Deception

欺骗
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    欺骗是儿童道德发展的重要组成部分。以前的发育研究表明,儿童在3岁时开始欺骗,随着年龄增长到5岁,几乎所有的孩子都能够欺骗自己的利益。尽管行为研究表明,欺骗的出现和发展与认知能力有关,他们的神经相关性仍然知之甚少。因此,本研究检查了学龄前儿童潜在的欺骗行为的神经相关性(N=89,44%的男孩,年龄3.13至5.96岁,汉族)使用功能近红外光谱法。修改后的捉迷藏范式被用来引发欺骗和讲真话的行为。结果表明,在3岁儿童的竞争性游戏中,双侧背外侧前额叶皮层的激活与欺骗对手的倾向呈正相关。此外,3岁的孩子表现出很高的欺骗倾向,在额极区域表现出与5岁的孩子在欺骗时相同的大脑激活,而在从未参与欺骗的3岁儿童中没有发现这种效果。这些发现强调了学龄前儿童欺骗与前额叶皮质功能之间的联系。
    Deception is an essential part of children\'s moral development. Previous developmental studies have shown that children start to deceive at the age of 3 years, and as age increased to 5 years, almost all children were able to deceive for their own benefit. Although behavioral studies have indicated that the emergence and development of deception are related to cognitive abilities, their neural correlates remain poorly understood. Therefore, the present study examined the neural correlates underlying deception in preschool-aged children (N = 89, 44% boys, age 3.13 to 5.96 years, Han Chinese) using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. A modified hide-and-seek paradigm was applied to elicit deceptive and truth-telling behaviors. The results showed that activation of bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was positively associated with the tendency to deceive an opponent in a competitive game in the 3-year-olds. In addition, 3-year-olds who showed a high tendency to deceive showed the same brain activation in the frontopolar area as 5-year-olds did when engaged in deception, whereas no such effect was found in 3-year-olds who never engaged in deception. These findings underscore the link between preschoolers\' deception and prefrontal cortex function.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项横断面研究调查了沙特阿拉伯一批护理学生学术作弊背后的原因。该研究涉及利雅得两所政府大学的482名护理专业学生。我们使用了新开发的自我报告问卷,称为作弊原因量表(RCS)来收集数据。研究人群中学术作弊的得分最高的原因包括渴望获得高分,鼓励朋友作弊,以及考试太难的感觉。由于不了解课程材料等原因,男学生的得分明显高于女学生,不清楚的测试问题和说明,来自家庭的压力,课程材料的难度,而忽视有效的学习方法(P<0.05)。年龄也有作用,由于15-20岁的学生在“考试太难”项目中的分数明显较高,而年龄≥25岁的“课程材料难度”得分较高(P<0.05)。此外,由于考试困难等原因,预科学生的分数明显高于其他年份,不清楚的测试问题和说明,害怕失败,课程材料的难度,和取悦家人的愿望(P<0.05)。总的来说,在沙特阿拉伯的护理专业学生中,获得高分的愿望成为学术作弊的主要原因。研究结果表明,社会人口统计学特征,包括性,年龄,和学年,在解决护生作弊问题时应该考虑。
    This cross-sectional study investigated the reasons behind academic cheating in a cohort of nursing students in Saudi Arabia. The study involved 482 nursing students from two government universities in Riyadh. We used a newly developed self-reported questionnaire called the Reasons for Cheating Scale (RCS) to collect data. The highest-scoring reasons for academic cheating in the study population included the desire to obtain high grades, encouragement from friends to cheat, and the perception that exams were too difficult. Male students scored significantly higher than female students for reasons such as not understanding the course material, unclear test questions and instructions, pressure from families to excel, difficulty of the course material, and ignorance of effective study methods (P < 0.05). Age also had a role, as students aged 15-20 years had significantly higher scores for the item \"Exams are too hard\", whereas those aged ≥25 years had higher scores for \"Difficulty of the course material\" (P < 0.05). Additionally, students in the preparatory year had significantly higher scores than