Vocalization

发声
  • 文章类型: Case Reports
    使用Blom®气管切开导管(包含袖带)的患者可以在机械通气时发声,可以显著提高患者的生活质量。这是由气管造口管的专用结构带来的,该结构允许通过声门排出呼气。然而,这种特性可能会使患者潮气量的测量复杂化,因为大部分过期不会回到呼吸机。由于需要插入语音套管,充当内部套管,为了让病人发声,空气通道可能变得收缩,从而增加气道阻力。施加适当的呼气末正压(PEEP)和呼吸机自动触发的困难也可能是有问题的。因此,在不调整通气设置的情况下,肺泡通气预计会减少。我们使用Blom®气管造口管的经验揭示了一些问题,并为患者管理提供建议。我们在此报告了在声乐训练期间插入Blom®气管造口管接受机械正压通气的患者的经验。
    Patients with a Blom® tracheostomy tube (containing a cuff) can vocalize while on mechanical ventilation, which can significantly improve the patient\'s quality of life. This is brought by the purpose-built structure of the tracheostomy tube that allows the expiration to be expelled through the glottis. However, this characteristic may complicate the measurement of the patient\'s tidal volume, as most of the expiration does not return to the ventilator. Owing to the necessity of insertion of the speech cannula, which acts as an inner cannula, to enable patient vocalization, the air passage likely becomes constricted, thus increasing airway resistance. Difficulty in applying appropriate positive end-expiratory pressure (PEEP) and ventilator auto-triggering may also be problematic. Therefore, alveolar ventilation is predicted to decrease without adjusting the ventilation settings. Our experience using the Blom® tracheostomy tube revealed some problems, and we provide suggestions for patient management. We herein report on the experience of a patient having inserted the Blom® tracheostomy tube receiving mechanical positive pressure ventilation during vocal training.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    The study investigated the effect of farrowing environment on the weaning adaptability of sows and piglets. One farrowing crate (FC) and two farrowing pens with temporary crating (TC: SWAP and JLF15) were compared. Sixty-four sows and 663 piglets were followed until 5 days post-weaning. At weaning (D24), sows and piglets were moved to group pens and nursery pens, respectively. Sows and piglets’ behaviors were observed on D24, D25, and D26. On D23, D25, and D26, piglets’ skin lesions were counted, and sows and piglets’ saliva samples were collected for stress biomarkers (cortisol and chromogranin A, CgA). Piglets were weighed on D23 and D29. All the piglets’ skin lesions increased on D25 and decreased on D26 (p < 0.05). Compared to D23, cortisol of JLF15 and CgA of FC piglets increased, whereas those of SWAP piglets remained similar after weaning (p < 0.05). Post-weaning performance in piglets was similar across farrowing systems. SWAP sows vocalized more than FC and JLF15 on D24 and D25 (p < 0.001). Results suggested that SWAP piglets showed a lower weaning stress response. Frequent post-weaning vocalization in SWAP sows might be linked with a negative effect of the abrupt separation from the piglets.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:声音交流模式对物种保护有影响:呼叫行为的变化可以,例如,反映了一个受干扰的栖息地。更重要的是,呼叫率是一个参数,允许保护规划者将呼叫密度转换为动物密度,当使用无源声学监测系统(PAM)检测呼叫时。
    方法:我们调查了伊萨河谷旱季后期黑猩猩(Pantroglodytesschweinfurthii)的呼叫率,坦桑尼亚西部通过进行焦点跟踪。我们研究了影响稀树草原林地黑猩猩呼叫率的社会生态因素。
    结果:我们发现性,在植被类型中花费的时间比例,旅行时间的比例,一天的时间,聚会人数和肿胀的女性存在对通话率有显着影响。在不同的人口统计学类别中,呼叫率有所不同,亚成人和成年男性的发声频率是亚成人和成年女性的两倍,是青少年的三倍。
    结论:使用PAM和最近的统计发展来估计动物密度是有希望的,但依赖于我们知道的个人呼叫率,通常不适用于许多物种。随着自动呼叫检测的改进,我们预计PAM将越来越广泛地应用于灵长类动物,为了保护。
    BACKGROUND: Patterns of vocal communication have implications for species conservation: a change in calling behaviour can, for instance, reflect a disturbed habitat. More importantly, call rate is a parameter that allows conservation planners to convert call density into animal density, when detecting calls with a passive acoustic monitoring system (PAM).
