emotions

情绪
  • DOI:
    文章类型: English Abstract
    背景:关注应对策略的研究,特别是在平民人质中,是scant。研究结果可能有助于预测从囚禁返回后重新调整的机会。本研究旨在研究平民人质在被囚禁期间面临的挑战以及他们如何应对被囚禁的条件。该研究基于对首次释放人质时从哈马斯被囚禁返回的公民进行的17次采访。所有采访均由经验丰富的记者进行,并在11.12.2024和4.1.2024之间在主要电视网络上播出。使用归纳主题分析确定了两个主要主题-囚禁中的困难(例如,处理不确定性,依赖关系,隔离的条件,和无聊)。第二个主题包括应对策略(例如,希望,重新获得控制权,建立确定性,等。).在每个主题中,确定了几个详细说明囚禁困难和处理这些困难的子主题。此外,从囚禁中返回的公民使用两种应对策略描述:以问题为中心,其目的是通过“移除”或改变压力因素来解决问题(例如,规划,建筑确定性),以情绪为中心的应对,其目的是减少或消除创伤后出现的情绪压力和情绪(如分心,呼吁信仰,幽默,等。).主题揭示了在试图了解情况的同时试图重新获得资源的尝试,解释它,减少情绪压力,并在威胁的情况下有效地采取行动,以使生存成为可能。
    BACKGROUND: Studies that focus on coping strategies, especially among civilian hostages, are scant. Research findings may help predict the chances of readjustment upon return from captivity. The present study aimed to examine the challenges that the civilian hostages faced during captivity and how they dealt with the conditions of captivity. The study was based on seventeen interviews conducted with citizens who returned from Hamas captivity in the first release of hostages. All interviews were conducted by experienced journalists and were broadcast on the main television networks between 11.12.2024 and 4.1.2024. Two main themes were identified using inductive thematic analysis-difficulties in captivity (e.g., dealing with uncertainty, dependent relationships, conditions of isolation, and boredom). The second theme included coping strategies (e.g., hope, regaining control, building certainty, etc.). In each of these themes, several sub-themes that detail the difficulties in captivity and the ways of dealing with them were identified. In addition, the citizens who returned from captivity described using two coping strategies: problem-focused, which aims to manage the problem by \"removing\" or changing the stressful factor (e.g., planning, building certainty), and emotion-focused coping, which aims to reduce or eliminate the emotional stress and emotions that arise following the trauma (such as distraction, appeal to beliefs, humor, etc.). The themes revealed attempts to regain resources while trying to understand the situation, interpret it, reduce the emotional stress, and act in threatening situations effectively in order to make it possible to survive.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • DOI:
    文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:我们提供了一个案例研究,涉及从10月事件开始的医生暴露于持续创伤压力的情绪应对7,2023年及接下来的几个月。
    背景:在过去的几个月中,由于加沙边界的持续冲突,以色列南部索罗卡医疗中心的医务人员面临着前所未有的挑战。自2023年10月7日以来,已有2,550多名受伤患者在Soroka接受了不同程度的伤害治疗。超过250个需要拯救生命的手术。作为该地区最大的创伤中心,索罗卡的工作人员经历了持续的创伤压力,痛苦,痛苦和悲伤-无论是在医院内还是在他们的个人生活中。
    结论:虽然长期影响仍有待观察,初步发现表明,在常规过程中的组织努力可能有助于减轻在紧急情况下暴露于创伤应激的潜在负面影响。需要进一步的研究来了解累积暴露如何与恢复和生长过程相互作用。
    结论:当前的报告介绍了创伤暴露的不同层次,相关的风险和弹性因素,以及在当前战争爆发前几年在医院灌输的观念,这种观念促进了一种关于情感困难的主动同伴话语文化,并在需要时将专业帮助转诊正常化。我们还介绍了许多员工和组织反应中常见的应对行为,考虑到当前挑战的独特性,他们的范围和持续时间。
    BACKGROUND: We present a case study dealing with the emotional coping of a physician\'s exposure to continuous traumatic stress starting with the events of Oct. 7, 2023 and during the following months.
    BACKGROUND: The medical staff at Soroka Medical Center in southern Israel have faced unprecedented challenges over the past months due to ongoing conflict along the Gaza border. Since October 7, 2023, over 2,550 wounded patients have been treated at Soroka for injuries of varying severity, with more than 250 requiring life-saving surgeries. As the region\'s largest trauma center, Soroka\'s staff have endured continuous exposure to traumatic stress, suffering, distress and grief - both within the hospital and in their personal lives.