those in other years for reasons such as difficult exams, unclear test questions and instructions, fear of failing, difficulty of the course material, and the desire to please their families (P < 0.05). Overall, the desire to obtain high grades emerged as the main reason for academic cheating in our cohort of nursing students in Saudi Arabia. The findings suggest that sociodemographic characteristics, including sex, age, and academic year, should be considered when addressing the issue of cheating among nursing students.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    儿童的高级心理理论(AToM)与他们的亲社会谎言同时相关。然而,AToM和亲社会撒谎之间的因果关系尚未得到证实。为了解决这个差距,本研究采用了训练范式,并调查了AToM在儿童中年时期的亲社会撒谎中的作用。招募了总共66名9岁和10岁的儿童,他们在基线时没有表现出失望礼物范式中的任何亲社会谎言,并随机分配到实验组(n=32)或主动对照组(n=34)。实验组进行了基于对话的四个课程的培训计划。结果显示,与对照组儿童相比,实验组儿童在后测中的AToM增加明显更大,控制家庭社会经济地位,儿童识字得分,工作记忆,和抑制。更重要的是,实验组的孩子比对照组的孩子更容易说出亲社会的谎言,即使在控制了预测试AToM和其他协变量之后。然而,训练效果在训练结束后的6个月随访测试中消失。这些发现为AToM在中年儿童亲社会说谎发展中的因果作用提供了第一个证据。在教育干预的背景下讨论了淡出效应。
    Children\'s advanced theory of mind (AToM) is concurrently associated with their prosocial lie-telling. However, the causal link between AToM and prosocial lie-telling has not yet been demonstrated. To address this gap, the current study adopted a training paradigm and investigated the role of AToM in children\'s prosocial lie-telling in middle childhood. A total of 66 9- and 10-year-old children who did not demonstrate any prosocial lie-telling in a disappointment gift paradigm at the baseline were recruited and randomly assigned to either the experimental group (n = 32) or an active control group (n = 34). The experimental group underwent a conversation-based training program of four sessions. The results showed significantly greater gains in AToM at the posttest for the experimental group children compared with the control group children, controlling for family socioeconomic status, children\'s literacy score, working memory, and inhibition. More important, the experimental group children were more likely to tell prosocial lies than the control group, even after controlling for the pretest AToM and other covariates. However, the training effects faded at the 6-month follow-up test after the training\'s completion. These findings provide the first evidence for the causal role of AToM in the development of prosocial lie-telling in middle childhood. The fade-out effect is discussed in the context of educational interventions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    欺骗行为正在全球护理专业学生中蔓延,需要开发经过验证的问卷,以评估此类行为的原因。来自沙特阿拉伯2所大学的护理学生(N=482)参加了这项观察性研究。受访者完成了一项包含社会人口统计学项目和33项作弊原因量表(RCS)的调查。RCS具有1因素结构;模型拟合指数在1-,2-,和三因素模型,但对于2因子和3因子模型,因素间相关性过高。因子得分估计质量的衡量标准如下:因子确定性指数,0.987;预期后验边际可靠性,0.974;灵敏度比,6.178;以及预期的真实差异百分比,97.3%。总体RCS与单维的紧密度度量如下:单维一致性,0.957;解释共同方差,0.875;和平均项目剩余绝对载荷,0.223.组内相关系数和麦当劳的欧米茄分别为0.96(CI:0.93-0.98)和0.962(95%CI:0.958-0.967),分别。严重性评分,init,和装备的范围分别为-0.847至-2.015、0.813至1.742和0.837至1.661。对于所有RCS项目,阈值排序为τi1<τi2<τi3<τi4,并显示出两性之间的不变性。RCS对经典和项目反应理论参数均显示出强大的心理有效性。它还具有出色的重测可靠性,内部一致性,项目歧视,阶乘有效性,测量不变性,以及响应的有序阈值水平。因此,RCS是评估护生作弊行为的有效和可靠的工具。
