dominance

优势
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    优势是指杂合基因型相对于两种纯合基因型的影响。适应性突变的优势程度可以对有害和有益的突变随时间的频率变化以及围绕此类选定等位基因的连锁中性遗传变异的模式产生深远的影响。因为支配地位是这样一个基本概念,它在整个人口遗传学历史上受到了极大的关注。费希尔的早期工作,Wright,霍尔丹专注于理解优势存在的概念基础。最近的工作试图通过估计突变的优势效应来测试这些理论和概念模型。然而,众所周知,估算优势系数具有挑战性,并且在有限的研究中只在少数物种中进行过。在这次审查中,我们首先描述一些早期的理论和概念模型,以理解支配存在的机制。第二,我们讨论了几种估计优势系数的方法,并总结了优势系数的估计。我们注意到不同物种观察到的趋势,突变类型,和基因的功能类别。通过比较不同类型基因的优势系数的估计,我们检验了几个存在优势的假设。最后,我们讨论了优势如何影响种群中有益和有害突变的动态,以及有害突变的优势程度如何影响近亲繁殖对适应性的影响.
    Dominance refers to the effect of a heterozygous genotype relative to that of the two homozygous genotypes. The degree of dominance of mutations for fitness can have a profound impact on how deleterious and beneficial mutations change in frequency over time as well as on the patterns of linked neutral genetic variation surrounding such selected alleles. Since dominance is such a fundamental concept, it has received immense attention throughout the history of population genetics. Early work from Fisher, Wright, and Haldane focused on understanding the conceptual basis for why dominance exists. More recent work has attempted to test these theories and conceptual models by estimating dominance effects of mutations. However, estimating dominance coefficients has been notoriously challenging and has only been done in a few species in a limited number of studies. In this review, we first describe some of the early theoretical and conceptual models for understanding the mechanisms for the existence of dominance. Second, we discuss several approaches used to estimate dominance coefficients and summarize estimates of dominance coefficients. We note trends that have been observed across species, types of mutations, and functional categories of genes. By comparing estimates of dominance coefficients for different types of genes, we test several hypotheses for the existence of dominance. Lastly, we discuss how dominance influences the dynamics of beneficial and deleterious mutations in populations and how the degree of dominance of deleterious mutations influences the impact of inbreeding on fitness.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    跟随蚂蚁的行为是新热带和非洲热带雨林鸟类的普遍现象,但在中非却鲜为人知。我们在这里报道喀麦隆低地雨林的现象,量化不同蚂蚁跟随鸟类物种与驱动蚂蚁相互作用的强度,并检验以下假设:蚂蚁跟随行为的专业化水平较高与优势或侵略依赖性羽毛和其他形态特征有关。羊群大小在1至11个个体之间变化,同时出现的平均大小为5.34±2.68(平均值±SD)。在一次突袭中观察到的最大物种数量是十个,而最小数量为4个,总体物种丰富度为6.89±2.1个。21种参与的鸟类在蚂蚁跟随行为的程度上差异很大。在种间比较中,羽毛特征,如彩色冠的存在,眼点,眼睛周围裸露的皮肤,结合meta骨的长度和重量,与蚂蚁跟随行为显著相关。这些结果表明,中非物种蚂蚁跟随鸟类组合的大小和身份与东非报道的相似。他们还建议,蚂蚁跟随有利于选择特征,这些特征标志着在林下为争夺有价值的食物资源而挣扎的个体之间的相互作用中占主导地位。
    Ant-following behavior is a common phenomenon in birds of Neotropical and Afrotropical rainforests but yet little is known from Central Africa. We here report on the phenomenon in lowland rainforest in Cameroon, quantifying the strength of the interaction of different ant-following bird species with driver ants and test the hypothesis that higher levels of specialization in ant-following behavior are associated with dominance or aggression-dependent plumage and other morphological traits. Flock size varied between 1 and 11 individuals with a mean size of 5.34 ± 2.68 (mean ± SD) individuals occurring at the same time. The maximum number of species present during one raid observed was ten, whereas the minimum number was four with an overall species richness of 6.89 ± 2.1 species. The 21 attending bird species strongly varied in the degree of ant-following behavior. In an interspecific comparison, plumage traits such as the presence of a colored crown, eyespots, and bare skin around the eye, in combination with metatarsus length and weight, were significantly correlated with ant-following behavior. These results suggest that-in size and identity of species-ant-following bird assemblages in Central Africa are similar to those reported from East Africa. They also suggest that ant following favors the selection of traits that signal dominance in interactions between individuals struggling for valuable food resources in the forest understory.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    鉴于人类社会生活的复杂性,观察到孩子们适应社会环境的速度是惊人的。被其他成员接受,理解和遵守该小组共享的规则和规范至关重要。幼儿如何以及从谁那里学习这些社会规则?在实验中,基于情感社会学习和优势等级在简单规则理解中的关键作用,我们展示了15至23个月大的儿童和3至5岁儿童的视频,其中控制了特工的体型和情感线索。在主导规则制定者的条件下,当一个较小的主角把一个物体放在一个位置时,更大的代理做出积极的反应;相反,当较小的主角将一个物体放在另一个位置时,更大的代理显示负面反应。在从属规则制定者条件下,角色转移了,但代理人不同。幼儿期望主角遵守规则(基于预期的外表),独立于规则制定主体的主导地位。三到五岁的学龄前儿童整体表现在机会水平,但希望主角在第一次审判中不遵守规则,如果规则制定者占主导地位,并在第二次审判中遵守规则。