    CONCLUSIONS: While the long-term impacts remain to be seen, preliminary findings suggest that organizational efforts during routine may help mitigate potential negative effects of exposure to traumatic stress in the course of emergency. Further research is needed to understand how cumulative exposure interacts with process of recovery and growth.
    CONCLUSIONS: The current report presents the different layers of traumatic exposure, the associated risk and resilience factors and a perception that was instilled in the hospital in the years preceding the current war which promoted a culture of proactive peer discourse about emotional hardship and normalization of referral for professional help when needed. We also present coping behaviors that were common among many staff members and organizational responses, considering the uniqueness of the current challenges, their scope and duration.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:非自杀性自我伤害(NSSI)的著名理论提出,行为的特征是放大的情绪反应。然而,关于自我伤害的人在情感挑战中的反应知之甚少。
    方法:我们测量了主观和生理反应(心率,心率变异性,和皮肤电反应)在过去一年的NSSI(n=51)和在静息基线期间没有终生NSSI(n=50)的年轻人中,应力诱导,和压力后休息阶段。参与者报告了他们在应激后阶段自发使用认知重新评估和表达抑制的程度。两周后,样本的一个子集(n=42)报告了他们在实验室期间的记忆感觉。
    结果:尽管NSSI组报告的情绪失调明显高于对照组,两组都表现出相似的主观和心理反应,和恢复,情感挑战。两组在急性应激后使用了相似程度的重新评估和抑制调节策略,后来以类似的方式记住了情感挑战。
    结论:在NSSI组中,过去一年的自我伤害往往是罕见和零星的。只有43.6%的样本参与了评估情绪挑战记忆的后续调查。
    结论:研究结果表明,情绪在NSSI中的作用比突出的理论所能解释的要复杂得多,提出了关于NSSI情绪性质的实质性问题。需要更全面地了解情绪在NSSI中的作用,以告知干预策略,以更好地支持自我伤害的人。
    BACKGROUND: Prominent theories of nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) propose that the behaviour is characterised by amplified emotional responses. However, little is known about how people who self-injure respond during emotional challenge.
    METHODS: We measured subjective and physiological responding (heart rate, heart rate variability, and electrodermal responding) among young adults with past-year NSSI (n = 51) and those with no lifetime NSSI (n = 50) during a resting baseline, a stress induction, and a post-stress resting phase. Participants reported the extent to which they spontaneously used cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression during the post-stress phase. Two weeks later, a subset of the sample (n = 42) reported how they remembered feeling during the laboratory session.
    RESULTS: Although the NSSI group reported considerably greater emotion dysregulation than Controls, both groups showed similar subjective and psychological reactivity to, and recovery from, emotional challenge. Both groups used reappraisal and suppression regulation strategies following acute stress to a similar extent, and later came to remember the emotional challenge in a similar manner.
    CONCLUSIONS: Within the NSSI group, past-year self-injury tended to be infrequent and sporadic. Only 43.6% of the sample participated in the follow-up survey assessing memory of emotional challenge.
    CONCLUSIONS: Findings demonstrate that the role of emotion in NSSI is more complex than prominent theories can account for, raising substantial questions regarding the nature of emotion in NSSI. A more comprehensive understanding of the role of emotion in NSSI is needed to inform intervention strategies to better support people who self-injure.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:在普通儿科人群中,听觉-言语幻觉经历(AVH)的患病率为12%。文献报道创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)发生AVH的风险较高。AVHs在青春期的持续存在代表了进化为精神病的风险。社会认知和情感标记可以被认为是这种进化的前驱标记。这项前瞻性观察性研究的目的是观察社会认知和情绪标志物与两年内AVH的存在和持续以及PTSD和精神病诊断的演变的相关性。
    方法:本前瞻性病例对照研究,纵向超过两年(在六个月和一年进行临时重新评估),将包括根据DSM-5(K-SADS-PL)标准诊断为PTSD且未诊断为精神病的40名8至16岁参与者。纳入的受试者分为有AVH和无AVH两组,按性别匹配,年龄和诊断。主要结果指标将是没有精神病的PTSD儿科人群中社会认知和情绪制造者与AVH的存在之间的相关性。用NEPSYII测试评估社会认知标记。使用差异情绪量表IV和修订后的关于声音的信念问卷评估情绪标记。次要结果指标是这些标志物与AVH的持续性和两年后患者初始诊断的进展的相关性。
    结论:我们的方案的独创性是通过认知偏差探索从PTSD到精神病的潜在进展。这项研究支持PTSD和AVH之间通过感觉连接的假设,情感和认知偏见。它提出了从PTSD到由于AVH等感知受损而导致的精神障碍的连续模型。
    背景:临床试验注册:ClinicalTrials.gov标识符:NCT03356028。
    BACKGROUND: Auditory-verbal hallucinatory experiences (AVH) have a 12% prevalence in the general pediatric population. Literature reports a higher risk of developing AVH in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The persistence of AVHs during adolescence represents a risk of evolution into psychotic disorders. Social cognition and emotional markers could be considered prodromes markers of this evolution. The objectives of this prospective observational study are to observe social cognition and emotional markers correlation with the presence and persistence of AVH over two years and with the evolution of PTSD and psychotic diagnosis.
