semantic processing

语义处理
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    精神分裂症的特点是复杂的言语和手势的异常处理,这可能在功能上导致其社交障碍。迄今为止,现存的精神分裂症神经科学研究主要是孤立地研究言语和手势功能失调,以前没有研究过这两种交流渠道在更自然的环境中如何相互作用。这里,我们测试了精神分裂症患者是否表现出语义上复杂的故事片段的异常神经处理,以及语音关联手势(协同语音手势)是否可以调节这种效果。在一项功能磁共振成像研究中,我们向34名参与者(16名患者和18名匹配对照)展示了一个连续故事的生态有效复述,通过语音和自发手势执行。我们把整个故事分成十个字的片段,并用思想密度测量每个片段的语义复杂性,临床上通常用于在语义水平上评估异常语言功能障碍的语言度量。每个细分市场,手势数量的存在不同(n=0,1,+2)。我们的研究结果表明,与控件相比,患者在双侧中额叶和下顶叶区域的更复杂节段的激活减少.重要的是,这种神经异常在手势显示的片段中被归一化。因此,第一次使用自然主义的多模态刺激范式,我们展示了手势在处理自然故事时减少了群体差异,可能是通过促进精神分裂症故事的语义复杂部分的处理。
    Schizophrenia is marked by aberrant processing of complex speech and gesture, which may contribute functionally to its impaired social communication. To date, extant neuroscientific studies of schizophrenia have largely investigated dysfunctional speech and gesture in isolation, and no prior research has examined how the two communicative channels may interact in more natural contexts. Here, we tested if patients with schizophrenia show aberrant neural processing of semantically complex story segments, and if speech-associated gestures (co-speech gestures) might modulate this effect. In a functional MRI study, we presented to 34 participants (16 patients and 18 matched-controls) an ecologically-valid retelling of a continuous story, performed via speech and spontaneous gestures. We split the entire story into ten-word segments, and measured the semantic complexity for each segment with idea density, a linguistic measure that is commonly used clinically to evaluate aberrant language dysfunction at the semantic level. Per segment, the presence of numbers of gestures varied (n = 0, 1, +2). Our results suggest that, in comparison to controls, patients showed reduced activation for more complex segments in the bilateral middle frontal and inferior parietal regions. Importantly, this neural aberrance was normalized in segments presented with gestures. Thus, for the first time with a naturalistic multimodal stimulation paradigm, we show that gestures reduced group differences when processing a natural story, probably by facilitating the processing of semantically complex segments of the story in schizophrenia.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当前的研究调查了表情符号在书面语境中作为句子谓词处理的神经机制。在更具认知吸引力的混合语态中,句子中间位置的表情符号被设计为与上下文一致或不一致。结果表明,不一致的单词导致了强烈的N400效应,而不一致的表情符号仅引起P600效应。这意味着单词的语义和语法可以分开,而表情符号的语义和语法似乎可以集成在一起。也就是说,当表情符号的含义违反了句子上下文时,它的语法作用不能很好地解释,尤其是当它被用作句子中的关键语法成分时,如谓词。因此,它表明,即使表情符号的含义可以被读者解释,它们的句法和语义功能不能明确分开。与文字处理相比,在350-500毫秒的时间窗口中,表情符号的振幅更大,在表情符号语义处理中显示出更多的认知努力,可能是由于视觉通道内的模态转换引起的,也就是说,多模态认知负荷。
    The current study investigated the neuro mechanisms of emoji processing as sentence predicate in written context. In the hybrid textuality which is more cognitively engaging, emojis in sentential intermediate positions were designed as either congruent or incongruent to the context. The results showed that incongruent words led to a robust N400 effect, while incongruent emojis only elicited the P600 effect. It implies that semantics and syntax of words can be separated while those of emojis seem to be integrated together. That is, when the meaning of the emoji is violated to the sentential context, its grammatical role cannot be well interpreted, especially when it is used as a key grammatical component in a sentence, such as the predicate. Thus, it shows that even though the meaning of emojis can be interpreted by readers, their syntactic and semantic functions cannot be clearly separated. In comparison with word processing, the larger amplitude with emojis in the time window of 350-500 ms shows more cognitive efforts in emoji semantic processing, possibly arising from the switch of modalities within the visual channel, that is, the multimodal cognitive load.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人类可以快速阅读和理解文本,暗示读者可能会处理每个固定的多个单词。然而,在何种程度上,半心词被预览和整合到不断发展的句子上下文仍然存在争议。我们通过使用MEG和眼动仪记录大脑活动和眼球运动,研究了自然阅读过程中的旁凹处理,而参与者则默默地阅读一行句子。句子包含一个不可预测的目标单词,该单词与句子上下文一致或不一致。为了测量半凹加工,我们以60Hz闪烁目标单词,并测量由此产生的大脑反应(即快速隐形频率标记,RIFT)在对预目标单词的注视期间。我们的结果表明,与相同的目标单词相比,与先前上下文不一致的目标单词的标记响应显着较弱,甚至在100ms内固定的单词紧前面的目标。还发现RIFT反应的这种降低可以预测个体的阅读速度。我们得出的结论是,语义信息不仅可以从parafovea中提取,而且可以在固定单词之前与先前的上下文集成。这种早期和广泛的半凹处理支持自然阅读所需的快速文字处理。我们的研究表明,自然阅读的理论框架应纳入深的副凹加工的概念。