    Given the complexity of human social life, it is astonishing to observe how quickly children adapt to their social environment. To be accepted by the other members, it is crucial to understand and follow the rules and norms shared by the group. How and from whom do young children learn these social rules? In the experiments, based on the crucial role of affective social learning and dominance hierarchies in simple rule understanding, we showed 15-to-23-month-olds and 3-to-5-year-old children videos where the agents\' body size and affective cues were manipulated. In the dominant rule-maker condition, when a smaller protagonist puts an object in one location, a bigger agent reacts with a positive reaction; on the contrary, when the smaller protagonist puts an object in another location, the bigger agent displays a negative reaction. In the subordinate rule-maker condition, the roles are shifted but the agents differ. Toddlers expect the protagonist to follow the rules (based on anticipatory looks), independent of the dominant status of the rule-making agent. Three-to-five-year-old pre-schoolers overall perform at the chance level but expect the protagonist to disobey a rule in the first trial, and obey the rule in the second trial if the rule-maker is dominant.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    团体生活的动物参与协调的发声,作为一个团体离开一个地点,而且经常,就运动方向达成共识。这里,我们第一次记录,使用协调的发声,“让我们走”隆隆声,野生雄性非洲象群从水坑出发。我们记录了发声并收集了行为数据,因为在2005年,2007年,2011年和2017年6月至7月的野外季节中,在埃托沙国家公园内的穆沙拉水坑中从事这些发声的已知个体,纳米比亚。在离境活动期间,我们记录了哪些人参与了电话,每个个人呼叫的签名结构,以及呼叫者的顺序,来电者的社会地位,以及那些发起离开的人。先前在紧密的家庭团体中描述了“让我们走”的隆隆声,以在协调离开期间使家人团结在一起。雄性大象被描述为生活在松散的社会群体中,使这一发现特别引人注目。我们发现这种声音协调发生在密切相关的群体中,高度结合的个体,很少发生在松散的同事之间。最有可能发起“让我们走”隆隆声的三个人都是高度社会融合的,其中一个人也是最占优势的。这一发现表明,更多社会融合的个体可能更有可能发起,或者铅,在离开水坑的背景下,一群亲密的同事,就像一个高级女性在家庭团体中所做的那样。事实上,许多人参与了声乐比赛,出发时间可以更短,较长,或与出发前相同的时间,所有这些都表明对离开的行为有共识,即使事件是由领先的个人触发的。
    Group-living animals engage in coordinated vocalizations to depart from a location as a group, and often, to come to a consensus about the direction of movement. Here, we document for the first time, the use of coordinated vocalizations, the \"let\'s go\" rumble, in wild male African elephant group departures from a waterhole. We recorded vocalizations and collected behavioral data as known individuals engaged in these vocal bouts during June-July field seasons in 2005, 2007, 2011, and 2017 at Mushara waterhole within Etosha National Park, Namibia. During departure events, we documented which individuals were involved in the calls, the signature structure of each individual\'s calls, as well as the ordering of callers, the social status of the callers, and those who initiated departure. The \"let\'s go\" rumble was previously described in tight-knit family groups to keep the family together during coordinated departures. Male elephants are described as living in loose social groups, making this finding particularly striking. We found that this vocal coordination occurs in groups of closely associated, highly bonded individuals and rarely occurs between looser associates. The three individuals most likely to initiate the \"let\'s go\" rumble bouts were all highly socially integrated, and one of these individuals was also the most dominant overall. This finding suggests that more socially integrated individuals might be more likely to initiate, or lead, a close group of associates in the context of leaving the waterhole, just as a high-ranking female would do in a family group. The fact that many individuals were involved in the vocal bouts, and that departure periods could be shorter, longer, or the same amount of time as pre-departure periods, all suggest that there is consensus with regard to the act of leaving, even though the event was triggered by a lead individual.