Language Development

语言发展
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:语言延迟会影响幼儿的近期和长期社交交流和学习,and,越来越多的专家关注它。韵律歧视的发展是语言发展的最早阶段之一,掌握了后期阶段的关键技能。因此,分析语言韵律的大脑辨别力与语言能力的关系,可以为语言延迟的诊断和干预提供客观依据。
    方法:在本研究中,所有病例(n=241)均来自三级妇女医院,从2021年到2022年。我们使用功能近红外光谱(fNIRS)来评估儿童的神经韵律辨别能力,并使用中文交际发展清单(CCDI)来评估他们的语言能力。
    结果:在I期和II期研究的最终分析中纳入了98名足月和108名早产幼儿,分别。足月CCDI筛查总异常率为9.2%,早产儿为34.3%。足月幼儿在除通道5外的所有通道中均显示出韵律辨别能力,而早产幼儿仅在通道6中显示出韵律辨别能力。多因素logistic回归分析表明,右角回(通道3)的韵律判别对足月幼儿的语言延迟有统计学意义(奇数比=0.301,P<0.05)。随机森林(RF)回归模型表明,基于fNIRS数据的通道和大脑区域反映的韵律判别是预测早产儿语言延迟的重要参数。其中由右角回(通道4)反映的韵律判别是最重要的参数。模型接收器工作特征(ROC)曲线下面积为0.687。
    结论:神经韵律辨别能力与语言发育呈正相关,通过fNIRS对脑韵律辨别能力的评估可作为未来临床应用中早期识别语言延迟儿童的客观指标.
    BACKGROUND: Language delay affects near- and long-term social communication and learning in toddlers, and, an increasing number of experts pay attention to it. The development of prosody discrimination is one of the earliest stages of language development in which key skills for later stages are mastered. Therefore, analyzing the relationship between brain discrimination of speech prosody and language abilities may provide an objective basis for the diagnosis and intervention of language delay.
    METHODS: In this study, all cases(n = 241) were enrolled from a tertiary women\'s hospital, from 2021 to 2022. We used functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to assess children\'s neural prosody discrimination abilities, and a Chinese communicative development inventory (CCDI) were used to evaluate their language abilities.
    RESULTS: Ninety-eight full-term and 108 preterm toddlers were included in the final analysis in phase I and II studies, respectively. The total CCDI screening abnormality rate was 9.2% for full-term and 34.3% for preterm toddlers. Full-term toddlers showed prosody discrimination ability in all channels except channel 5, while preterm toddlers showed prosody discrimination ability in channel 6 only. Multifactorial logistic regression analyses showed that prosody discrimination of the right angular gyrus (channel 3) had a statistically significant effect on language delay (odd ratio = 0.301, P < 0.05) in full-term toddlers. Random forest (RF) regression model presented that prosody discrimination reflected by channels and brain regions based on fNIRS data was an important parameter for predicting language delay in preterm toddlers, among which the prosody discrimination reflected by the right angular gyrus (channel 4) was the most important parameter. The area under the model Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve was 0.687.
    CONCLUSIONS: Neural prosody discrimination ability is positively associated with language development, assessment of brain prosody discrimination abilities through fNIRS could be used as an objective indicator for early identification of children with language delay in the future clinical application.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:作者在多机构结节性硬化症(TSC)自闭症卓越研究网络(TACERN)研究中评估了癫痫手术时机对术后神经认知结果的影响。
    方法:TACERN队列中159例患者中有27例患有药物难治性癫痫并接受了手术。手术年龄范围为15.86至154.14周(中位数为91.93周)。在三个神经心理学测试中,患者术前第一次(10-58周)到术后最后一次(155-188周)得分的变化-早期学习的Mullen量表(MSEL),Vineland适应性行为量表,第2版(VABS-2),和学龄前语言量表,计算第5版(PLS-5)。使用Pearson相关性和多元线性回归模型分别将测试结果与手术年龄和手术前癫痫持续时间相关联。分别对癫痫发作负担减轻的患者(n=21)和癫痫发作负担没有减轻的患者(n=6)进行分析。回归分析特别针对21例成功控制癫痫发作的患者。
    结果:手术时的年龄与MSEL的组合言语子测验的变化呈显着负相关(R=-0.45,p=0.039),并在多元线性回归模型中预测了该得分(β=-0.09,p=0.035)。在PLS-5的总语言得分(R=-0.4,p=0.089;β=-0.12,p=0.014)以及在手术前检查癫痫持续时间的分析中也看到了类似的趋势。手术年龄和手术前癫痫持续时间与VABS-2语言评分变化之间的关联差异更大(R=-0.15,p=0.52;β=-0.05,p=0.482)。
    结论:早期手术和手术前癫痫持续时间较短与TSC患者术后语言改善有关。需要前瞻性或比较有效性临床试验来进一步阐明手术时机对神经认知结果的影响。
    OBJECTIVE: The authors evaluated the impact of the timing of epilepsy surgery on postoperative neurocognitive outcomes in a cohort of children followed in the multiinstitutional Tuberous Sclerosis Complex (TSC) Autism Center of Excellence Research Network (TACERN) study.
