Deception

欺骗
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项横断面研究调查了沙特阿拉伯一批护理学生学术作弊背后的原因。该研究涉及利雅得两所政府大学的482名护理专业学生。我们使用了新开发的自我报告问卷,称为作弊原因量表(RCS)来收集数据。研究人群中学术作弊的得分最高的原因包括渴望获得高分,鼓励朋友作弊,以及考试太难的感觉。由于不了解课程材料等原因,男学生的得分明显高于女学生,不清楚的测试问题和说明,来自家庭的压力,课程材料的难度,而忽视有效的学习方法(P<0.05)。年龄也有作用,由于15-20岁的学生在“考试太难”项目中的分数明显较高,而年龄≥25岁的“课程材料难度”得分较高(P<0.05)。此外,由于考试困难等原因,预科学生的分数明显高于其他年份,不清楚的测试问题和说明,害怕失败,课程材料的难度,和取悦家人的愿望(P<0.05)。总的来说,在沙特阿拉伯的护理专业学生中,获得高分的愿望成为学术作弊的主要原因。研究结果表明,社会人口统计学特征,包括性,年龄,和学年,在解决护生作弊问题时应该考虑。
    This cross-sectional study investigated the reasons behind academic cheating in a cohort of nursing students in Saudi Arabia. The study involved 482 nursing students from two government universities in Riyadh. We used a newly developed self-reported questionnaire called the Reasons for Cheating Scale (RCS) to collect data. The highest-scoring reasons for academic cheating in the study population included the desire to obtain high grades, encouragement from friends to cheat, and the perception that exams were too difficult. Male students scored significantly higher than female students for reasons such as not understanding the course material, unclear test questions and instructions, pressure from families to excel, difficulty of the course material, and ignorance of effective study methods (P < 0.05). Age also had a role, as students aged 15-20 years had significantly higher scores for the item \"Exams are too hard\", whereas those aged ≥25 years had higher scores for \"Difficulty of the course material\" (P < 0.05). Additionally, students in the preparatory year had significantly higher scores than those in other years for reasons such as difficult exams, unclear test questions and instructions, fear of failing, difficulty of the course material, and the desire to please their families (P < 0.05). Overall, the desire to obtain high grades emerged as the main reason for academic cheating in our cohort of nursing students in Saudi Arabia. The findings suggest that sociodemographic characteristics, including sex, age, and academic year, should be considered when addressing the issue of cheating among nursing students.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    由于社会交往和认知功能的差异,与神经典型的成年人相比,自闭症成年人可能更难以参与和检测欺骗。因此,自闭症成年人可能会经历紧张的社会关系或面临增加的受害风险。因此,至关重要的是,研究调查导致自闭症成年人在欺骗过程中遇到困难的心理机制,以便为降低风险所需的干预措施提供信息。然而,现有研究探索自闭症欺骗的弱点包括对儿童的重视和对潜在心理机制的有限理论探索。为了解决这些弱点,这篇综述旨在为自闭症成年后的欺骗研究引入一个系统层面的理论框架:BrunswikLens欺骗模型。这里,我们全面介绍了自闭症如何影响所有参与欺骗的过程,包括:选择撒谎(1),产生欺骗线索(2),感知欺骗线索(3),并做出准确性决定(4)。这篇综述还提供了基于证据的,关于自闭症和神经典型成年人的行为在欺骗过程的每个阶段可能有所不同的理论预测和可测试的假设。呼吁组织未来与联合理论观点有关的研究,这将鼓励该领域做出实质性的,从理论上讲,这是对自闭症成年欺骗综合模型发展的动力。此外,在未来的自闭症研究中使用Brunswik欺骗镜头模型可能有助于制定干预措施,以帮助保护自闭症成年人免受操纵和伤害。
    Due to differences in social communication and cognitive functioning, autistic adults may have greater difficulty engaging in and detecting deception compared to neurotypical adults. Consequently, autistic adults may experience strained social relationships or face increased risk of victimization. It is therefore crucial that research investigates the psychological mechanisms that are responsible for autistic adults\' difficulties in the deception process in order to inform interventions required to reduce risk. However, weaknesses of extant research exploring deception in autism include a heavy focus on children and limited theoretical exploration of underlying psychological mechanisms. To address these weaknesses, this review aims to introduce a system-level theoretical framework to the study of deception in autistic adulthood: The Brunswik Lens Model of Deception. Here, we provide a comprehensive account of how autism may influence all processes involved in deception, including: Choosing to Lie (1), Producing Deception Cues (2), Perceiving Deception Cues (3), and Making the Veracity Decision (4). This review also offers