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    不同年龄段不同的死亡率趋势促使人们监测寿命不平等和预期寿命。当预期寿命和寿命不平等朝同一方向发展时,或者当不平等措施显示出不一致的趋势时,结论就模棱两可。我们建议使用非参数优势分析来获得死亡年龄分布的稳健排名。对2006-2021年美国寿命表的应用表明,直到2014年,最近几年通常占主导地位,这意味着如果寿命更长、分布不均匀分布的寿命被认为更好,就会有所改善。与非西班牙裔白人相比,非西班牙裔黑人和西班牙裔黑人的改善更为明显。自2014年以来,对于所有亚群-特别是西班牙裔-早些年通常占主导地位,最近几年,如果更短和更不平等的寿命被认为更糟,则表明死亡年龄分布恶化。在COVID-19大流行期间,2020-2021年分布的急剧恶化对西班牙裔个体最为明显。
    Diverging mortality trends at different ages motivate the monitoring of lifespan inequality alongside life expectancy. Conclusions are ambiguous when life expectancy and lifespan inequality move in the same direction or when inequality measures display inconsistent trends. We propose using nonparametric dominance analysis to obtain a robust ranking of age-at-death distributions. Application to U.S. period life tables for 2006-2021 reveals that, until 2014, more recent years generally dominate earlier years, implying improvement if longer lifespans that are less unequally distributed are considered better. Improvements were more pronounced for non-Hispanic Black and Hispanic individuals than for non-Hispanic White individuals. Since 2014, for all subpopulations-particularly Hispanics-earlier years often dominate more recent years, indicating worsening age-at-death distributions if shorter and more unequal lifespans are considered worse. Dramatic deterioration of the distributions in 2020-2021 during the COVID-19 pandemic is most evident for Hispanic individuals.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们研究的目的是比较踝关节背屈和足内收过程中胫骨前肌(TA)运动单位的力稳定性和放电特征。15名年轻男子用双腿在五个目标力下进行了最大和次最大收缩,使用高密度肌电图记录TA的运动单位活动。最大力和亚最大收缩期间的力波动在两条腿之间相似(p>0.05)。运动单位活动通过平均放电率(MDR)的测量来表征,尖峰间隔的变异系数(ISI的CoV),和过滤后的累积尖峰序列的标准偏差(fCST的SD)。踝关节背屈期间,两腿之间的运动单位活动没有统计学上的显着差异。相比之下,ISI的MDR和CoV,而不是FCST的SD,与足内收期间的优势足相比,非优势足更大。尽管如此,运动单位活动的这些差异不足以影响亚最大收缩期间的力波动.这些结果表明,在两次动作中对TA产生的力的控制不受肢体优势的影响。
    The aim of our study was to compare the force steadiness and the discharge characteristics of motor units in the tibialis anterior (TA) during ankle dorsiflexion and foot adduction produced by submaximal isometric contractions with the dominant and non-dominant foot. Fifteen young men performed maximal and submaximal contractions at five target forces with both legs, and motor unit activity in TA was recorded using high-density electromyography. Maximal force and the fluctuations in force during submaximal contractions were similar between the two legs (p > 0.05). Motor unit activity was characterized by measures of mean discharge rate (MDR), coefficient of variation for interspike interval (CoV for ISI), and standard deviation of the filtered cumulative spike train (SD of fCST). There were no statistically significant differences in motor unit activity between legs during ankle dorsiflexion. In contrast, the MDR and the CoV for ISI but not the SD of fCST, were greater for the non-dominant foot compared with the dominant foot during foot adduction. Nonetheless, these differences in motor unit activity were not sufficient to influence the force fluctuations during the submaximal contractions. These results indicate that control of the force produced by TA during the two actions was not influenced by limb dominance.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对于群居物种的雄性,优势地位和隶属关系的强度可能会产生重大的健身后果。社会动态也会通过影响糖皮质激素来增加成本,体内平衡的中介和对挑战和组内竞争的生理反应的指标。我们调查了优势之间的关系,社会纽带,季节性挑战,PhuKhieo野生动物保护区野生阿萨姆猕猴(Macacaassamensis)的粪便糖皮质激素代谢产物(fGC)测量,泰国,将行为数据与来自62名15岁以上成年男性的4129份样本相结合。我们先前对该种群的研究表明,在交配季节竞争加剧与fGC水平升高有关,对雄性灵长类动物来说是不寻常的,较低的等级位置与较高的fGC水平相关。有了更大的数据集和社会性的动态测量,我们重新检验了这些关系,并额外检验了社会支持的潜在fGC衰减效应.与我们之前的研究相反,但与大多数关于雄性灵长类动物的工作一致,优势等级与fGC水平呈正相关,高状态与糖皮质激素措施升高相关。在交配季节开始时,fGC水平增加。我们证明了男性支持关系的fGC减少作用,并表明隶属关系的动态可以与生理反应的动态相关。我们的结果表明,在具有中等竞赛潜力的系统中,高优势地位会给男性带来生理成本,而这种成本可能会受到社会关系的影响。我们强调需要考虑影响荷尔蒙过程的社会性和竞争的动态。