    METHODS: Twenty-seven of 159 patients in the TACERN cohort had drug-refractory epilepsy and underwent surgery. Ages at surgery ranged from 15.86 to 154.14 weeks (median 91.93 weeks). Changes in patients\' first preoperative (10-58 weeks) to last postoperative (155-188 weeks) scores on three neuropsychological tests-the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd edition (VABS-2), and the Preschool Language Scales, 5th edition (PLS-5)-were calculated. Pearson correlation and multivariate linear regression models were used to correlate test outcomes separately with age at surgery and duration of epilepsy prior to surgery. Analyses were separately conducted for patients whose seizure burdens decreased postoperatively (n = 21) and those whose seizure burdens did not (n = 6). Regression analysis was specifically focused on the 21 patients who achieved successful seizure control.
    RESULTS: Age at surgery was significantly negatively correlated with the change in the combined verbal subtests of the MSEL (R = -0.45, p = 0.039) and predicted this score in a multivariate linear regression model (β = -0.09, p = 0.035). Similar trends were seen in the total language score of the PLS-5 (R = -0.4, p = 0.089; β = -0.12, p = 0.014) and in analyses examining the duration of epilepsy prior to surgery as the independent variable of interest. Associations between age at surgery and duration of epilepsy prior to surgery with changes in the verbal subscores of VABS-2 were more variable (R = -0.15, p = 0.52; β = -0.05, p = 0.482).
    CONCLUSIONS: Earlier surgery and shorter epilepsy duration prior to surgery were associated with greater improvement in postoperative language in patients with TSC. Prospective or comparative effectiveness clinical trials are needed to further elucidate surgical timing impacts on neurocognitive outcomes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    语言的复杂性各不相同;看护者与儿童的交流互动结构不同;男孩和女孩的语言技能可能不同。使用多层次建模方法,我们探讨了这些因素如何影响世界各地成长的幼儿(平均年龄2岁9个月;56名女孩)的语言习得路径.在43个不同的地点,我们分析了103对说3,170,633句话的母子对,16,209,659个语素,分为20种不同的语言:南非荷兰语,加泰罗尼亚语,粤语,丹麦语,荷兰人,英语,波斯语,法语,德语,希伯来语,冰岛语,爱尔兰人,意大利语,日本人,普通话,挪威语,葡萄牙语,西班牙语,瑞典语和土耳其语。使用平均语长(MLU)作为语言复杂性和发展技能的衡量标准,我们发现,在儿童的MLU的变化显着解释(a)语言之间的差异;即儿童MLU的增长速度与他们的母语的复杂性,和(b)母亲之间的差异;即使用较高MLU的母亲倾向于有较高MLU的孩子,无论他们学习哪种语言,尤其是在很小的时候(<2.5岁)。控制家庭和语言环境,我们在儿童言语和针对男孩和女孩的言语中没有发现MLU性别差异的证据.通过将语言建模为具有跨文化变异的多层次结构,我们能够解开那些使儿童的语言路径不同的因素和那些使它相同的因素。研究重点:来自20种不同语言的103对母子的语音显示出早期语言发展路径的巨大差异。语言,家庭,但不是孩子的性别,占儿童言语个体差异的很大比例,尤其是在很小的时候。儿童学习语言的速度与母语的复杂性相协调,更复杂的语言轨迹更陡峭。结果表明文化的相对影响,家庭,和性别塑造了不同儿童的语言习得路径。
    Languages vary in their complexity; caregivers vary in the way they structure their communicative interactions with children; and boys and girls can differ in their language skills. Using a multilevel modelling approach, we explored how these factors influence the path of language acquisition for young children growing up around the world (mean age 2-years 9-months; 56 girls). Across 43 different sites, we analysed 103 mother-child pairs who spoke 3,170,633 utterances, 16,209,659 morphemes, divided across 20 different languages: Afrikaans, Catalan, Cantonese, Danish, Dutch, English, Farsi, French, German, Hebrew, Icelandic, Irish, Italian, Japanese, Mandarin, Norwegian, Portuguese, Spanish, Swedish and Turkish. Using mean length of utterance (MLU) as a measure of language complexity and developmental skill, we found that variation in children\'s MLU was significantly explained by (a) between-language differences; namely the rate of child MLU growth was attuned to the complexity of their mother tongue, and (b) between-mother differences; namely mothers who used higher MLUs tended to have children with higher MLUs, regardless of which language they were learning and especially in the very young (<2.5 years-old). Controlling for family and language environment, we found no evidence of MLU sex differences in child speech nor in the speech addressed to boys and girls. By modelling language as a multilevel structure with cross-cultural variation, we were able to disentangle those factors that make children\'s pathway to language different and those that make it alike. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: The speech of 103 mother-child pairs from 20 different languages showed large variation in the path of early language development. Language, family, but not the sex of the child, accounted for a significant proportion of individual differences in child speech, especially in the very young. The rate at which children learned language was attuned to the complexity of their mother tongue, with steeper trajectories for more complex language. Results demonstrate the relative influence of culture, family, and sex in shaping the path of language acquisition for different children.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    组合思想,或将有限的概念集组合成无数复杂的思想和知识结构的能力,是人类思维生产力的关键,是沟通的基础,科学,技术,和艺术。尽管组合思维对人类认知和文化的重要性,它的发展起源仍然未知。为了解决这个问题,我们测试了12个月大的婴儿(N=60)他们还不能说话,只能听懂几句话,可以结合由口头输入激活的数量和种类概念。我们分两步进行:第一,我们教婴儿两个表示数量的新标签(例如,1项\"mize\";2项\"padu\",实验1)。然后,我们评估了他们是否可以在听到包含其标签的复杂表达式时结合数量和种类概念(例如,“帕杜鸭子”,实验2-3)。在测试中,婴儿查看了四组不同的对象(例如,1只鸭子,2只鸭子,1个球,2个球),同时呈现目标短语(例如,\“paduduck\”)命名其中一个(例如,2只鸭子)。他们成功地在线检索和组合了标记的概念,这一点得到了证明,增加了对命名集的关注,而不是对干扰集的关注。我们的结果表明,在生命的第一年结束时,可以使用组合过程来构建复杂的表示形式。婴儿的思想似乎旨在以新颖的生产性方式整合概念。这种能力可以是破译(一种或多种)环境语言和构建能够实现快速和灵活学习的抽象经验模型的先决条件。
    Combinatorial thought, or the ability to combine a finite set of concepts into a myriad of complex ideas and knowledge structures, is the key to the productivity of the human mind and underlies communication, science, technology, and art. Despite the importance of combinatorial thought for human cognition and culture, its developmental origins remain unknown. To address this, we tested whether 12-mo-old infants (N = 60), who cannot yet speak and only understand a handful of words, can combine quantity and kind concepts activated by verbal input. We proceeded in two steps: first, we taught infants two novel labels denoting quantity (e.g., \"mize\" for 1 item; \"padu\" for 2 items, Experiment 1). Then, we assessed whether they could combine quantity and kind concepts upon hearing complex expressions comprising their labels (e.g., \"padu duck\", Experiments 2-3). At test, infants viewed four different sets of objects (e.g., 1 duck, 2 ducks, 1 ball, 2 balls) while being presented with the target phrase (e.g., \"padu duck\") naming one of them (e.g., 2 ducks). They successfully retrieved and combined on-line the labeled concepts, as evidenced by increased looking to the named sets but not to distractor sets. Our results suggest that combinatorial processes for building complex representations are available by the end of the first year of life. The infant mind seems geared to integrate concepts in novel productive ways. This ability may be a precondition for deciphering the ambient language(s) and building abstract models of experience that enable fast and flexible learning.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    世界卫生组织(WHO)估计,全球约3200万儿童受到听力损失(HL)的影响。人工耳蜗是严重至严重的感觉神经性HL的一线治疗方法。它被认为是迄今为止开发的最成功的假体之一。目的评估巴西南部某参考医院治疗HL的舌前耳聋患儿的口语发育情况。方法我们进行了一项回顾性队列研究,回顾了2009年1月至2018年12月接受人工耳蜗植入手术患者的病历。通过与耳蜗植入物组的言语治疗专业人员进行的咨询来评估语言发展。结果共纳入152名儿童。人工耳蜗植入手术的平均年龄为41个月(标准偏差[SD]:±15)。考虑到日常生活中最常用的语言类型,将患者分为六组。我们发现,36%的儿童使用口头语言作为他们的主要沟通方式。在子分析中,我们观察到,口头语言发达或发展的患者比使用巴西手语的患者更早接受了人工耳蜗植入手术(LínguaBrasileiradeSinais,LIBRAS,葡萄牙语)或没有发达语言的语言。结论人工耳蜗是一种先进的技术,可以重建听觉和发展口语。然而,语言发展是一个复杂的过程,众所周知,这是一个正确发生的关键时期。我们仍然看到许多患者接受晚期诊断和治疗,这意味着延迟,经常,无法发展口头交流。证据水平3级(队列研究)。