    Humans can read and comprehend text rapidly, implying that readers might process multiple words per fixation. However, the extent to which parafoveal words are previewed and integrated into the evolving sentence context remains disputed. We investigated parafoveal processing during natural reading by recording brain activity and eye movements using MEG and an eye tracker while participants silently read one-line sentences. The sentences contained an unpredictable target word that was either congruent or incongruent with the sentence context. To measure parafoveal processing, we flickered the target words at 60 Hz and measured the resulting brain responses (i.e. Rapid Invisible Frequency Tagging, RIFT) during fixations on the pre-target words. Our results revealed a significantly weaker tagging response for target words that were incongruent with the previous context compared to congruent ones, even within 100ms of fixating the word immediately preceding the target. This reduction in the RIFT response was also found to be predictive of individual reading speed. We conclude that semantic information is not only extracted from the parafovea but can also be integrated with the previous context before the word is fixated. This early and extensive parafoveal processing supports the rapid word processing required for natural reading. Our study suggests that theoretical frameworks of natural reading should incorporate the concept of deep parafoveal processing.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们建立意义的方式不仅在语言研究中而且在发展科学中都是一个深刻的问题。在最近的语用交际方法中,语言形式和内容之间的关系已经放松,表明基于代码的语言理解模型必须通过上下文敏感来增强,恢复说话者意图意义的语用推理机制。传统上,语言习得被认为涉及建立心理词典并从嘈杂的语言输入中提取句法规则,而交际-语用推论也被认为是不可或缺的。最近的研究发现探索语义加工的电生理指标,N400提出了关于语义解码和语用推理过程之间传统分离的严重问题。N400似乎从其发展开始就对心智化敏感-将信念归因于社会伙伴的能力。这一发现提出了这样一种可能性,即思维化可能不仅有助于丰富语言解码过程的语用推理,而且语义系统可能以从根本上的思维方式运作。本综述首先总结了语用交际模式对语言理解的主要贡献。然后,它概述了交际意图如何在交际发展理论中被解释,特别强调心智化。接下来,它讨论了婴儿对语言信息传播潜力的敏感性,他们能够获得类似代码的功能,以及他们使用心智化跟踪社会伙伴语言理解的能力。总之,我认为,语言交流过程中意义的恢复并未充分建模为基于代码的语义检索过程,并辅之以语用推理。相反,语义系统可以建立意义,按照预期,在语言理解和习得过程中,通过对交际伙伴的内容进行心理归因。
    The way we establish meaning has been a profound question not only in language research but in developmental science as well. The relation between linguistic form and content has been loosened up in recent pragmatic approaches to communication, showing that code-based models of language comprehension must be augmented by context-sensitive, pragmatic-inferential mechanisms to recover the speaker\'s intended meaning. Language acquisition has traditionally been thought to involve building a mental lexicon and extracting syntactic rules from noisy linguistic input, while communicative-pragmatic inferences have also been argued to be indispensable. Recent research findings exploring the electrophysiological indicator of semantic processing, the N400, have raised serious questions about the traditional separation between semantic decoding and pragmatic inferential processes. The N400 appears to be sensitive to mentalization-the ability to attribute beliefs to social partners-already from its developmental onset. This finding raises the possibility that mentalization may not simply contribute to pragmatic inferences that enrich linguistic decoding processes but that the semantic system may be functioning in a fundamentally mentalistic manner. The present review first summarizes the key contributions of pragmatic models of communication to language comprehension. Then, it provides an overview of how communicative intentions are interpreted in developmental theories of communication, with a