    For males of gregarious species, dominance status and the strength of affiliative relationships can have major fitness consequences. Social dynamics also impose costs by affecting glucocorticoids, mediators of homeostasis and indicators of the physiological response to challenges and within-group competition. We investigated the relationships between dominance, social bonds, seasonal challenges, and faecal glucocorticoid metabolite (fGC) measures in wild Assamese macaques (Macaca assamensis) at Phu Khieo Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand, combining behavioural data with 4129 samples from 62 adult males over 15 years. Our previous work on this population suggested that increased competition during the mating season was associated with elevated fGC levels and that, unusually for male primates, lower rank position correlated with higher fGC levels. With a much larger dataset and dynamic measures of sociality, we re-examined these relationships and additionally tested the potentially fGC-attenuating effect of social support. Contrary to our previous study, yet consistent with the majority of work on male primates, dominance rank had a positive relationship with fGC levels, as high status correlated with elevated glucocorticoid measures. fGC levels were increased at the onset of the mating season. We demonstrated an fGC-reducing effect of supportive relationships in males and showed that dynamics in affiliation can correlate with dynamics in physiological responses. Our results suggest that in a system with intermediate contest potential, high dominance status can impose physiological costs on males that may potentially be moderated by social relationships. We highlight the need to consider the dynamics of sociality and competition that influence hormonal processes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    猪是一种社会物种,他们建立层次结构以更好地利用资源并减少冲突。然而,在养猪生产中,优势动物和从属动物的生长机会可能不同。在本研究中,对一个系统进行了测试,以对生长中的猪进行显性和下级测试,以调查层次结构如何影响喂养行为,增长,和粪便中评估的肠道微生物群。使用了八种不同围栏中的64只动物,有四个被阉割的雄性和四个雌性,到达时重18公斤,并在整个生长期保持,直到140公斤。在研究开始后58、100和133天直接从肛门从动物获得三个粪便样品以避免粪便污染,以调查微生物群组成。优势动物在生长期的收获高于下属。此外,他们全天都在进行更多的喂食器访问。在五个细菌属中观察到差异丰度模式,Oliverpabstia,肽球菌,在优势动物中,粪细菌更为丰富,而在从属动物中,霍德曼氏杆菌和Acetotomaculum的代表过多。该微生物生物标志物以0.92的AUC准确地对样品的显性与从属组进行分类。
    Pigs are a social species, and they establish hierarchies for better use of resources and to reduce conflicts. However, in pig production, the opportunities for growth can differ between dominant and subordinate animals. In the present study, a system was tested to perform a dominant versus subordinate test in growing pigs to investigate how the hierarchy affects feeding behaviour, growth, and gut microbiota assessed in faeces. Sixty-four animals housed in eight different pens were used, with four castrated males and four females in each one, weighing 18 kg at arrival and maintained during the whole growing period, until 140 kg. Three stool samples were obtained from the animals directly from the anus to avoid contamination of the faeces 58, 100, and 133 days after the start of the study to investigate the microbiota composition. The dominant animals had higher gains during the growing period than the subordinates. In addition, they were performing more visits to the feeder throughout the day. Differential abundance patterns were observed in five bacterial genera, with Oliverpabstia, Peptococcus, and Faecalbacterium being more abundant in dominant animals and Holdemanella and Acetitomaculum being overrepresented in subordinate ones. This microbial biomarker accurately classified dominant versus subordinate groups of samples with an AUC of 0.92.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    注意焦点是一种现象,其中转移注意力的焦点会改变站立姿势控制的表现。它可以归类为内部焦点(IF),将注意力转移到身体部位,或外部焦点(EF),将注意力集中在外部环境上。尽管改善年轻人站立姿势控制的注意力集中表现出个人优势,老年人站立姿势控制中注意力集中的优势仍然不明确。因此,这项研究旨在比较健康的年轻人和老年人在站立姿势控制中注意力集中的优势,了解衰老过程的关键步骤。