    Cheating behavior is spreading among nursing students worldwide, necessitating the development of a validated questionnaire evaluating the reasons for such behavior. Nursing students (N = 482) from 2 universities in Saudi Arabia participated in this observational study. A survey containing items on socio-demographics and the 33-item Reasons for Cheating Scale (RCS) was completed by the respondents. The RCS had a 1-factor structure; the model fit indices were similar between the 1-, 2-, and 3-factor models, but the inter-factor correlations were too high for the 2- and 3-factor models. The measures of the quality of the factor score estimates were as follows: factor determinacy index, 0.987; expected a posteriori marginal reliability, 0.974; sensitivity ratio, 6.178; and expected percentage of true differences, 97.3%. The measures of the closeness to unidimensionality for the overall RCS were as follows: unidimensional congruence, 0.957; explained common variance, 0.875; and mean item residual absolute loading, 0.223. The intraclass correlation coefficient and McDonald\'s omega were 0.96 (CI: 0.93-0.98) and 0.962 (95% CI: 0.958-0.967), respectively. The severity score, infit, and outfit ranged from -0.847 to -2.015, 0.813 to 1.742, and 0.837 to 1.661, respectively. For all RCS items, the thresholds ranked τi1 < τi2 < τi3 < τi4 and showed invariance between the sexes. The RCS showed robust psychometric validity for both classical and item response theory parameters. It also had excellent test-retest reliability, internal consistency, item discrimination, factorial validity, measurement invariance, and ordered threshold level for the responses. Therefore, the RCS is a valid and reliable tool for assessing cheating behavior among nursing students.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对于参与者被欺骗的研究研究,通常建议或要求提供诸如汇报之类的道德保障。然而,关于这些保障措施的现有指南似乎不够连贯和准确,这可能与它们在实践中的次优实现有关。这项研究旨在通过对24名具有丰富欺骗经验的研究人员进行半结构化访谈,为欺骗性研究中道德保障的更连贯和精确的框架做出贡献。受访者讨论了他们实施了哪些道德保障措施以及如何实施,以及它们与真实性概念的关系(即,真实信息的有意沟通)。此外,受访者提供了支持和反对实施这些保障措施的各种理由,以及这些原因如何随着研究的特定背景而变化。总的来说,当前的研究有助于对欺骗性研究中的道德保障有更连贯和准确的理解,这可能有助于指导研究人员和道德审稿人进行道德决策,尽管某些不精确和不连贯的方面仍需要进一步调查和规范反思。
    Ethical safeguards such as debriefing are often recommended or required for research studies in which participants are deceived. However, existing guidance on these safeguards seems insufficiently coherent and precise, which may be associated with their suboptimal implementation in practice. This study aimed to contribute to a more coherent and precise framework of ethical safeguards in deceptive studies through semi-structured interviews with a diverse sample of 24 researchers who had significant experience with deception. Interviewees discussed which ethical safeguards they implemented and how, as well as their relation to the notion of truthfulness (i.e., the intentional communication of true information). Moreover, interviewees provided a variety of reasons for and against implementing these safeguards, as well as how these reasons varied with the particular context of a study. Overall, the current study contributes to a more coherent and precise understanding of ethical safeguards in deceptive research that could be useful for guiding researchers and ethics reviewers in their ethical decision-making, although certain imprecisions and incoherent aspects remain in need of further investigation and normative reflection.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在篮球中,进攻球员经常在向一侧传球的同时向另一侧传球。这头假在观察对手中引发冲突,因为头部取向的加工会干扰通过方向的加工。因此,对带有头部假货的通行证的响应比对没有头部假货的通行证的响应慢,并且导致更多的错误(头部假货效应)。头假效应和结构相似的干扰效应(例如,Stroop效应)由相互冲突的试验频率调节。以前的研究大多采用比例一致性的分块操作。然而,在篮球(以及其他团队运动)中,在那里可以遇到不同的个人对手,采取个人频率(例如,20%vs.80%)的这些对手被考虑在内。因此,本研究调查了快速(即,在逐个试验的基础上)将反应行为重新配置为不同比例的不一致试验,取决于不同的篮球运动员。结果指出,参与者确实适应了不同篮球运动员的假频率,这可能是战略适应过程的结果。多层次分析,然而,表明玩家特定的对假频率的适应的很大一部分是由情节检索过程来解释的,表明特定项目的比例一致性效应可以用刺激-反应结合和检索来解释:头部方向(例如,在当前刺激的右侧)检索具有相同头部方向的最后一集,包括作为最后一集的一部分的响应。因此,从理论的角度来看,如果他/他重复相同的头部运动但改变了传球的方向,那么进攻球员将对对手产生最强的不利影响。是否有可能在实践中战略性地应用这一建议仍然需要回答。