    METHODS: This prospective case-control study, longitudinal over two years (with an interim reassessment at six months and one year), will include 40 participants aged 8 to 16 years old with a diagnosis of PTSD and without a diagnosis of psychosis according to the criteria of DSM-5 (K-SADS-PL). Subjects included are divided into two groups with AVH and without AVH matched by gender, age and diagnosis. The primary outcome measure will be the correlation between social cognition and emotional makers and the presence of AVH in the PTSD pediatric population without psychotic disorders. The social cognition marker is assessed with the NEPSY II test. The emotional marker is assessed with the Differential Emotion Scale IV and the Revised Beliefs About Voices Questionnaire. The secondary outcome measures are the correlation of these markers with the persistence of AVH and the evolution of the patient\'s initial diagnosis two years later.
    CONCLUSIONS: The originality of our protocol is to explore the potential progression to psychosis from PTSD by cognitive biases. This study supports the hypothesis of connections between PTSD and AVH through sensory, emotional and cognitive biases. It proposes a continuum model from PTSD to psychotic disorder due to impaired perception like AVH.
    BACKGROUND: Clinical trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03356028.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    民主社会必须面对日益增长的情感两极分化的挑战-强大的,情感上的燃料,不同政党的支持者之间的反对,或不同社会群体的观点和态度相互冲突。社会和心理科学可以有助于更好地理解这种现象以及情感理论和研究在这方面的特殊作用。在他们的邀请贡献中,Bakker和Lelkes注意到情感一词缺乏概念上的精确性,并邀请情感科学家用一种理论结构来填补这一概念,这种理论结构鼓励跨不同问题领域和文化的系统研究。关于本文的两个邀请评论支持这一倡议,但赞成一种特权离散情绪的方法。在他的评论中,VonScheve认为,意图和相关的行动倾向对于理解两极分化的情感基础至关重要。Halperin等人。,基于概念分析和自己的实证研究,他们突出了多方面的失望情绪,涵盖消极和积极的方面,作为最有前途的候选人。最后,我考虑了混合或混合情绪的潜在作用以及评估理论的潜在贡献,特别是在评估偏见和随之而来的情绪倾向和行动倾向方面。
    Democratic societies have to face the challenge of increasing affective polarisation - a strong, emotionally fuelled, opposition between adherents of different political parties, or conflicting views and attitudes in different social groups. Social and psychological sciences can contribute to a better understanding of this phenomenon and the special role of emotion theory and research in this respect. In their invited contribution, Bakker and Lelkes note the lack of conceptual precision of the term affective and invite emotion scientists to fill this notion with a theoretical structure that encourages systematic research across different problem areas and cultures. Two invited comments on this article support this initiative but favour an approach that privileges discrete emotions. In his comment, Von Scheve argues that intentionality and the associated action tendencies are essential to understand the emotional basis of polarisation. Halperin et al., based on a conceptual analysis and their own empirical research, they highlight the multi-faceted emotion of disappointment, covering both negative and positive aspects, as the most promising candidate. Finally, I consider the potential role of mixed or blended emotions as well as the potential contributions of appraisal theory, especially with respect to appraisal bias and ensuing emotion dispositions and action tendencies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    关于人类情感的有趣观点,根据贝叶斯大脑假设,构建的情感理论将情感视为生成模型。这一理论为现有的发现带来了新的见解,但是它的复杂性使得实验测试具有挑战性。我们认为疼痛的实验室研究可以支持这一理论,因为尽管有些人可能不认为疼痛是一种真正的情绪,该理论必须至少能够解释疼痛感知及其病理功能障碍。我们回顾了与这个问题有关的新证据。我们涵盖行为和神经实验室发现,计算模型,安慰剂痛觉过敏,和慢性疼痛。我们得出的结论是,有大量证据表明痛苦经历的预测性处理,为更好地理解其他情绪的神经元和计算机制铺平了道路。