    参与者在IF和EF条件下进行了站立姿势控制任务。基于他们表现出卓越性能的条件,参与者分为两组:IF-显性和EF-显性.随后比较了在IF和EF条件下每组的站立姿势控制表现。
    结果显示,参与者,包括年轻人和老年人,分为IF显性和EF显性,确认注意力集中的优势。老年人在EF条件下的表现也受到注意力焦点优势的影响。
    这些结果突出了基于注意力集中优势的干预方法的潜在重要性,为未来的研究和临床实践提供有价值的见解。
    UNASSIGNED: Attentional focus is a phenomenon in which shifting the focus of attention alters performance of standing postural control. It can be categorized as internal focus (IF), which directs attention to the body parts, or external focus (EF), which directs attention to the external environment. Although attentional focus that improves standing postural control in younger people exhibits individual dominance, the dominance of attentional focus in standing postural control in older adults remains ambiguous. Therefore, this study aimed to compare the dominance of attentional focus in standing postural control between healthy younger and older adults, a crucial step for understanding the aging process.
    UNASSIGNED: The participants performed a standing postural control task under the IF and EF conditions. Based on the condition during which they exhibited superior performance, the participants were divided into two groups: IF-dominant and EF-dominant. The standing postural control performance in each group under the IF and EF conditions was subsequently compared.
    UNASSIGNED: The results showed that the participants, encompassing both younger and older adults, were divided into the IF-dominant and EF-dominant groups, confirming the dominance of attentional focus. The performance under the EF condition in older adults was also influenced by the dominance of attentional focus.
    UNASSIGNED: These results highlight the potential importance of intervention methods based on the dominance of attentional focus, providing valuable insights into future research and clinical practice.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    文字等传统媒体,images,音频,视频主要针对特定的感官,如视觉和听觉。相比之下,多种感官媒体旨在通过整合其他感官方式(如触摸)来创造身临其境的体验,气味,并在适用的地方品尝。触觉增强的视听内容除了视觉和听觉刺激外,还利用触觉,旨在为用户创建更具沉浸感和吸引力的交互。以前,触觉增强内容已在2D情感空间(效价和唤醒)中进行了探索。在本文中,针对触觉增强视听内容的EEG数据基于3维的自我评估人体模型量表进行标记,即价,唤醒,和优势。统计显著性(95%置信区间)也是基于收集的分数来建立的,强调了传统媒体和触觉增强媒体在唤醒和优势维度上的显著差异。提出了一种使用依赖于分类器的特征选择方法对效价进行分类的新方法,唤醒,和使用三个不同分类器的优势状态。最高准确率为75%,73.8%,75%用于对化合价进行分类,唤醒,和优势国家,分别。所提出的方案在准确性方面优于先前基于情感识别的研究,以响应增强的多媒体内容,F分数,和其他错误参数。
    Traditional media such as text, images, audio, and video primarily target specific senses like vision and hearing. In contrast, multiple sensorial media aims to create immersive experiences by integrating additional sensory modalities such as touch, smell, and taste where applicable. Tactile enhanced audio-visual content leverages the sense of touch in addition to visual and auditory stimuli, aiming to create a more immersive and engaging interaction for users. Previously, tactile enhanced content has been explored in 2D emotional space (valence and arousal). In this paper, EEG data against tactile enhanced audio-visual content is labeled based on a self-assessment manikin scale in 3 dimensions i.e., valence, arousal, and dominance. Statistical significance (with a 95% confidence interval) is also established based on gathered scores, highlighting a significant difference in the arousal and dominance dimension of traditional media and tactile enhanced media. A new methodology is proposed using classifier-dependent feature selection approach to classify valence, arousal, and dominance states using three different classifiers. A highest accuracy of 75%, 73.8%, and 75% is achieved for classifying valence, arousal, and dominance states, respectively. The proposed scheme outperforms previous emotion recognition based studies in response to enhanced multimedia content in terms of accuracy, F-score, and other error parameters.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号