    In basketball, an attacking player often plays a pass to one side while looking to the other side. This head fake provokes a conflict in the observing opponent, as the processing of the head orientation interferes with the processing of the pass direction. Accordingly, responses to passes with head fakes are slower and result in more errors than responses to passes without head fakes (head-fake effect). The head-fake effect and structurally similar interference effects (e.g., Stroop effect) are modulated by the frequency of conflicting trials. Previous studies mostly applied a block-wise manipulation of proportion congruency. However, in basketball (and also in other team sports), where different individual opponents can be encountered, it might be important to take the individual frequency (e.g., 20% vs. 80%) of these opponents into account. Therefore, the present study investigates the possibility to quickly (i.e., on a trial-by-trial basis) reconfigure the response behavior to different proportions of incongruent trials, which are contingent on different basketball players. Results point out that participants indeed adapted to the fake-frequency of different basketball players, which could be the result of strategic adaptation processes. Multi-level analyses, however, indicate that a substantial portion of the player-specific adaptation to fake frequencies is accounted by episodic retrieval processes, suggesting that item-specific proportion congruency effects can be explained in terms of stimulus-response binding and retrieval: The head orientation (e.g., to the right) of a current stimulus retrieves the last episode with the same head orientation including the response that was part of this last episode. Thus, from a theoretical perspective, an attacking player would provoke the strongest detrimental effect on an opponent if s/he repeats the same head movement but changes the direction of the pass. Whether it is at all possible to strategically apply this recommendation in practice needs still to be answered.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究旨在研究护理人员在照顾痴呆症患者时对撒谎的看法和理由。
    数据包括对具有各种教育背景的家庭和专业护理人员之间的讨论的访谈和观察。主题分析用于确定与护理人员对说谎的看法相关的关键主题。
    该研究表明,护理人员经常使用谎言,并被视为护理人员工具包中的有效工具。这些做法通常被贴上“善意的谎言”的标签,并根据其增强痴呆症患者福祉或促进顺畅互动的潜力进行合理化。撒谎的潜在负面后果也得到了承认。此外,一些护理人员认为,“走进痴呆症患者的现实”的做法可能不构成撒谎。
    研究结果表明,护理人员在照顾痴呆症患者时认为撒谎是一种合法的策略;令人惊讶的是,有些人根本不承认他们的做法构成了说谎的行为。这一发现具有重要的临床意义,因为对撒谎的不同看法强调了对欺骗采取一致方法的潜在需求。解决这种复杂性可以导致更多的道德护理实践,最终提高痴呆症患者的护理质量。
    UNASSIGNED: This study aims to examine caregivers\' perspectives on and justifications for lying when caring for people with dementia.
    UNASSIGNED: The data consisted of interviews and observations of discussions among family and professional caregivers with various educational backgrounds. Thematic analysis was applied to identify key themes related to caregivers\' perspectives on lying.
