speech production

【语音制作】
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    名称协议(NA)是指发言者对图片名称的同意程度。一个有力的发现是,说话者比那些具有低一致性(LA)的人更快地命名具有高一致性(HA)的图片。这种NA效应被认为是因为LA图片强烈激活了几个名字,所以演讲者需要时间来选择一个。HA图片,相比之下,强烈激活单个名称,因此不需要从多个备选方案中选择一个名称。最近的词汇访问模型表明,心理词汇的结构随经验而变化。因此,演讲者在命名洛杉矶图片时应该考虑一系列的名字,但是他们认为每个名字的程度应该随着经验而改变。我们在两个图片命名实验中测试了这些假设。在实验1中,参与者在为每张图片命名一次时,命名LA的速度比HA图片快。重要的是,他们比洛杉矶图片的替代名称更快地产生模态名称(由大多数参与者提供),与扬声器为LA图片激活多个名称的观点一致。在实验2中,参与者在实验前熟悉了模态名称,并为每张图片命名了三次。尽管参与者第一次命名图片时仍然存在NA效果,与实验1相比,它减少了,并且随着每次图片重复而进一步减少。因此,熟悉和重复降低了NA效应,但没有消除它,建议演讲者激活一系列合理的名字。
    Name agreement (NA) refers to the degree to which speakers agree on a picture\'s name. A robust finding is that speakers are faster to name pictures with high agreement (HA) than those with low agreement (LA). This NA effect is thought to occur because LA pictures strongly activate several names, and so speakers need time to select one. HA pictures, in contrast, strongly activate a single name and so there is no need to select one name out of several alternatives. Recent models of lexical access suggest that the structure of the mental lexicon changes with experience. Thus, speakers should consider a range of names when naming LA pictures, but the extent to which they consider each of these names should change with experience. We tested these hypotheses in two picture-naming experiments. In Experiment 1, participants were faster to name LA than HA pictures when they named each picture once. Importantly, they were faster to produce modal names (provided by most participants) than alternative names for LA pictures, consistent with the view that speakers activate multiple names for LA pictures. In Experiment 2, participants were familiarised with the modal name before the experiment and named each picture three times. Although there was still an NA effect when participants named the pictures the first time, it was reduced in comparison to Experiment 1 and was further reduced with each picture repetition.Thus, familiarisation and repetition reduced the NA effect, but did not eliminate it, suggesting speakers activate a range of plausible names.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:开发LittlEARS®早期言语制作问卷(LEESPQ)旨在为专业人士提供有关儿童最早语言发展的有价值信息,并已成功地用多种语言进行验证。这项研究旨在验证塞尔维亚版本的LEESPQ在典型发育儿童中的应用,并将结果与其他语言的验证研究进行比较。
    方法:英文版的LEESPQ被回译为塞尔维亚语。父母在访问儿科诊所或通过个人联系以纸质或电子形式填写问卷。共收集了206份完整的问卷。使用二阶多项式模型计算18个月以下儿童的标准化期望值,以创建塞尔维亚语的规范曲线。然后将结果用于确定置信区间,下限是典型的语音语言发展的关键限制。最后,将结果与德国和加拿大英语的发展规范进行比较。
    结果:塞尔维亚LEESPQ版本显示出高同质性(r=.622)和内部一致性(α=.882),这表明它几乎完全衡量言语能力。男女婴儿总分无显著差异(U=4429.500,p=.090),所以它可以被认为是性别独立的问卷。塞尔维亚语和德语(U=645.500,p=.673)与塞尔维亚语和英语规范曲线(U=652.000,p=.725)之间的比较结果表明,LEESPQ可以应用于不同的人口群体,不管语言,文化或社会学差异。
    结论:LEESPQ是有效的,适合评估出生至18个月儿童早期言语发育的年龄依赖性和性别无关问卷。
    OBJECTIVE: The LittlEARS® Early Speech Production Questionnaire (LEESPQ) was developed to provide professionals with valuable information about children\'s earliest language development and has been successfully validated in several languages. This study aimed to validate the Serbian version of the LEESPQ in typically developing children and compare the results with validation studies in other languages.