evidence-based, theoretical predictions and testable hypotheses concerning how autistic and neurotypical adults\' behavior may differ at each stage in the deception process. The call to organize future research in relation to a joint theoretical perspective will encourage the field to make substantive, theoretically motivated progress toward the development of a comprehensive model of deception in autistic adulthood. Moreover, the utilization of the Brunswik Lens Model of Deception in future autism research may assist in the development of interventions to help protect autistic adults against manipulation and victimization.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    医学领域的虚假信息是一个日益严重的问题,具有很大的风险。因此,有效地发现和打击它至关重要。在这篇文章中,我们提供了三个要素来帮助这场斗争:1)一个新的框架,从验证实体收集健康相关的文章,并促进他们在句子层面的检查价值和事实检查注释;2)使用这个框架生成的语料库,由这两个概念中注释的10335个句子组成,分为327篇文章,我们称之为KEANE(faKenewsAtSENtencelevel);3)一种新的验证假新闻的模型,该模型将医疗领域的特定标识符与三元组主语-谓语-宾语相结合,在句子层面使用变形金刚和前馈神经网络。该模型可以预测句子的事实检查,并评估整篇文章的准确性。在我们的语料库上训练了这个模型后,我们在句子的二元分类(验证性F1:0.749,事实检查F1:0.698)和完整文章的最终分类(F1:0.703)方面取得了显着成果。我们还针对另一个公共数据集测试了它的性能,发现它的性能优于该数据集上评估的大多数系统。此外,我们提供的语料库在句子-文章注释的二重性上与其他现有语料库不同,它可以为模型所做的真实或不真实的预测提供额外的合理性。
    Disinformation in the medical field is a growing problem that carries a significant risk. Therefore, it is crucial to detect and combat it effectively. In this article, we provide three elements to aid in this fight: 1) a new framework that collects health-related articles from verification entities and facilitates their check-worthiness and fact-checking annotation at the sentence level; 2) a corpus generated using this framework, composed of 10335 sentences annotated in these two concepts and grouped into 327 articles, which we call KEANE (faKe nEws At seNtence lEvel); and 3) a new model for verifying fake news that combines specific identifiers of the medical domain with triplets subject-predicate-object, using Transformers and feedforward neural networks at the sentence level. This model predicts the fact-checking of sentences and evaluates the veracity of the entire article. After training this model on our corpus, we achieved remarkable results in the binary classification of sentences (check-worthiness F1: 0.749, fact-checking F1: 0.698) and in the final classification of complete articles (F1: 0.703). We also tested its performance against another public dataset and found that it performed better than most systems evaluated on that dataset. Moreover, the corpus we provide differs from other existing corpora in its duality of sentence-article annotation, which can provide an additional level of justification of the prediction of truth or untruth made by the model.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:当演员试图伪造时,就会发生欺骗性的动作,隐藏或延迟有关其真实运动结果的运动学信息。欺骗性运动的目的是削弱对手(“观察者”)的感知,以获得对他们的优势。我们认为,缺乏概念上的明确性导致人们对欺骗是什么以及理解演员可以欺骗对手的不同方法感到困惑。本文的目的是概述一个概念框架,用于理解体育运动中的欺骗性运动。
    方法:从交际领域采用人际欺骗理论,我们将欺骗定义为当演员故意改变他们的行为,试图削弱观察者预测他们真实行为结果的能力。Further,欺骗可以通过我们定义的欺骗来实现,提供虚假信息的行为,或者伪装,隐瞒行动结果的行为。熟练的运动员往往会有难以预料的动作,但是,只有当演员有明确的意图欺骗观察者时,行为才被归类为包含欺骗。在概述了概念框架之后,然后,我们回顾现有的经验发现,熟练的感知欺骗运动考虑框架。这种方法包括对已知的机制进行严格评估,以促进防止被欺骗的感知能力,包括视觉搜索策略的考虑,信心,视觉和运动体验的贡献,以及反应偏差和行动能力对知觉表现的影响。
    结论:欺骗和伪装之间的区别特别有助于表明大多数研究都检查了欺骗,鲜为人知的是演员如何更有效地掩饰自己的行为,或者观察者如何提高他们预测伪装行为结果的能力。这些见解有助于确定未来研究的富有成果的领域,并概述对技能获取和绩效提高的影响。
    BACKGROUND: Deceptive movements occur when an actor seeks to fake, hide or delay kinematic information about their true movement outcomes. The purpose of deceptive movements is to impair the perception of opponents (the \'observer\') to gain an advantage over them. We argue though that a lack of conceptual clarity has led to confusion about what deception is and in understanding the different approaches by which an actor can deceive their opponent. The aim of this article is to outline a conceptual framework for understanding deceptive movements in sport.