    UNASSIGNED: The study revealed that lies were frequently employed by caregivers and were seen as effective tools in the caregivers\' toolkit. These practices were often labelled \'white lies\' and were rationalised based on their potential to enhance the well-being of people with dementia or to facilitate smooth interactions. The potential negative consequences of lying were also acknowledged. In addition, some caregivers suggested that the practice of \'stepping into the person with dementia\'s reality\' might not constitute lying.
    UNASSIGNED: The findings suggest that the caregivers perceived lying to be a legitimate strategy when caring for people with dementia; surprisingly, some did not recognize their practices as constituting acts of lying at all. This finding carries significant clinical relevance, as the varying perceptions of lying underscore the potential need for a consistent approach to deception. Addressing this complexity can lead to more ethical caregiving practices, ultimately enhancing the quality of care provided to people with dementia.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:COVID-19大流行加速并放大了虚拟研究方法的使用。虽然在线研究有几个优点,它还为个人提供了更大的机会来歪曲自己的身份,以欺诈方式参与研究以获取经济利益。参与者欺骗和欺诈已成为虚拟研究日益关注的问题。关于欺骗和预防策略的报告已经在在线定量研究中进行了讨论,特别是调查研究。不过,围绕这些与定性研究有关的问题的文献很少,特别是在物质使用研究中。结果:在本评论中,我们详细介绍了一项不可预见的案例研究,该案例研究涉及几个人,他们在参与虚拟同步定性物质使用研究期间似乎故意歪曲了自己的身份和信息。通过我们的经验,我们提供检测和防止参与者欺骗和欺诈的策略,以及实施这些方法时要考虑的挑战。结论:没有一般意识和保护措施,虚拟研究方法的完整性仍然容易出现不准确的情况。随着在线研究的不断扩大,必须积极设计创新的解决方案,以保护未来的研究免受日益复杂的欺骗和欺诈。
    Background: The COVID-19 pandemic has accelerated and amplified the use of virtual research methods. While online research has several advantages, it also provides greater opportunity for individuals to misrepresent their identities to fraudulently participate in research for financial gain. Participant deception and fraud have become a growing concern for virtual research. Reports of deception and preventative strategies have been discussed within online quantitative research, particularly survey studies. Though, there is a dearth of literature surrounding these issues pertaining to qualitative studies, particularly within substance use research. Results: In this commentary, we detail an unforeseen case study of several individuals who appeared to deliberately misrepresent their identities and information during participation in a virtual synchronous qualitative substance use study. Through our experiences, we offer strategies to detect and prevent participant deception and fraud, as well as challenges to consider when implementing these approaches. Conclusions: Without general awareness and protective measures, the integrity of virtual research methods remains vulnerable to inaccuracy. As online research continues to expand, it is essential to proactively design innovative solutions to safeguard future studies against increasingly sophisticated deception and fraud.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Randomized Controlled Trial
    目的:精神分裂症与心理理论(ToM)障碍有关,这被定义为对精神状态做出判断的能力,并且与内侧前额叶皮层活动有关。齐拉西酮,但不是氟哌啶醇,已知对内侧前额叶皮层有保护作用。因此,我们假设这两种药物在改善精神分裂症患者的ToM任务表现方面具有不同的疗效.