    Introduction  The World Health Organization (WHO) estimates that ∼ 32 million children worldwide are affected by hearing loss (HL). Cochlear implant is the first-line treatment for severe to profound sensorineural HL. It is considered one of the most successful prostheses developed to date. Objective  To evaluate the oral language development of pediatric patients with prelingual deafness implanted in a reference hospital for the treatment of HL in southern Brazil. Methods  We conducted a retrospective cohort study with a review of medical records of patients undergoing cochlear implant surgery between January 2009 and December 2018. Language development was assessed by reviewing consultations with speech therapy professionals from the cochlear implant group. Results  A total of 152 children were included in the study. The mean age at cochlear implant surgery was of 41 months (standard deviation [SD]: ± 15). The patients were divided into six groups considering the type of language most used in their daily lives. We found that 36% of children use oral language as their primary form of communication. In a subanalysis, we observed that patients with developed or developing oral language had undergone cochlear implant surgery earlier than patients using Brazilian Sign Language (Língua Brasileira de Sinais, LIBRAS, in Portuguese) or those without developed language. Conclusion  The cochlear implant is a state-of-the-art technology that enables the re-establishment of the sense of hearing and the development of oral language. However, language development is a complex process known to present a critical period to properly occur. We still see many patients receiving late diagnosis and treatment, which implies a delay and, often, the impossibility of developing oral communication. Level of Evidence  Level 3 (cohort study).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    有大量证据表明,儿童在话语中明显遗漏了语法语素,例如“她打网球”和“妈咪吃”实际上是委托错误,其中在输入中遇到的上下文许可的未标记形式以上下文盲的方式再现。那么孩子们如何停止犯这样的错误呢?在这项研究中,我们测试了这样一个假设,即儿童从错误中恢复的能力与他们对更远距离依赖关系的敏感性有关。我们使用预先注册的语料库分析来探索不同线索对儿童动词标记错误的预测价值,并观察与此帐户一致的发展模式。我们查看上下文无关的线索(所使用的特定动词的身份)和上下文相关线索的相对值(所使用的特定主语+动词序列的身份)。我们发现,在一组2至3岁的孩子和一组3至4岁的孩子中,唯一一致的效果是使用未标记形式的特定主语动词序列的相对频率。仅在较年轻的年龄组中,动词的未标记形式的相对频率才可以预测。这与一个帐户一致,在该帐户中,儿童通过对上下文变得越来越敏感而从错误中恢复,首先是前面的词汇上下文(例如,动词前面的主语)和最终更遥远的语法标记(例如,在问题中位于主题前面的辅助)。研究重点:我们提供了一个语料库分析,调查输入对幼儿动词标记错误的影响(例如,木乃伊去)跨越发展(2至4岁之间)。我们发现有证据表明,这些明显的遗漏错误实际上是输入驱动的委托错误,持续到生命的第三年。我们比较了与上下文无关的错误率的相对影响(例如,动词)和上下文相关(例如,主语+动词序列)跨发展时间的线索。我们的发现支持以下建议:儿童通过对先前的上下文变得越来越敏感,从而从动词标记错误中恢复过来。
    There is substantial evidence that children\'s apparent omission of grammatical morphemes in utterances such as \"She play tennis\" and \"Mummy eating\" is in fact errors of commission in which contextually licensed unmarked forms encountered in the input are reproduced in a context-blind fashion. So how do children stop making such errors? In this study, we test the assumption that children\'s ability to recover from error is related to their developing sensitivity to longer-range dependencies. We use a pre-registered corpus analysis to explore the predictive value of different cues with regards to children\'s verb-marking errors and observe a developmental pattern consistent with this account. We look at context-independent cues (the identity of the specific verb being used) and at the relative value of context-dependent cues (the identity of the specific subject+verb sequence being used). We find that the only consistent effect across a group of 2- to 3-year-olds and a group of 3- to 4-year-olds is the relative frequency of unmarked forms of specific subject+verb sequences being used. The relative frequency of unmarked forms of the verb alone is predictive only in the younger age group. This is consistent with an account in which children recover from making errors by becoming progressively more sensitive to context, at first the immediately preceding lexical contexts (e.g., the subject that precedes the verb) and eventually more