    METHODS: Adopting Interpersonal Deception Theory from the field of communication, we define deception as when an actor deliberately alters their actions in an attempt to impair the ability of an observer to anticipate their true action outcomes. Further, deception can be achieved either by what we define as deceit, the act of providing false information, or disguise, the act of concealing the action outcome. Skilled athletes often have actions that are difficult to anticipate, but an action is only classified as containing deception if the actor has explicit intent to deceive an observer. Having outlined the conceptual framework, we then review existing empirical findings on the skilled perception of deceptive movements considering the framework. This approach includes a critical evaluation of the mechanisms known to facilitate the perceptual ability to prevent being deceived, including a consideration of visual search strategies, confidence, the contribution of visual and motor experiences, and the influence of response biases and action capabilities on perceptual performance.
    CONCLUSIONS: The distinction between deceit and disguise particularly helps to show that most research has examined deceit, with little known about how an actor can more effectively disguise their action, or about how an observer can improve their ability to anticipate the outcome of disguised actions. The insights help to identify fruitful areas for future research and outline implications for skill acquisition and performance enhancement.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    跨情境的不一致在诚实特征的表达中很常见;然而,在多个背景下,对行为不诚实的重视不够。当前的研究旨在调查各种情况下的行为不诚实,并揭示特质诚实之间的关联,行为不诚实,和观察他人的神经模式在视频中诚实或不诚实地表现(缩写。:(dis)诚实视频观看)。首先,结果表明,使用特质诚实来反映不诚实行为的变化并预测行为不诚实的局限性。这一发现强调了在理解和预测不诚实行为时考虑神经模式的重要性。第二,通过比较三种神经网络中七种类型数据的预测性能,结果表明,在(dis)诚实视频观看期间,假设驱动的网络中的功能连通性在预测多任务行为不诚实方面提供了最高的预测能力。最后,通过应用特征消除方法,中线自我参照区(内侧前额叶皮层,后扣带皮质,和前扣带皮质),前岛,纹状体被认为是预测行为不诚实的信息最多的大脑区域。总之,这项研究提供了对欺骗的个体差异和特质诚实之间错综复杂的联系的见解,行为不诚实,和神经模式在(不)诚实的视频观看。
    Cross-situational inconsistency is common in the expression of honesty traits; yet, there is insufficient emphasis on behavioral dishonesty across multiple contexts. The current study aimed to investigate behavioral dishonesty in various contexts and reveal the associations between trait honesty, behavioral dishonesty, and neural patterns of observing others behave honestly or dishonestly in videos (abbr.: (dis)honesty video-watching). First, the results revealed limitations in using trait honesty to reflect variations in dishonest behaviors and predict behavioral dishonesty. The finding highlights the importance of considering neural patterns in understanding and predicting dishonest behaviors. Second, by comparing the predictive performance of seven types of data across three neural networks, the results showed that functional connectivity in the hypothesis-driven network during (dis)honesty video-watching provided the highest predictive power in predicting multitask behavioral dishonesty. Last, by applying the feature elimination method, the midline self-referential regions (medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex), anterior insula, and striatum were identified as the most informative brain regions in predicting behavioral dishonesty. In summary, the study offered insights into individual differences in deception and the intricate connections among trait honesty, behavioral dishonesty, and neural patterns during (dis)honesty video-watching.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    欺骗行为正在全球护理专业学生中蔓延,需要开发经过验证的问卷,以评估此类行为的原因。来自沙特阿拉伯2所大学的护理学生(N=482)参加了这项观察性研究。受访者完成了一项包含社会人口统计学项目和33项作弊原因量表(RCS)的调查。RCS具有1因素结构;模型拟合指数在1-,2-,和三因素模型,但对于2因子和3因子模型,因素间相关性过高。因子得分估计质量的衡量标准如下:因子确定性指数,0.987;预期后验边际可靠性,0.974;灵敏度比,6.178;以及预期的真实差异百分比,97.3%。总体RCS与单维的紧密度度量如下:单维一致性,0.957;解释共同方差,0.875;和平均项目剩余绝对载荷,0.223.组内相关系数和麦当劳的欧米茄分别为0.96(CI:0.93-0.98)和0.962(95%CI:0.958-0.967),分别。严重性评分,init,和装备的范围分别为-0.847至-2.015、0.813至1.742和0.837至1.661。对于所有RCS项目,阈值排序为τi1<τi2<τi3<τi4,并显示出两性之间的不变性。RCS对经典和项目反应理论参数均显示出强大的心理有效性。它还具有出色的重测可靠性,内部一致性,项目歧视,阶乘有效性,测量不变性,以及响应的有序阈值水平。因此,RCS是评估护生作弊行为的有效和可靠的工具。