    方法:患有精神疾病诊断和统计手册的患者,第四版(DSM-IV)精神分裂症的性别匹配诊断,疾病的持续时间,和教育随机接受齐拉西酮(n=30)或氟哌啶醇(n=30)。使用阳性和阴性综合征量表以及个人和社会功能量表对所有患者进行评估。使用一阶错误信念任务评估了ToM,二阶错误信念任务,假任务,和读心的任务,按发展的复杂性和难度排序。主要结果是从基线到治疗16周的ToM性能变化。
    结果:对于一阶错误信念任务,组间差异无统计学意义(P>0.05)。对于二阶错误信念任务,交互作用显著(P<0.05),仅在齐拉西酮组中,时间的简单效应显示出显着差异(P<0.001)。对于假任务,交互效应不显著(P>0.05)。对于阅读眼中的心灵任务,交互作用显著(P<0.05),仅在齐拉西酮组中,时间的简单效应显示出显着差异(P<0.001)。两组之间的阳性和阴性综合征量表结果相似。在个人和社会功能量表上,齐拉西酮组的表现优于氟哌啶醇组。没有重大安全问题或不良事件。
    结论:这项研究的结果表明,齐拉西酮可以改善精神分裂症患者的ToM,并且在改善复杂的ToM以及个人和社会功能方面可能优于氟哌啶醇。
    ChiCTR2200060542。
    OBJECTIVE: Schizophrenia is associated with impairment in theory of mind (ToM), which is defined as the ability to make judgments about mental states and is related to medial prefrontal cortical activity. Ziprasidone, but not haloperidol, is known to have a protective effect in the medial prefrontal cortex. Thus, we hypothesized that these 2 drugs would have different efficacy in improving ToM task performance in patients with schizophrenia.
    METHODS: Patients with a Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) diagnosis of schizophrenia matched for sex, duration of illness, and education were randomized to receive ziprasidone (n=30) or haloperidol (n=30). All patients were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale and the Personal and Social Functioning Scale. ToM was assessed using a first-order false belief task, a second-order false belief task, the faux-pas task, and the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task, in order of developmental complexity and difficulty. The primary outcome was change in ToM performance from baseline to 16 weeks of treatment.
    RESULTS: For the first-order false belief task, there were no significant differences between the groups (P>0.05). For the second-order false belief task, the interaction effect was significant (P<0.05), and the simple effect of time showed a significant difference only in the ziprasidone group (P<0.001). For the faux-pas task, the interaction effect was not significant (P>0.05). For the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task, the interaction effect was significant (P<0.05), and the simple effect of time showed a significant difference only in the ziprasidone group (P<0.001). The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale results were similar between the groups. The ziprasidone group performed better than the haloperidol group on the Personal and Social Functioning Scale. There were no major safety concerns or adverse events.
    CONCLUSIONS: The findings of this study suggest that ziprasidone could improve ToM and might be superior to haloperidol for improving complex ToM as well as personal and social functioning in patients with schizophrenia.
    UNASSIGNED: ChiCTR2200060542.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    事件相关电位(ERPs)用于探讨时间压力下自我消耗对自发性欺骗的影响。StroopColor-Word测试用于操纵实验中参与者的自我消耗。采用视觉感知任务来评估参与者的欺骗倾向。结果表明,自我耗竭组比非耗竭组更容易发生欺骗,并引起更大的P3振幅。与高时间压力组相比,无时间压力组更可能欺骗并诱导更大的P3振幅。这些结果表明,拥有足够资源进行自我控制的个人更有可能抵制诱惑,而不太可能进行自我服务的欺骗。更高的时间压力使受试者更容易作弊。欺骗在一定条件下是自动和自发的。自我消耗和高时间压力促进欺骗的发生。
    Event-related potentials (ERPs) were used to explore the influence of ego depletion on spontaneous deception under time pressure. The Stroop Color-Word test was used to manipulate the participants\' ego depletion in the experiment. A visual perception task was employed to assess the participants\' deceptive tendency. The results indicated that the ego-depleted group was more prone to engaging deception and induced a larger P3 amplitude than did the nondepleted group. The no-time pressure group was more likely to deceive and induced a larger P3 amplitude than did the high-time pressure group. These results suggest that individuals with sufficient resources for self-control are more likely to resist temptation and less likely to engage in self-serving deception. Higher time pressure made subjects more likely to cheat. Deception is automatic and spontaneous under certain conditions. Ego depletion and high time pressure promote the occurrence of deception.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号