distant grammatical markers (e.g., the fronted auxiliary that precedes the subject in questions). RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We provide a corpus analysis investigating input effects on young children\'s verb-marking errors (e.g., Mummy go) across development (between 2 and 4 years of age). We find evidence that these apparent errors of omission are in fact input-driven errors of commission that persist into the third year of life. We compare the relative effect on error rates of context-independent (e.g., verb) and context-dependent (e.g., subject+verb sequence) cues across developmental time. Our findings support the proposal that children recover from making verb-marking errors by becoming progressively more sensitive to preceding context.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    手语经常被描述为具有三个动词类。一,\'同意\'动词,通过修改动词的开始和结束位置来指示其主语和宾语的人/号码。第二个,\'空间\'动词,对动词运动进行类似的出现修改,以表示运动动词主题的源和目标位置。第三类,\'普通\'动词,其特征在于没有这些类型的调制。许多研究人员提出了使所有这些类型崩溃的帐户,或人同意和空间动词。在这里,我们提供了来自美国手语的晚期学习者以及新手语的出现的证据,表明在这些条件下,人协议和位置协议具有不同的地位,我们声称他们的分析应该保持清晰,至少在某些方面。
    Sign languages are frequently described as having three verb classes. One, \'agreeing\' verbs, indicates the person/number of its subject and object by modification of the beginning and ending locations of the verb. The second, \'spatial\' verbs, makes a similar appearing modification of verb movement to represent the source and goal locations of the theme of a verb of motion. The third class, \'plain\' verbs, is characterized as having neither of these types of modulations. A number of researchers have proposed accounts that collapse all of these types, or the person-agreeing and spatial verbs. Here we present evidence from late learners of American Sign Language and from the emergence of new sign languages that person agreement and locative agreement have a different status in these conditions, and we claim their analysis should be kept distinct, at least in certain ways.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    幼儿在最初几年的快速词汇增长是由与照顾者的社交互动中的输入支持的,越来越多,从数字媒体。然而,两种来源的暴露量在不同的社会经济类别中可能会有很大差异,人们对社交互动和数字媒体如何一起使用来预测生命最初几年的词汇量知之甚少。当前的研究迈出了第一步,以检查与其他个人的社交互动增加是否可以缓冲数字媒体使用对社会经济多样化样本中语言的潜在不利影响。305名17至30个月儿童的照顾者完成了关于他们家庭人口统计数据的问卷,他们孩子的技术使用,以及孩子的日常生活和社交互动。研究结果表明,与社交更多,使用更少技术的同龄人相比,经历较少的人际交往和更多技术接触的孩子的词汇量较小,随着孩子年龄的增长,这种差距变得越来越大。此外,社交互动的数量缓和了SES之间的联系,数字媒体,和词汇,使数字媒体对低SES家庭儿童词汇的负面影响可以通过增加的社交互动来抵消。一起,这表明,在数字媒体使用突出的世界中,增加人类互动的数量可能成为词汇结果的保护因素。
    Young children\'s rapid vocabulary growth during the first few years is supported by input during social interactions with caregivers and, increasingly, from digital media. However, the amount of exposure to both sources can vary substantially across socioeconomic classes, and little is known about how social interactions and digital media use together predict vocabulary in the first few years of life. The current study takes a first step toward examining whether increased social interactions with other individuals may buffer the potentially detrimental effects of digital media use on language among a socioeconomically diverse sample. 305 caregivers of children between 17 and 30-months completed questionnaires about their family demographics, their child\'s technology use, and the child\'s daily routines and social interactions. Findings suggest children who experience fewer human interactions and greater technology exposure have smaller vocabularies than their peers who socialize more and use less technology, and this disparity becomes greater as children get older. Moreover, the number of social interactions moderates the link between SES, digital media, and vocabulary such that the negative impact of digital media on vocabulary for children from low SES households can be offset with increased social interactions. Together, this suggests that increasing the amount of human interactions may serve as a protective factor for vocabulary outcomes in a world where digital media use is prominent.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    语言障碍,它们仍然很难被检测到,经常出现在受虐待的儿童身上。虽然后果是众所周知的,而且是持久的,对这些孩子的发育和具体特征知之甚少,取决于它们被放置在哪里,他们遭受的虐待类型和他们所处的年龄。这一发现导致了对文献的回顾,旨在更好地定义关于该主题的知识状态,为了更好的检测和治疗。