    METHODS: The English version of the LEESPQ was back-translated into Serbian. Parents completed the questionnaire in paper or electronic form either during the visit to the paediatric clinic or through personal contact. A total of 206 completed questionnaires were collected. Standardized expected values were calculated using a second-order polynomial model for children up to 18 months of age to create a norm curve for the Serbian language. The results were then used to determine confidence intervals, with the lower limit being the critical limit for typical speech-language development. Finally, the results were compared with German and Canadian English developmental norms.
    RESULTS: The Serbian LEESPQ version showed high homogeneity (r = .622) and internal consistency (α = .882), indicating that it almost exclusively measures speech production ability. No significant difference in total score was found between male and female infants (U = 4429.500, p = .090), so it can be considered a gender-independent questionnaire. The results of the comparison between Serbian and German (U = 645.500, p = .673) and Serbian and English norm curves (U = 652.000, p = .725) show that the LEESPQ can be applied to different population groups, regardless of linguistic, cultural or sociological differences.
    CONCLUSIONS: The LEESPQ is a valid, age-dependent and gender-independent questionnaire suitable for assessing early speech development in children aged from birth to 18 months.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:多发性硬化(MS)患者声音的主要改变是发声不稳定,声带无力和粗糙,呼吸急促,张力减退和鼻音亢进.然而,关于声学参数变化的研究很少,结果不同。目的调查MS患者的语音障碍;采用客观措施(生物力学分析)和主观措施(量表和问卷)。
    方法:实验研究,共有20名MS患者。采集了语音样本,和生物力学相关性通过临床语音系统程序进行分析,在线实验室APP。VHI-30(语音障碍指数)问卷,GRBAS(年级,粗糙度,呼吸,虚弱,应变)量表和医院焦虑抑郁量表作为主观测量。
    结果:95%的参与者感到并描述了呼吸困难。在女性样本中,声音残疾的自我感知与听觉声音感知分析相关。
    结论:生物力学参数显示声门闭合强度的改变,在效率指数和结构失衡指数中。
    BACKGROUND: The predominant alterations in voice of patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) are phonatory instability, vocal asthenia and roughness, shortness of breath, hypophonia, and hypernasality. However, research on alterations of acoustic parameters has few studies and disparate results. The objective of this study was to investigate voice disturbances in patients with MS, both with objective measures (analysis of biomechanical) and subjective measures (scales and questionnaires).
    METHODS: This is an experimental study with a total of 20 participants with MS. Voice samples were collected, and biomechanical correlates were analyzed through the Clinical Voice Systems program, Online Lab App. The VHI-30 (Voice Handicap Index) questionnaire, the GRBAS (grade, roughness, breathiness, asthenia, strain) scale, and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale were used as subjective measures.
    RESULTS: Ninety-five percentages of participants feel and describe dysphonic difficulties. Self-perception of vocal disability correlated with auditory vocal perceptual analysis in the sample of women.
    CONCLUSIONS: The biomechanical parameters showed alterations in the strength of the glottic closure, the efficiency index, and the structural imbalance index.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:本调查研究了诸如裂隙类型之类的因素,初级腭手术的年龄,诊断综合征,听力问题,咬合不正可以预测持续的言语困难和学龄儿童对言语服务的需求。
    方法:参与者包括100名学龄儿童腭裂。Americleft语音协议用于评估语音产生的感知方面。进行逻辑回归以评估自变量(IV)对因变量(DV)的影响:清晰度,后口腔CSCs,听得见的鼻发射,鼻音亢进,前口腔CSCs,需要言语治疗。
    结果:65%的儿童参加(或接受过)言语治疗。逻辑回归模型显示出与言语治疗需求数据的良好拟合(Hosmer和Lemeshow的χ2(8)=9.647,p=.291)。没有发现静脉注射对言语治疗的需要有重大影响。诊断的综合征与较低的清晰度相关(Pulkstenis-Robinson'sχ2(11)=7.120,p=.789)。被诊断为综合症的儿童的鼻塞性评分(赔率=5.703)的几率是其他儿童的六倍。裂隙类型与可听见的鼻发射显着相关(Fisher\'sexactp=.006)。同时,错牙合畸形与前口腔CSCs有显著关联(Fisher'sexactp=.005)。
    结论:根据英国Cleft注册和审计网络年度报告的最新数据,大多数腭裂儿童在五岁时达到典型的语言水平。然而,研究可能影响这个年龄后言语障碍持续的因素至关重要。这种理解对于制定旨在减轻个体年龄增长时言语障碍的长期影响的干预策略至关重要。
    OBJECTIVE: The present investigation examined how factors such as cleft type, age of primary palatal surgery, diagnosed syndromes, hearing problems, and malocclusions could predict persistent speech difficulties and the need for speech services in school-aged children with cleft palate.