special emphasis on mentalization. Next, it discusses the sensitivity of infants to the information-transmitting potential of language, their ability to pick up its code-like features, and their capacity to track language comprehension of social partners using mentalization. In conclusion, I argue that the recovery of meaning during linguistic communication is not adequately modeled as a process of code-based semantic retrieval complemented by pragmatic inferences. Instead, the semantic system may establish meaning, as intended, during language comprehension and acquisition through mentalistic attribution of content to communicative partners.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究研究了在视觉上理解实时口语单词的过程中,患有发展语言障碍(DLD)的儿童的词汇语义处理。现有证据表明,患有DLD的儿童可能会在词汇访问和检索方面遇到挑战,与具有典型发展(TD)的同行相比,词汇竞争也更大。然而,这些困难的具体性质尚不清楚。使用眼动追踪方法,该研究调查了DLD儿童及其年龄匹配的同龄人对语义关系的实时理解。结果显示,对于相对频繁的名词,两组对语义关系的理解相似。当语义竞争者出现时,两组都偏爱语义竞争者。反过来,当语义竞争者与口语单词的视觉参照一起出现时,这两个团体都无视竞争对手。这一发现表明,尽管患有DLD的儿童通常词汇相对贫乏,频繁的名词可能不会给他们带来更大的困难。虽然两组之间对竞争对手或对象的偏好的时间过程相似,数值,虽然不重要,观察到簇的延伸差异。总之,这项研究表明,与使用TD的同龄人相比,使用DLD的单语学龄前儿童对频繁单词的词汇访问相似。未来的研究应该调查DLD儿童在频率较低的单词上的表现,以全面了解他们的词汇语义能力。
    This study examined lexical-semantic processing in children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) during visually situated comprehension of real-time spoken words. Existing evidence suggests that children with DLD may experience challenges in lexical access and retrieval, as well as greater lexical competition compared to their peers with Typical Development (TD). However, the specific nature of these difficulties remains unclear. Using eye-tracking methodology, the study investigated the real-time comprehension of semantic relationships in children with DLD and their age-matched peers. The results revealed that, for relatively frequent nouns, both groups demonstrated similar comprehension of semantic relationships. Both groups favored the semantic competitor when it appeared with an unrelated visual referent. In turn, when the semantic competitor appeared with the visual referent of the spoken word, both groups disregarded the competitor. This finding shows that, although children with DLD usually present a relatively impoverished vocabulary, frequent nouns may not pose greater difficulties for them. While the temporal course of preference for the competitor or the referent was similar between the two groups, numerical, though non-significant, differences in the extension of the clusters were observed. In summary, this research demonstrates that monolingual preschoolers with DLD exhibit similar lexical access to frequent words compared to their peers with TD. Future studies should investigate the performance of children with DLD on less frequent words to provide a comprehensive understanding of their lexical-semantic abilities.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们调查了是否,在视觉单词识别过程中,语义处理由注意力控制机制调节,该机制旨在将语义信息与任务相关目标相匹配。在以前的研究中,我们分析了语义Stroop干扰作为反应潜伏期的函数(delta-plot分析),发现这种现象主要发生在最慢的反应中.这里,在这些最慢的试验中,我们调查了这种模式是否由于主动维持任务目标的能力降低.在两对实验中,参与者完成了两个语义Stroop任务:经典语义Stroop任务(实验1A和2A)和语义Stroop任务结合n-back任务(实验1B和2B)。这两对实验只是试验速度不同,这在实验2A和2B中比在实验1A和1B中略快。通过对执行控制系统征税,预计n-back任务会阻碍主动控制。语义Stroop任务的Delta图分析在最慢的响应中复制了增强的效果,但只有在足够的时间压力下。将语义Stroop任务与n-back任务相结合,产生了语义Stroop干扰的分布轮廓的变化,我们认为这是使用主动控制的普遍困难。我们的研究结果表明,语义Stroop干扰是,在某种程度上,取决于可用的执行资源,同时对任务条件的细微变化也很敏感。