    Cheating behavior is spreading among nursing students worldwide, necessitating the development of a validated questionnaire evaluating the reasons for such behavior. Nursing students (N = 482) from 2 universities in Saudi Arabia participated in this observational study. A survey containing items on socio-demographics and the 33-item Reasons for Cheating Scale (RCS) was completed by the respondents. The RCS had a 1-factor structure; the model fit indices were similar between the 1-, 2-, and 3-factor models, but the inter-factor correlations were too high for the 2- and 3-factor models. The measures of the quality of the factor score estimates were as follows: factor determinacy index, 0.987; expected a posteriori marginal reliability, 0.974; sensitivity ratio, 6.178; and expected percentage of true differences, 97.3%. The measures of the closeness to unidimensionality for the overall RCS were as follows: unidimensional congruence, 0.957; explained common variance, 0.875; and mean item residual absolute loading, 0.223. The intraclass correlation coefficient and McDonald\'s omega were 0.96 (CI: 0.93-0.98) and 0.962 (95% CI: 0.958-0.967), respectively. The severity score, infit, and outfit ranged from -0.847 to -2.015, 0.813 to 1.742, and 0.837 to 1.661, respectively. For all RCS items, the thresholds ranked τi1 < τi2 < τi3 < τi4 and showed invariance between the sexes. The RCS showed robust psychometric validity for both classical and item response theory parameters. It also had excellent test-retest reliability, internal consistency, item discrimination, factorial validity, measurement invariance, and ordered threshold level for the responses. Therefore, the RCS is a valid and reliable tool for assessing cheating behavior among nursing students.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    大型语言模型(LLM)目前处于将AI系统与人类交流和日常生活交织在一起的最前沿。因此,使它们与人类价值观保持一致非常重要。然而,鉴于推理能力的稳步提高,未来的LLM被怀疑能够欺骗人类操作员并利用这种能力来绕过监控工作。作为先决条件,LLM需要对欺骗策略有概念性的理解。这项研究表明,这种策略出现在最先进的LLM中,ButwerenonexistenceinearlyLLM.Weconductaseriesofexperimentsshowingthatstate-of-the-artLLMareabletounderstandandinducefalsebelieveinotheragents,他们在复杂的欺骗场景中的表现可以利用思维链推理来放大,在LLM中引发马基雅维利主义会引发错位的欺骗行为。例如,GPT-4,在简单的测试场景中表现出99.16%的欺骗行为(P<0.001)。在复杂的二阶欺骗测试场景中,目的是误导期望被欺骗的人,当用思维链推理增强时,GPT-4在71.46%的时间内诉诸欺骗行为(P<0.001)。总之,揭示LLM中迄今为止未知的机器行为,我们的研究有助于机器心理学的新兴领域。
    Large language models (LLMs) are currently at the forefront of intertwining AI systems with human communication and everyday life. Thus, aligning them with human values is of great importance. However, given the steady increase in reasoning abilities, future LLMs are under suspicion of becoming able to deceive human operators and utilizing this ability to bypass monitoring efforts. As a prerequisite to this, LLMs need to possess a conceptual understanding of deception strategies. This study reveals that such strategies emerged in state-of-the-art LLMs, but were nonexistent in earlier LLMs. We conduct a series of experiments showing that state-of-the-art LLMs are able to understand and induce false beliefs in other agents, that their performance in complex deception scenarios