    An intriguing perspective about human emotion, the theory of constructed emotion considers emotions as generative models according to the Bayesian brain hypothesis. This theory brings fresh insight to existing findings, but its complexity renders it challenging to test experimentally. We argue that laboratory studies of pain could support the theory because although some may not consider pain to be a genuine emotion, the theory must at minimum be able to explain pain perception and its dysfunction in pathology. We review emerging evidence that bear on this question. We cover behavioral and neural laboratory findings, computational models, placebo hyperalgesia, and chronic pain. We conclude that there is substantial evidence for a predictive processing account of painful experience, paving the way for a better understanding of neuronal and computational mechanisms of other emotions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:大流行对儿童及其家庭的日常生活产生了重大影响,特别是医护人员的孩子,由于父母的工作日程导致家庭惯例的变化。我们旨在探讨COVID-19大流行期间医护人员母亲(HCWM)子女的社会情感和行为(SEB)问题,并将其与年龄匹配的儿童及其来自其他职业的母亲进行比较。
    方法:采用病例对照研究设计,并使用滚雪球方法招募年龄在6至36个月之间的志愿者,通过Google调查。我们使用了简短的婴儿-幼儿社会和情绪评估(BITSEA)问卷来评估儿童的SEB问题,并使用了简短的症状清单(BSI)来评估母亲的心理困扰。BITSEA的差异,使用卡方和Mann-WhitneyU检验检查BSI和MSPSS分数,视情况而定。使用逻辑回归模型来确定儿童行为和情绪问题的独立预测因素。
    结果:总计,分析了600份问卷。据观察,HCWM组的儿童与母亲分离的频率更高,分离的时间也更长(p<0.010,p=0.002)。在流感大流行期间,儿童的结构性户外活动的变化更可能在HCWM组中观察到(p<0.05)。BITSEA问题子量表高于亚临床截止值的儿童百分比,HCWM组的外化和失调评分显著较高(p=0.044,p=0.031和p=0.016).此外,BSI整体指数评分(p<0.001,RR:3.34,95CI:1.91~5.82)的每增加一个点被认为是临床显著SEB问题的危险因素.
    结论:总体而言,目前的研究表明,HCWM经历了职业不平等,在大流行期间,HCWM的幼儿面临外部化和失调问题的风险增加。母亲的心理压力对子女的社会情绪幸福感有显著影响。
    BACKGROUND: The pandemic has had a significant impact on the daily lives of children and their families, particularly the children of health care workers, due to changes in family routines as a result of their parents\' work schedules. We aimed to explore the socioemotional and behavioural (SEB) problems of children of healthcare worker mothers (HCWM) during the COVID-19 pandemic and compare them with age-matched children and their mothers from other occupations.
    METHODS: A case-control study design was applied, and a snowball approach was used to enrol volunteered participants aged between 6 and 36 months of age, through a Google survey. We used the Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) questionnaire to assess children\'s SEB problems and a Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) to evaluate the psychological distress of mothers. Differences in BITSEA, BSI and MSPSS scores were examined using chi-square and Mann-Whitney U tests, as appropriate. A logistic regression model was used to identify independent predictors of children\'s behavioural and emotional problems.
    RESULTS: In total, 600 questionnaires were analysed. It was observed that children in the HCWM group were separated from their mothers more often and for longer periods of time than their counterparts (p < 0.010, p = 0.002). Changes in the child\'s structured outdoor activities during the pandemic period were more likely to be observed in the HCWM group (p < 0.05). The percentage of children with the BITSEA problem subscale above the subclinical cut-off, externalizing and dysregulation scores were significantly higher in the HCWM group (p = 0.044, p = 0.031, and p = 0.016). Moreover, each point increase in BSI global index scores (p < 0.001, RR:3.34, 95%CI:1.91-5.82) was found as a risk factor for clinically significant SEB problems.