    METHODS: We investigated chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) call rate during the late dry season in the Issa Valley, western Tanzania by conducting focal follows. We examined the socio-ecological factors that influence call production rate of savanna woodland chimpanzees.
    RESULTS: We found that sex, proportion of time spent in a vegetation type, proportion of time spent travelling, time of the day, party size and swollen parous female presence had a significant effect on the call rate. Call rate differed among the different demographic classes with subadult and adult males vocalising twice as often as the subadult and adult females and three times as often as the juveniles.
    CONCLUSIONS: The use of PAM and recent statistical developments to estimate animal density is promising but relies on our knowing individual call rate, often not available for many species. With the improvement in automatic call detection, we anticipate that PAM will increasingly be broadly applied to primates but also across taxa, for conservation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    外侧枕部-颞叶皮层的节外体区(EBA)在相互相互作用中起着重要作用,因为它检测到自我和他人手部动作之间的一致性。然而,目前尚不清楚EBA是否可以检测到同等,无论动作类型如何.在本研究中,我们检查了三种类型动作的同等检测的神经基础:手势,发声,和面部表情。单因素分析揭示了一致性效应,尤其是模仿行动,对于EBA中的所有三种类型的操作。EBA中的多体素模式分析分类器能够区分所有实验中的起始相互作用与对相互作用的响应。相应地,单变量分析显示的EBA中的一致性效应对反应比对起始相互作用更强。这些发现表明,无论使用的身体部位(即面部或手)和动作类型(即手势或声音)如何,EBA都可能有助于检测一致性。此外,启动和响应交互可能在EBA中进行不同的处理。这项研究强调了EBA在比较自我和超越手的行为方面的作用。跑步头:EBA在相互模仿中的作用。
    The extrastriate body area (EBA) in the lateral occipito-temporal cortex has an important role in reciprocal interaction, as it detects congruence between self and other\'s hand actions. However, it is unclear whether the EBA can detect congruence regardless of the type of action. In the present study, we examined the neural substrate underlying congruence detection of three types of actions: hand gestures, vocalizations, and facial expressions. A univariate analysis revealed a congruency effect, especially for imitating action, for all three types of actions in the EBA. A multi-voxel pattern analysis classifier in the EBA was able to distinguish between initiating interaction from responding to interaction in all experiments. Correspondingly, the congruency effect in the EBA revealed by univariate analysis was stronger for responding to than for initiating interaction. These findings suggest that the EBA might contribute to detect congruence regardless of the body part used (i.e. face or hand) and the type of action (i.e. gestural or vocal). Moreover, initiating and responding to interaction might be processed differently within the EBA. This study highlights the role of the EBA in comparing between self and other\'s actions beyond hand actions.Running head: Function of EBA in reciprocal imitation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    非人类动物之间的交流通常表现为僵化和僵化,反映情绪状态,而不是有任何认知基础。用世界上最小的猴子,侏儒猴(Cebuellapygmaea),以猿猴灵长类动物中最小的绝对大脑大小为例,我回顾了认知在侏儒mar猴发声的发展和使用中的作用,并提供了有关babbling和食物相关发声的工具性使用的新数据。侏儒猿猴有几个联系电话,它们在声音定位的心理声学特性以及它们穿过雨林的距离方面都有所不同。Marmosets根据与邻居的距离战略性地使用这些呼叫。当暴露于新的群体和新交配时,Marmosets会改变呼叫结构的光谱和时间方面。它们显示特定于人口的声乐方言。年轻的侏儒mar猴从事父母奖励的广泛的胡言乱语行为,这有助于年轻人发展成人的声音结构,但是年长的猴子在冲突的情况下也会使用喋喋不休的工具。特定的食物参考调用通常与食物偏好有关,但是在有生命的猎物的情况下,食物的叫声被抑制了。未交配的动物系统地将长途电话与食物电话结合在一起,就像在为伴侣做广告一样。一起来看,这些例子表明,即使是小脑灵长类动物也能灵活地、有策略地利用他们的声音信号,以应对各种环境和社会条件。
    Communication among nonhuman animals is often presented as rigid and inflexible, reflecting emotional states rather than having any cognitive basis. Using the world\'s smallest monkey, the pygmy marmoset (Cebuella pygmaea), with the smallest absolute brain size amongst simian primates as a case study, I review the role of cognition in the development and usage of vocalizations in pygmy marmosets and present new data on the instrumental use of babbling and of food associated vocalizations. Pygmy marmosets have several contact calls that differ in the psychoacoustic properties for sound localization as well as the distance at which they carry