can be amplified utilizing chain-of-thought reasoning, and that eliciting Machiavellianism in LLMs can trigger misaligned deceptive behavior. GPT-4, for instance, exhibits deceptive behavior in simple test scenarios 99.16% of the time (P < 0.001). In complex second-order deception test scenarios where the aim is to mislead someone who expects to be deceived, GPT-4 resorts to deceptive behavior 71.46% of the time (P < 0.001) when augmented with chain-of-thought reasoning. In sum, revealing hitherto unknown machine behavior in LLMs, our study contributes to the nascent field of machine psychology.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在篮球中,进攻球员经常在向一侧传球的同时向另一侧传球。这头假在观察对手中引发冲突,因为头部取向的加工会干扰通过方向的加工。因此,对带有头部假货的通行证的响应比对没有头部假货的通行证的响应慢,并且导致更多的错误(头部假货效应)。头假效应和结构相似的干扰效应(例如,Stroop效应)由相互冲突的试验频率调节。以前的研究大多采用比例一致性的分块操作。然而,在篮球(以及其他团队运动)中,在那里可以遇到不同的个人对手,采取个人频率(例如,20%vs.80%)的这些对手被考虑在内。因此,本研究调查了快速(即,在逐个试验的基础上)将反应行为重新配置为不同比例的不一致试验,取决于不同的篮球运动员。结果指出,参与者确实适应了不同篮球运动员的假频率,这可能是战略适应过程的结果。多层次分析,然而,表明玩家特定的对假频率的适应的很大一部分是由情节检索过程来解释的,表明特定项目的比例一致性效应可以用刺激-反应结合和检索来解释:头部方向(例如,在当前刺激的右侧)检索具有相同头部方向的最后一集,包括作为最后一集的一部分的响应。因此,从理论的角度来看,如果他/他重复相同的头部运动但改变了传球的方向,那么进攻球员将对对手产生最强的不利影响。是否有可能在实践中战略性地应用这一建议仍然需要回答。
    In basketball, an attacking player often plays a pass to one side while looking to the other side. This head fake provokes a conflict in the observing opponent, as the processing of the head orientation interferes with the processing of the pass direction. Accordingly, responses to passes with head fakes are slower and result in more errors than responses to passes without head fakes (head-fake effect). The head-fake effect and structurally similar interference effects (e.g., Stroop effect) are modulated by the frequency of conflicting trials. Previous studies mostly applied a block-wise manipulation of proportion congruency. However, in basketball (and also in other team sports), where different individual opponents can be encountered, it might be important to take the individual frequency (e.g., 20% vs. 80%) of these opponents into account. Therefore, the present study investigates the possibility to quickly (i.e., on a trial-by-trial basis) reconfigure the response behavior to different proportions of incongruent trials, which are contingent on different basketball players. Results point out that participants indeed adapted to the fake-frequency of different basketball players, which could be the result of strategic adaptation processes. Multi-level analyses, however, indicate that a substantial portion of the player-specific adaptation to fake frequencies is accounted by episodic retrieval processes, suggesting that item-specific proportion congruency effects can be explained in terms of stimulus-response binding and retrieval: The head orientation (e.g., to the right) of a current stimulus retrieves the last episode with the same head orientation including the response that was part of this last episode. Thus, from a theoretical perspective, an attacking player would provoke the strongest detrimental effect on an opponent if s/he repeats the same head movement but changes the direction of the pass. Whether it is at all possible to strategically apply this recommendation in practice needs still to be answered.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在这项研究中,我们提出了一种方法,通过将响应延迟和错误分析与意外问题技术相结合,在调查性访谈中检测欺骗。60名参与者被分配到诚实组(n=30)或欺骗性组(n=30)。欺骗性团体被指示记住虚构身份的虚假传记细节。在整个采访中,参与者被提供了随机对照序列,预期,以及关于身份的意想不到的开放式问题。对反应进行音频记录以进行详细检查。我们的发现表明,欺骗性参与者在回答预期(需要欺骗)和意外问题(不可能有预谋的欺骗)时表现出明显更长的等待时间和更高的错误率。在回答控制问题时尝试欺骗的参与者中也观察到了更长的反应延迟(这需要真实的答案)。此外,受试者内部分析强调,与回答控制和预期问题相比,回答意外问题会严重损害个人的表现。利用机器学习算法,我们的方法在区分欺骗性和诚实的参与者方面获得了98%的分类准确率。此外,对单一应答水平进行了分类分析.我们的发现强调了将响应延迟指标和错误率与意外询问合并为调查性访谈中身份欺骗检测的可靠方法的有效性。我们还讨论了加强面试策略的重要意义。
