calming effect on the restrained cows. The day of the test did not influence any of the measured parameters; therefore, no effect of the routine was observed. The yes-no sequence of the sound treatment significantly reduced the pNN50 and flight speed values, suggesting a somewhat more positive association with the headlock and the effectiveness of the processed nasal sound. In conclusion, we have demonstrated that, by means of sound analyses, not only information about individuals and the herd can be gathered but that, with proper processing, the sound obtained can be used to improve animal welfare.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号