    We investigated whether, during visual word recognition, semantic processing is modulated by attentional control mechanisms directed at matching semantic information with task-relevant goals. In previous research, we analyzed the semantic Stroop interference as a function of response latency (delta-plot analyses) and found that this phenomenon mainly occurs in the slowest responses. Here, we investigated whether this pattern is due to reduced ability to proactively maintain the task goal in these slowest trials. In two pairs of experiments, participants completed two semantic Stroop tasks: a classic semantic Stroop task (Experiment 1A and 2A) and a semantic Stroop task combined with an n-back task (Experiment 1B and 2B). The two pairs of experiments only differed in the trial pace, which was slightly faster in Experiments 2A and 2B than in Experiments 1A and 1B. By taxing the executive control system, the n-back task was expected to hinder proactive control. Delta-plot analyses of the semantic Stroop task replicated the enhanced effect in the slowest responses, but only under sufficient time pressure. Combining the semantic Stroop task with the n-back task produced a change in the distributional profile of semantic Stroop interference, which we ascribe to a general difficulty in the use of proactive control. Our findings suggest that semantic Stroop interference is, to some extent, dependent on the available executive resources, while also being sensitive to subtle variations in task conditions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Systematic Review
    瞳孔扩张与执行各种认知任务所需的努力有关。在词汇层面,一些研究表明,这种神经生理学措施将提供客观的,在文字处理和词汇访问过程中的实时信息。然而,由于这一研究领域的稀缺和起步,其适用性,使用,敏感性并不完全清楚。本范围审查旨在通过提供该领域研究的最新概述,确定瞳孔测量法在词汇访问研究中的适用性和实用性。按照PRISMA协议,这篇综述包括16篇文章。结果表明,有用的,和评估单词识别的词汇技能的敏感方法,单词检索,和语义激活。此外,它很容易适应该领域的传统研究范式和方法。因为它是非侵入性的,目标,和自动化程序,它可以适用于任何人口或年龄组。然而,这一特定研究领域的新兴发展和纳入研究中观察到的方法多样性尚不允许在这一领域得出明确的结论,这反过来又不允许荟萃分析或关于处理单词时瞳孔反应实际反映的完全结论性陈述。需要定义标准化的瞳孔记录和分析方法,以生成更准确的,具有更可靠结果的可复制研究设计,以加强这一研究路线。
    Pupil dilation has been associated with the effort required to perform various cognitive tasks. At the lexical level, some studies suggest that this neurophysiological measure would provide objective, real-time information during word processing and lexical access. However, due to the scarcity and incipient advancement of this line of research, its applicability, use, and sensitivity are not entirely clear. This scoping review aims to determine the applicability and usefulness of pupillometry in the study of lexical access by providing an up-to-date overview of research in this area. Following the PRISMA protocol, 16 articles were included in this review. The results show that pupillometry is a highly applicable, useful, and sensitive method for assessing lexical skills of word recognition, word retrieval, and semantic activation. Moreover, it easily fits into traditional research paradigms and methods in the field. Because it is a non-invasive, objective, and automated procedure, it can be applied to any population or age group. However, the emerging development of this specific area of research and the methodological diversity observed in the included studies do not yet allow for definitive conclusions in this area, which in turn does not allow for meta-analyses or fully conclusive statements about what the pupil response actually reflects when processing words. Standardized pupillary recording and analysis methods need to be defined to generate more accurate, replicable research designs with more reliable results to strengthen this line of research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    语义表示的中枢理论将语义知识的神经基础分为两个基本组成部分:感觉运动模态特定区域和至关重要的语义中枢区域。我们先前在语义痴呆患者中的研究发现,枢纽区域位于左梭状回。然而,因为这个区域位于语义痴呆患者的脑损伤范围内,尚不清楚语义缺陷是由枢纽区域本身的结构损伤还是由其与其他大脑区域的断开引起的。中风患者左梭状回没有任何损伤,但在语义处理中表现出模态和模态特异性缺陷。因此,在这项研究中,我们从脑网络角度对79例卒中患者的语义中枢区域进行了验证,并探讨了与之相关的白质连接.首先,我们收集了一般语义任务和模态特定语义任务的弥散加权成像和行为表现数据(评估形式上的对象知识,颜色,动议,声音,操纵和功能)。然后,我们使用相关性和回归分析来检查中风患者全脑结构网络中脑区域的节点程度值与一般语义表现之间的关联。结果表明,左梭状回的连通性显着预测一般语义表现,表明该区域是语义中心。要识别语义中心的语义相关连接,然后,我们将连接到左梭状回的每个束的白质完整性值分别与一般和特定于模态的语义处理性能相关联。我们发现,中枢区域通过与左上颞极的白质连接来完成一般语义处理,颞中回,颞下回和海马。中枢区域和左侧海马体之间的连通性,颞上极,颞中回,颞下回和海马旁回在物体形式上有差异,颜色,动议,声音,操作和功能处理。在统计上消除了潜在混杂变量的影响(即全脑病变体积,感兴趣区域的病变体积和在非语义控制任务上的表现),观察到的效果仍然显著。一起,我们的研究结果支持左梭状回作为卒中患者的语义枢纽区域的作用,并揭示了其在网络中的关键连通性.本研究为人脑语义记忆的神经解剖学组织提供了新的见解和证据。