    In this study, we propose an approach to detect deception during investigative interviews by integrating response latency and error analysis with the unexpected question technique. Sixty participants were assigned to an honest (n = 30) or deceptive group (n = 30). The deceptive group was instructed to memorize the false biographical details of a fictitious identity. Throughout the interviews, participants were presented with a randomized sequence of control, expected, and unexpected open-ended questions about identity. Responses were audio recorded for detailed examination. Our findings indicate that deceptive participants showed markedly longer latencies and higher error rates when answering expected (requiring deception) and unexpected questions (for which premeditated deception was not possible). Longer response latencies were also observed in participants attempting deception when answering control questions (which necessitated truthful answers). Moreover, a within-subject analysis highlighted that responding to unexpected questions significantly impaired individuals\' performance compared to answering control and expected questions. Leveraging machine-learning algorithms, our approach attained a classification accuracy of 98% in distinguishing deceptive and honest participants. Additionally, a classification analysis on single response levels was conducted. Our findings underscore the effectiveness of merging response latency metrics and error rates with unexpected questioning as a robust method for identity deception detection in investigative interviews. We also discuss significant implications for enhancing interview strategies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对欺骗行为的脑机制进行了比较充分的研究,并建立了涉及其处理的关键大脑区域。同时,准备欺骗过程的大脑机制鲜为人知。
    我们研究了在电脑游戏过程中对手对先前欺骗性或诚实行为的反馈呈现过程中BOLD信号的变化。游戏的目的是通过欺骗或说实话来误导对手。
    因此,研究表明,以前被证明参与欺骗执行的几个大脑区域,比如左前扣带回皮质和前岛,也是与欺骗准备相关的过程的基础。
    获得的结果还使我们能够建议负责性能监测的大脑区域,意向评估,抑制非选择解决方案,奖励处理可以参与塑造未来的行动选择和欺骗准备。通过揭示欺骗背后的大脑机制,我们的研究有助于更深入地理解这一复杂的认知过程。此外,它强调了在决策的各个阶段探索支配欺骗和真理之间选择的大脑机制的重要性。
    UNASSIGNED: The brain mechanisms of deceptive behavior are relatively well studied, and the key brain regions involved in its processing were established. At the same time, the brain mechanisms underlying the processes of preparation for deception are less known.
    UNASSIGNED: We studied BOLD-signal changes during the presentation of the opponent\'s feedback to a previous deceptive or honest action during the computer game. The goal of the game was to mislead the opponent either by means of deception or by means of telling the truth.
    UNASSIGNED: As a result, it was shown that several brain regions that were previously demonstrated as involved in deception execution, such as the left anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula, also underlie processes related to deception preparation.
    UNASSIGNED: The results obtained also allowed us to suggest that brain regions responsible for performance monitoring, intention assessment, suppression of non-selected solutions, and reward processing could be involved in shaping future action selection and preparation for deception. By shedding light on the brain mechanisms underlying deception, our study contributes to a deeper understanding of this complex cognitive process. Furthermore, it emphasizes the significance of exploring brain mechanisms governing the choice between deception and truth at various stages of decision-making.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号