    METHODS: Participants included 100 school-aged children with cleft palate. Americleft speech protocol was used to assess the perceptual aspects of speech production. The logistic regression was performed to evaluate the impact of independent variables (IV) on the dependent variables (DV): intelligibility, posterior oral CSCs, audible nasal emission, hypernasality, anterior oral CSCs, and speech therapy required.
    RESULTS: Sixty-five percent of the children were enrolled in (or had received) speech therapy. The logistic regression model shows a good fit to the data for the need for speech therapy (Hosmer and Lemeshow\'s χ2(8)=9.647,p=.291). No IVs were found to have a significant impact on the need for speech therapy. A diagnosed syndrome was associated with poorer intelligibility (Pulkstenis-Robinson\'s χ2(11)=7.120,p=.789). Children with diagnosed syndromes have about six times the odds of a higher hypernasality rating (Odds Ratio = 5.703) than others. The cleft type was significantly associated with audible nasal emission (Fisher\'sexactp=.006). At the same time, malocclusion had a significant association with anterior oral CSCs (Fisher\'sexactp=.005).
    CONCLUSIONS: According to the latest data in the Cleft Registry and Audit Network Annual Report for the UK, the majority of children with cleft palate attain typical speech by age five. However, it is crucial to delve into the factors that may influence the continuation of speech disorders beyond this age. This understanding is vital for formulating intervention strategies aimed at mitigating the long-term effects of speech disorders as individuals grow older.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究检验了一个假设,即与其他声音说话会影响对自己声音的感觉运动预测。听觉反馈的实时操作用于驱动语音中的感觉运动适应,当参与者与另一个声音同步说出句子时,已知会引起隐式模仿(语音收敛)的任务。其他声音的声学语音特性在组间进行了操纵,这样与它的趋同要么反对(不一致的群体,n=15)或与(全等组对齐,n=16)语音运动适应。正如预测的那样,与不一致组相比,一致组的适应性明显更大。这表明在语音中使用共享的感觉目标来预测他人行为(语音感知)和自我行为(语音产生)的感觉结果。这一发现对跨感知和行动的共享预测机制的更广泛理论具有重要意义。比如主动推理。
    This study tested the hypothesis that speaking with other voices can influence sensorimotor predictions of one\'s own voice. Real-time manipulations of auditory feedback were used to drive sensorimotor adaptation in speech, while participants spoke sentences in synchrony with another voice, a task known to induce implicit imitation (phonetic convergence). The acoustic-phonetic properties of the other voice were manipulated between groups, such that convergence with it would either oppose (incongruent group, n = 15) or align with (congruent group, n = 16) speech motor adaptation. As predicted, significantly greater adaptation was seen in the congruent compared to the incongruent group. This suggests the use of shared sensory targets in speech for predicting the sensory outcomes of both the actions of others (speech perception) and the actions of the self (speech production). This finding has important implications for wider theories of shared predictive mechanisms across perception and action, such as active inference.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在用意大利语进行的两项演讲制作研究中,我们研究了语音邻域特性,例如邻域密度和邻域的平均频率对语音处理的影响。测试了健康(研究1)和神经受损(研究2)个体的两个群体。我们采用多元回归方法来分析研究1中的命名延迟和研究2中的准确率,同时控制各种心理语言学预测因子。在研究1中,来自高密度社区的单词图片的命名速度比来自低密度社区的单词更快。此外,具有高频邻居的单词在研究1中被更快地命名,并且在研究2中获得更高的准确率.结果表明,语音邻域密度和频率邻域变量都具有促进作用。此外,我们观察到这两个语音邻域变量之间的相互作用以及名称一致性和重复。具体来说,对于名称一致性较低的图片以及图片的初始呈现期间,促进效果更为明显。这些发现是在先前文献的背景下以及在语音产生的交互式模型的框架内进行讨论的。