through the rainforest. Marmosets use these calls strategically based on distance from neighbors. Marmosets alter spectral and temporal aspects of call structure when exposed to new groups and when newly mated. They display population specific vocal dialects. Young pygmy marmosets engage in extensive babbling behavior rewarded by parents that helps the young develop adult vocal structures, but older monkeys also use babbling instrumentally in conflict situations. Specific food referential calls generally relate to food preferences, but food calls are suppressed in the presence of animate prey. Unmated animals systematically combine a long distance call with food calls as though advertising for mates. Taken together, these examples show that even small brained primates use their vocal signals flexibly and strategically in response to a variety of environmental and social conditions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:在手指轻敲听觉起搏信号时感觉运动同步(SMS)性能的稳健特征是轻敲相对于起搏信号的负异步性。Paillard-Fraisse假设表明,负异步是模态间集成的结果,其中大脑比较两种模式(听觉和触觉)的感觉信息。当前的研究将发声和手指敲击的异步性与听觉起搏信号进行了比较。我们的第一个假设是,由于仅在单个(听觉)模态(模态内整合)内需要感觉整合,因此发声与手指敲击相比具有更少的负面异步性。然而,由于发声和手指反应的测量不同,解释这两种反应模式之间的比较是有问题的。为了解决这个问题,我们在同步任务中包含了停止信号。这种操纵的基本原理是,与模态内任务相比,停止信号将在模态间任务中更多地干扰同步。我们假设包含停止信号会引起主动抑制,这减少了负异步。我们进一步假设,与手指敲击相比,发声时的负异步性降低程度较小。
    方法:共有30名参与者参加了这项研究。我们将单感觉模态(发声(或听觉)到听觉起搏信号)中的SMS与双感觉模态(手指(或触觉)到听觉起搏信号)进行了比较。该任务与停止信号任务相结合,其中停止信号在某些块中相关而在其他块中不相关。跨模态和跨两种类型的停止信号块比较了响应对起搏信号的异步和停止信号反应时间。
    结果:在与停止无关的块中,我们发现,与手指敲击(-128.29ms)相比,发声(-61.47ms)与听觉起搏信号更同步.在停车相关的街区,停止信号诱导的主动抑制,稍后更改响应时间。然而,与手指敲击(58.06ms)相比,主动抑制(26.11ms)在发声方面不太明显.
    结论:这些结果支持这样的解释,即手指敲击中相对较大的负异步是模态间集成的结果,而较小的异步与模态内集成有关。这项研究还支持以下解释:与模态间集成相比,模态内集成对同步差异更敏感。
    BACKGROUND: A robust feature of sensorimotor synchronization (SMS) performance in finger tapping to an auditory pacing signal is the negative asynchrony of the tap with respect to the pacing signal. The Paillard-Fraisse hypothesis suggests that negative asynchrony is a result of inter-modal integration, in which the brain compares sensory information across two modalities (auditory and tactile). The current study compared the asynchronies of vocalizations and finger tapping in time to an auditory pacing signal. Our first hypothesis was that vocalizations have less negative asynchrony compared to finger tapping due to the requirement for sensory integration within only a single (auditory) modality (intra-modal integration). However, due to the different measurements for vocalizations and finger responses, interpreting the comparison between these two response modalities is problematic. To address this problem, we included stop signals in the synchronization task. The rationale for this manipulation was that stop signals would perturb synchronization more in the inter-modal compared to the intra-modal task. We hypothesized that the inclusion of stop signals induce proactive inhibition, which reduces negative asynchrony. We further hypothesized that any reduction in negative asynchrony occurs to a lesser degree for vocalization than for finger tapping.
    METHODS: A total of 30 participants took part in this study. We compared SMS in a single sensory modality (vocalizations (or auditory) to auditory pacing signal) to a dual sensory modality (fingers (or tactile) to auditory pacing signal). The task was combined with a stop signal task in which stop signals were relevant in some blocks and irrelevant in others. Response-to-pacing signal asynchronies and stop signal reaction times were compared across modalities and across the two types of stop signal blocks.
    RESULTS: In the blocks where stopping was irrelevant, we found that vocalization (-61.47 ms) was more synchronous with the auditory pacing signal compared to finger tapping (-128.29 ms). In the blocks where stopping was relevant, stop signals induced proactive inhibition, shifting the response times later. However, proactive inhibition (26.11 ms) was less evident for vocalizations compared to finger tapping (58.06 ms).