    The hub-and-spoke theory of semantic representation fractionates the neural underpinning of semantic knowledge into two essential components: the sensorimotor modality-specific regions and a crucially important semantic hub region. Our previous study in patients with semantic dementia has found that the hub region is located in the left fusiform gyrus. However, because this region is located within the brain damage in patients with semantic dementia, it is not clear whether the semantic deficit is caused by structural damage to the hub region itself or by its disconnection from other brain regions. Stroke patients do not have any damage to the left fusiform gyrus, but exhibit amodal and modality-specific deficits in semantic processing. Therefore, in this study, we validated the semantic hub region from a brain network perspective in 79 stroke patients and explored the white matter connections associated with it. First, we collected data of diffusion-weighted imaging and behavioural performance on general semantic tasks and modality-specific semantic tasks (assessing object knowledge on form, colour, motion, sound, manipulation and function). We then used correlation and regression analyses to examine the association between the nodal degree values of brain regions in the whole-brain structural network and general semantic performance in the stroke patients. The results revealed that the connectivity of the left fusiform gyrus significantly predicted general semantic performance, indicating that this region is the semantic hub. To identify the semantic-relevant connections of the semantic hub, we then correlated the white matter integrity values of each tract connected to the left fusiform gyrus separately with performance on general and modality-specific semantic processing. We found that the hub region accomplished general semantic processing through white matter connections with the left superior temporal pole, middle temporal gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus and hippocampus. The connectivity between the hub region and the left hippocampus, superior temporal pole, middle temporal gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus and parahippocampal gyrus was differentially involved in object form, colour, motion, sound, manipulation and function processing. After statistically removing the effects of potential confounding variables (i.e. whole-brain lesion volume, lesion volume of regions of interest and performance on non-semantic control tasks), the observed effects remained significant. Together, our findings support the role of the left fusiform gyrus as a semantic hub region in stroke patients and reveal its crucial connectivity in the network. This study provides new insights and evidence for the neuroanatomical organization of semantic memory in the human brain.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    后皮质萎缩(PCA)是一种综合征,其特征是高阶视觉空间处理逐渐下降,导致空间感知缺陷等症状,同时失认症,和对象感知障碍。虽然PCA主要因其对视觉空间能力的影响而闻名,最近的研究记录了PCA患者的语言异常。本研究旨在描述PCA中语言障碍的性质和起源,假设语言障碍反映了疾病的视觉空间处理障碍。
    我们比较了25名PCA患者的语言样本与年龄匹配的认知正常(CN)个体在两个不同的任务中的语言样本:视觉依赖的图片描述和视觉独立的工作描述任务。我们提取了词频,单词发音延迟,和空间关系词进行比较。然后,我们对图片描述任务中使用的语言进行了深入分析,以确定反映PCA视觉空间处理缺陷的特定语言指标。
    PCA患者在视觉依赖性任务中表现出明显的语言障碍,以更高的单词频率为特征,延长的话语延迟,更少的空间关系词,但不是在视觉独立的任务中。对图片描述任务的深入分析进一步表明,PCA患者难以识别某些视觉元素以及图片的整体主题。基于这些语言特征的预测模型以很高的分类精度将PCA患者与CN个体区分开来。
    研究结果表明,语言是一种敏感的行为结构,可以检测PCA的视觉空间处理异常。这些见解为理解和管理PCA提供了理论和临床途径,强调语言是阿尔茨海默病这种非典型变体的视觉空间缺陷的关键标记。
    UNASSIGNED: Posterior Cortical Atrophy (PCA) is a syndrome characterized by a progressive decline in higher-order visuospatial processing, leading to symptoms such as space perception deficit, simultanagnosia, and object perception impairment. While PCA is primarily known for its impact on visuospatial abilities, recent studies have documented language abnormalities in PCA patients. This study aims to delineate the nature and origin of language impairments in PCA, hypothesizing that language deficits reflect the visuospatial processing impairments of the disease.