    CONCLUSIONS: Overall, the current study suggests HCWM\'s have experienced occupational inequality, and young children of HCWM\'s were at increased risk for externalizing and dysregulation problems during the pandemic. Maternal psychological stress had a significant impact on their children\'s socio-emotional well-being.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    由于COVID-19大流行的限制,进行需要直接互动的感官评估变得具有挑战性。作为回应,研究人员一直有动力设计非面对面的测试方法作为替代方案。本研究旨在将两种非面对面的家庭使用测试(HUT)与传统的面对面中央位置测试(CLT)进行比较。这两个HUT都涉及在线招聘和向参与者家中提供样本。一个HUT提供了没有直接交互的书面协议(非接触式HUT;C-HUT),而另一个包括与研究人员进行实时指导的在线会议(在线HUT;O-HUT)。根据喜好,感官和情感属性评估了四个咖啡样品。CLT和O-HUT之间的比较显示RV系数为0.92,0.93和0.98(P<0.05)的喜好和感觉和情绪属性,分别。此外,根据RV系数,与C-HUT相比,CLT结果显示与O-HUT的相似性明显更大。O-HUT在其显著区分样品的能力方面也优于C-HUT。因此,研究人员和参与者之间的实时互动,在O-HUT的推动下,与C-HUT相比,在某些情况下可能更合适,完全依赖于书面协议。总的来说,这些发现表明,C-HUT和O-HUT是收集感官数据和克服地理和面对面接触限制的合适方法,提供更大的灵活性,并减少与传统感官评价相关的时间和成本。
    Due to the constraints of the COVID-19 pandemic, conducting sensory evaluations requiring direct interactions became challenging. In response, researchers have been motivated to devise non-face-to-face testing methods as alternatives. This study aimed to compare two non-face-to-face home-use tests (HUT) with the traditional face-to-face central location test (CLT). Both HUTs involved online recruitment and sample delivery to participants\' homes. One HUT provided a written protocol with no direct interaction (contactless HUT; C-HUT), whereas the other included an online meeting with a researcher for live guidance (online HUT; O-HUT). Four coffee samples were evaluated on the basis of liking and sensory and emotional attributes. The comparison between CLT and O-HUT showed RV coefficients of 0.92, 0.93, and 0.98 (P < 0.05) for liking and sensory and emotional attributes, respectively. In addition, based on the RV coefficient, the CLT results showed a significantly greater similarity to those of O-HUT compared to those of C-HUT. The O-HUT also outperformed the C-HUT in its ability to significantly discriminate between samples. Hence, real-time interactions between researchers and participants, as facilitated by O-HUT, may be more suitable in certain scenarios compared to C-HUT, which relies solely on a written protocol. Overall, these findings suggest that C-HUT and O-HUT are suitable methods for collecting sensory data and overcoming geographic and face-to-face contact limitations, providing greater flexibility, and reducing the time and cost associated with traditional sensory evaluations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:在低收入和中等收入国家,关于青少年情绪和行为问题(EBPs)的交叉信息协议的研究很少。这项研究旨在评估尼泊尔父母-青少年和教师-青少年关于EBP和相关因素的协议。
    方法:这项横断面调查包括1904名11-18岁的上学青少年,他们就读于尼泊尔16个地区的政府和私立学校。尼泊尔版本的青年自我报告,儿童行为清单,和教师的报告表被用来评估青少年报告的EBP,他们的父母,和老师们,分别。进行重复测量方差分析(ANOVA)以评估问题得分的平均差异。Pearson的相关性用于评估交叉信息协议。线性回归分析用于探索与EBPs交叉信息差异相关的因素。
    结果:青少年报告的问题明显多于他们的父母和老师。青少年自我报告中90个常见项目的平均总问题得分,父报告,教师报告为34.5(标准偏差[SD]:21.4),24.1(标准差=19.2),和20.2(SD=17.5)。家长和青少年在总问题上的协议是适度的,而教师与青少年的一致性较低。对于两个宽带量表和所有综合征量表,父母与青少年的协议是中等到低的,而教师与青少年的一致性在所有尺度上都很低。女性性别和少数民族地位影响了父母-青少年和教师-青少年的差异。家庭压力/冲突影响父母-青少年差异,而学习成绩影响教师与青少年的差异。
    结论:尼泊尔青少年报告的EBP比他们的父母和老师多。青少年的自我报告与父母和老师的报告之间的协议是中等到低的。性别,种姓/种族,家庭压力/冲突,学业成绩与交叉信息差异有关。从不同来源收集信息至关重要,考虑特定环境的需求,并辨别影响交叉信息差异的因素,以准确评估青少年的EBP,并制定个性化的治疗计划方法。
    BACKGROUND: Studies on cross-informant agreement on adolescents\' emotional and behavioral problems (EBPs) are sparse in low- and middle-income countries. This study aimed to assess parent-adolescent and teacher-adolescent agreement on EBPs and associated factors in Nepal.