    Language disorders, which are still very poorly detected, are often present in abused children. While the consequences are well known and long-lasting, little is known about the development and specific characteristics of these children, depending on where they were placed, the type of abuse they suffered and the age at which they were placed. This finding led to a review of the literature aimed at better defining the state of knowledge on the subject, for the benefit of better detection and treatment.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究旨在调查约旦阿拉伯语的六个喉音辅音的语音发展,/χ/,//,//,//,//,和/h/。
    一种衔接测试旨在涉及两个任务:图片命名和重复。测试包括54个单词的图片命名和18个单词的重复,代表目标喉音的所有可能位置。样本是从40名通常发育中的安玛尼-约旦阿拉伯语单语儿童中收集的,住在安曼,乔丹。受访者平均分为八个与年龄相关的轨迹:2-2;6,2;6-3,3-3;6,3;6-4,4-4;6,4;6-5,5-5;6和5;6-6(岁;月)。没有有听力史的孩子,演讲,或包括视力障碍。使用生产精度分析数据,其中生产的三个发展轨迹(习惯,收购,和掌握)是为每个喉部确定的,和错误分析,基于感知判断,提供每个发音错误或删除的喉音细节。
    结果表明/χ/,//,//,和//在6岁之前获得,而//和/h/仍然没有获得这个年龄。受访者对发音错误的同源词使用相对不同的替代方案,包括喉部,非喉音,和元音替换。//是替代数量最多的喉音,而/↔/得到的最少。分析还揭示了喉部缺失的模式,随着不同的喉音和位置的变化。尽管有错误/偏差,受访者得分的准确度百分比随着年龄的相关性逐渐增加。尾音//以最低精度百分比开始,而//和//以最高开始。
    这些发现阐明了喉获得的发展轨迹,丰富了我们对儿童不断发展的感知和生产能力的理解。他们提供了对约旦阿拉伯语儿童的喉音产生模式的宝贵见解,为进一步研究和制定有针对性的评估和干预策略奠定基础,以支持该人群的语音发展。
    UNASSIGNED: This study aims at investigating the phonological development of the six guttural consonants of Jordanian Arabic, /χ/, /ʁ/, /ħ/, /ʕ/, /ʔ/, and /h/.
    UNASSIGNED: An articulation test is designed to involve two tasks: picture naming and repetition. The test includes 54 words for picture naming and 18 words for repetition, representing all possible positions of the targeted guttural sounds. Samples are collected from 40 typically developing Ammani-Jordanian Arabic-speaking monolingual children, living in Amman, Jordan. Respondents are equally divided into eight age-related trajectories: 2-2;6, 2;6-3, 3-3;6, 3;6-4, 4-4;6, 4;6-5, 5-5;6, and 5;6-6 (years;months). No child with a history of hearing, speech, or vision disorders is included. The data are analyzed using production accuracy, where the three developmental trajectories of production (customary, acquisition, and mastery) are determined for each guttural, and error analysis, addressed based on perceptual judgments, providing details of every mispronounced or deleted guttural.
    UNASSIGNED: The results show that /χ/, /ħ/, /ʕ/, and /ʔ/ are acquired before the age of 6 years, while /ʁ/ and /h/ are still not acquired by this age. Respondents use relatively variant alternatives for the mispronounced cognates, including guttural, nonguttural sounds, and vowel substitution. The /ʁ/ is the guttural with the highest number of alternatives, while /ʔ/ gets the least. The analysis also reveals patterns of guttural deletion, with variations across different guttural sounds and positions. Despite errors/deviations made, respondents score accuracy percentages that gradually increase in correlation with age. The guttural /ʁ/ starts with the lowest accuracy percentages, while /ʔ/ and /ħ/ start with the highest.
    UNASSIGNED: These findings illuminate on the developmental trajectory of guttural acquisition and enrich our understanding of children\'s evolving perception and production abilities. They offer valuable insights into the patterns of guttural sound production in Jordanian Arabic-speaking children, laying the groundwork for further research and the development of targeted assessment and intervention strategies to support phonological development in this population.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号