    UNASSIGNED: We compared the language samples of 25 patients with PCA with age-matched cognitively normal (CN) individuals across two distinct tasks: a visually-dependent picture description and a visually-independent job description task. We extracted word frequency, word utterance latency, and spatial relational words for this comparison. We then conducted an in-depth analysis of the language used in the picture description task to identify specific linguistic indicators that reflect the visuospatial processing deficits of PCA.
    UNASSIGNED: Patients with PCA showed significant language deficits in the visually-dependent task, characterized by higher word frequency, prolonged utterance latency, and fewer spatial relational words, but not in the visually-independent task. An in-depth analysis of the picture description task further showed that PCA patients struggled to identify certain visual elements as well as the overall theme of the picture. A predictive model based on these language features distinguished PCA patients from CN individuals with high classification accuracy.
    UNASSIGNED: The findings indicate that language is a sensitive behavioral construct to detect visuospatial processing abnormalities of PCA. These insights offer theoretical and clinical avenues for understanding and managing PCA, underscoring language as a crucial marker for the visuospatial deficits of this atypical variant of Alzheimer\'s disease.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    有一种误解,认为图片很容易理解,这在包括图片的教学实践中是有问题的。例如,如果一个孩子对图片序列的口头叙述有困难,它可能被解释为特定的口语,即使孩子可能有额外的困难,理解视觉叙事的形式的图片序列。因此,本研究的目的是增加我们对图形领域的语义处理与语言领域的语义处理的理解,重点是9-13岁具有典型语言发展的儿童。为此,我们测量了脑电反应(事件相关电位,ERPs)在17名儿童中,以(i)在传达视觉叙事的面板序列中预测与未预测的图片(面板),以及(ii)在传达口头叙事的句子中预测与未预测的单词。结果显示了相似性,因为在跨域的N400效应的大小上没有显著差异。域之间的唯一差异是预测的分布差异,也就是说,在语言领域比在图形领域更后验的N400效应。这项研究有助于加深对视觉叙事处理的复杂性及其与言语叙事处理的共同特征的理解,这应该在教学实践中加以考虑。
    There is a misconception that pictures are easy to comprehend, which is problematic in pedagogical practices that include pictures. For example, if a child has difficulties with verbal narration to picture sequences, it may be interpreted as specific to spoken language even though the child may have additional difficulties with comprehension of visual narratives in the form of picture sequences. The purpose of the present study was therefore to increase our understanding of semantic processing in the pictorial domain in relation to semantic processing in the verbal domain, focusing on 9-13 years-old children with typical language development. To this end, we measured electrical brain responses (event related potentials, ERPs) in 17 children to (i) pictures (panels) that were predicted versus unpredicted in sequences of panels that conveyed visual narratives and (ii) words that were predicted versus unpredicted in sentences that conveyed verbal narratives. Results demonstrated similarities as there were no significant difference in the magnitude of the N400 effect across domains. The only difference between domains was the predicted difference in distribution, that is, a more posterior N400 effect in the verbal domain than in the pictorial domain. The study contributes to an increased understanding of the complexity of processing of visual narratives and its shared features with processing of verbal narratives, which should be considered in pedagogical practices.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号