    CONCLUSIONS: These results support the interpretation that relatively large negative asynchrony in finger tapping is a consequence of inter-modal integration, whereas smaller asynchrony is associated with intra-modal integration. This study also supports the interpretation that intra-modal integration is more sensitive to synchronization discrepancies compared to inter-modal integration.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Many vertebrates communicate with ultrahigh frequency (UHF) vocalizations to limit auditory detection by predators. The mechanisms underlying the neural encoding of such UHF sounds may provide important insights for understanding neural processing of other complex sounds (e.g. human speeches). In the auditory system, sound frequency is normally encoded topographically as tonotopy, which, however, contains very limited representation of UHFs in many species. Instead, electrophysiological studies suggested that two neural mechanisms, both exploiting the interactions between frequencies, may contribute to UHF processing. Neurons can exhibit excitatory or inhibitory responses to a tone when another UHF tone is presented simultaneously (combination sensitivity). They can also respond to such stimulation if they are tuned to the frequency of the cochlear-generated distortion products of the two tones, e.g. their difference frequency (cochlear distortion). Both mechanisms are present in an early station of the auditory pathway, the midbrain inferior colliculus (IC). Currently, it is unclear how prevalent the two mechanisms are and how they are functionally integrated in encoding UHFs. This study investigated these issues with large-view BOLD fMRI in rat auditory system, particularly the IC. UHF vocalizations (above 40kHz), but not pure tones at similar frequencies (45, 55, 65, 75kHz), evoked robust BOLD responses in multiple auditory nuclei, including the IC, reinforcing the sensitivity of the auditory system to UHFs despite limited representation in tonotopy. Furthermore, BOLD responses were detected in the IC when a pair of UHF pure tones was presented simultaneously (45 & 55kHz, 55 & 65kHz, 45 & 65kHz, 45 & 75kHz). For all four pairs, a cluster of voxels in the ventromedial side always showed the strongest responses, displaying combination sensitivity. Meanwhile, voxels in the dorsolateral side that showed strongest secondary responses to each pair of UHF pure tones also showed the strongest responses to a pure tone at their difference frequency, suggesting that they are sensitive to cochlear distortion. These BOLD fMRI results indicated that combination sensitivity and cochlear distortion are employed by large but spatially distinctive neuron populations in the IC to represent UHFs. Our imaging findings provided insights for understanding sound feature encoding in the early stage of the auditory pathway.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    口吃的神经原因仍然未知。一种解释来自神经影像学研究,该研究报告了口吃者大脑中激活的异常偏侧化。然而,这些发现通常是基于长期口吃的成年人的数据,提高了观察到的偏侧化异常是补偿性而不是因果关系的可能性。当前的研究调查了在口吃发作后不久,幼儿语言相关感兴趣区域的大脑活动的偏侧化。我们测试了24名学龄前儿童,其中一半的人有口吃的阳性诊断。所有儿童都参加了图片命名实验,同时通过脑磁图记录了他们的大脑活动。在言语发作之前的时期进行的源分析用于评估三个感兴趣区域中的侧向激活。在两组中,激活明显偏侧到左半球,组间没有差异。这项研究首次表明,在图片命名过程中,可以在幼儿中识别出重要的言语准备大脑激活,并支持以下论点:在口吃者中,脑功能异常偏侧化可能是随着病情变得慢性而发生的神经可塑性适应的结果。
    The neural causes of stuttering remain unknown. One explanation comes from neuroimaging studies that have reported abnormal lateralization of activation in the brains of people who stutter. However, these findings are generally based on data from adults with a long history of stuttering, raising the possibility that the observed lateralization anomalies are compensatory rather than causal. The current study investigated lateralization of brain activity in language-related regions of interest in young children soon after the onset of stuttering. We tested 24 preschool-aged children, half of whom had a positive diagnosis of stuttering. All children participated in a picture-naming experiment whilst their brain activity was recorded by magnetoencephalography. Source analysis performed during an epoch prior to speech onset was used to assess lateralized activation in three regions of interest. Activation was significantly lateralized to the left hemisphere in both groups and not different between groups. This study shows for the first time that significant speech preparatory brain activation can be identified in young children during picture-naming and supports the contention that, in stutterers, aberrant lateralization of brain function may be the result of neuroplastic adaptation that occurs as the condition becomes chronic.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号