    METHODS: This cross-sectional survey included 1904 school-going adolescents aged 11-18, enrolled in government and private schools located in sixteen districts of Nepal. The Nepali versions of the Youth Self Report, Child Behavior Checklist, and Teacher\'s Report Form were administered to assess EBPs reported by adolescents, their parents, and teachers, respectively. Repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was done to assess mean differences in problem scores. Pearson\'s correlation was used to assess cross-informant agreement. Linear regression analysis was used to explore factors associated with cross-informant discrepancies in EBPs.
    RESULTS: Adolescents reported significantly more problems than their parents and teachers. Mean Total Problem scores for the 90 common items in the adolescents\' self-reports, parent reports, and teacher reports were 34.5 (standard deviation [SD]: 21.4), 24.1 (SD = 19.2), and 20.2 (SD = 17.5) respectively. Parent-adolescent agreement on Total Problems was moderate, whereas teacher-adolescent agreement was low. The parent-adolescent agreement was moderate to low for the two broadband scales and all syndrome scales, whereas the teacher-adolescent agreement was low for all scales. Female gender and ethnic minority status impacted both parent-adolescent and teacher-adolescent discrepancies. Family stress/conflicts impacted parent-adolescent discrepancies, while academic performance impacted teacher-adolescent discrepancies.
    CONCLUSIONS: Nepali adolescents reported more EBPs than their parents and teachers. The agreement between adolescents\' self-reports and reports by their parents and teachers was moderate to low. Gender, caste/ethnicity, family stress/conflicts, and academic performance were associated with cross-informant discrepancies. It is crucial to collect information from different sources, consider context-specific needs, and discern factors influencing cross-informant discrepancies to accurately assess adolescents\' EBPs and develop personalized approaches to treatment planning.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    高度优越的自传体记忆(HSAM)是一种罕见的特殊记忆形式,其特征是能够根据日期回忆个人事件。单例“DT”是全球不到100名HSAM个体之一,人们对这些人如何组织他们可以回忆的大量事件知之甚少。我们执行了2个新的启动任务来探索自传记忆之间的导航。在这两项任务中,屏幕上出现了一个“prime”日期,指示DT从该日期开始访问并重新激活特定的内存。3s后,出现了一个“目标”日期,和DT切换到新的内存。已记录延迟。实验一探讨情绪效价对记忆导航的影响。DT更快地从积极或消极记忆转变为中性记忆,与中性记忆相比,支持情感在连接HSAM记忆中的作用。实验2研究了语义内容和心理时间线配置在组织中的作用。DT向前移动得更快(例如,1996-1997)比倒退(例如,2023-2022),表明对时间的向前感知。在语义内容方面没有观察到差异。结果提供了对DT的记忆维度的深入了解,并支持使用此任务来探索组织。
    Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM) is a rare form of exceptional memory, characterised by an ability to recall personal episodes in response to dates. The single case \"DT\" is one of less than 100 HSAM individuals globally, and little is known about how these individuals organise the vast number of events they can recollect. We administered 2 novel priming tasks to explore navigation between autobiographical memories. In both tasks, a \"prime\" date appeared on the screen and DT was instructed to access and begin reliving a specific memory from that date. After 3 s, a \"target\" date appeared, and DT switched to the new memory. Latencies were recorded. Experiment 1 explored the influence of emotional valence on memory navigation. DT was quicker moving from positive or negative memories to neutral memories, compared to between neutral memories, supporting the role of emotionality in connecting memories in HSAM. Experiment 2 investigated semantic content and mental timeline configuration\'s role in organisation. DT was faster moving forward (e.g., 1996-1997) than backwards (e.g., 2023-2022), indicating a forwards perception of time. No differences were observed regarding semantic content. Results provide insight into DT\'s memory dimensions and support the use of this task to explore organisation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号