    In two speech production studies conducted in Italian, we investigated the impact of phonological neighbourhood properties such as the neighbourhood density and the mean frequency of the neighbours on speech processing. Two populations of healthy (Study 1) and neurologically impaired (Study 2) individuals were tested. We employed multi-regression methods to analyse naming latencies in Study 1 and accuracy rates in Study 2 while controlling for various psycholinguistic predictors. In Study 1, pictures with words from high-density neighbourhoods were named faster than those from low-density neighbourhoods. Additionally, words with high-frequency neighbours were named faster in Study 1 and yielded higher accuracy rates in Study 2. The results suggest facilitatory effects of both the phonological neighbourhood density and frequency neighbourhood variables. Furthermore, we observed interactions between these two phonological neighbourhood variables and name agreement and repetition. Specifically, the facilitation effect was more pronounced for pictures with lower name agreement and during the initial presentation of the pictures. These findings are discussed in the context of previous literature and within the framework of interactive models of speech production.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人类的新兴研究已经建立了重复经颅磁刺激(rTMS)对初级体感皮层(S1)的调节作用。然而,到目前为止,这方面的研究主要集中在手和手指上,在我们对rTMS对口面部系统和言语发音器的体感感知的调节作用的理解上留下了空白。
    本研究旨在研究不同类型的theta爆发刺激-连续TBS(cTBS)的影响,间歇性TBS(iTBS),或在舌头的触觉敏锐度上伪造左侧S1的舌头表示。
    在重复措施设计中,15名志愿者参加了四个单独的会议,其中cTBS,iTBS,sham,或未在左侧S1的舌头上施加刺激。使用定制的振动触觉刺激器测量TBS对舌头的时间和空间感知敏锐度的影响。
    CTBS在刺激后16-30分钟的时间窗口显着损害了空间振幅阈值,而iTBS在同一时间窗口改进了它。iTBS的效果,然而,小于cTBS。相比之下,cTBS和iTBS对时间辨别阈值均无影响.
    本研究确立了使用TBS调节口面系统的体感感知的有效性。直接改变口腔系统的体感有可能使触觉敏锐度异常的临床人群受益。提高我们对感官系统在言语产生中的作用的理解,加强言语运动学习和康复。
    Theta爆发刺激(TBS)可以调节口面系统cTBS超过S1的舌头空间敏锐度受损TBS超过S1的舌头空间敏锐度改善。
    UNASSIGNED: Emerging studies in humans have established the modulatory effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) over primary somatosensory cortex (S1) on somatosensory cortex activity and perception. However, to date, research in this area has primarily focused on the hand and fingers, leaving a gap in our understanding of the modulatory effects of rTMS on somatosensory perception of the orofacial system and speech articulators.
    UNASSIGNED: The present study aimed to examine the effects of different types of theta-burst stimulation-continuous TBS (cTBS), intermittent TBS (iTBS), or sham-over the tongue representation of left S1 on tactile acuity of the tongue.
    UNASSIGNED: In a repeated-measures design, fifteen volunteers participated in four separate sessions, where cTBS, iTBS, sham, or no stimulation was applied over the tongue representation of left S1. Effects of TBS were measured on both temporal and spatial perceptual acuity of tongue using a custom vibrotactile stimulator.
    UNASSIGNED: CTBS significantly impaired spatial amplitude threshold at the time window of 16-30 minutes after stimulation, while iTBS improved it at the same time window. The effect of iTBS, however, was smaller than cTBS. In contrast, neither cTBS nor iTBS had any effect on the temporal discrimination threshold.
    UNASSIGNED: The current study establishes the validity of using TBS to modulate somatosensory perception of the orofacial system. Directly modifying somatosensation in the orofacial system has the potential to benefit clinical populations with abnormal tactile acuity, improve our understanding of the role of sensory systems in speech production, and enhance speech motor learning and rehabilitation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    关于听觉和运动语音系统之间的相互作用存在相当多的争论。一些人认为常见的神经机制,而其他人则断言共享资源很少。在四个实验中,我们检验了以下假设:与通过按钮按下涉及相同-不同判断的启动辨别任务相比,通过大声重复音节对来启动语音运动系统可以改善随后的音节辨别噪声。我们的结果一致表明,从事音节重复的参与者在噪声中的音节辨别方面比从事启动辨别任务的参与者表现更好。这种精度的提高被观察到了初始和新的音节对,强调对语音细节的敏感性增加。无论启动任务涉及听觉还是视觉表现,好处都是可比的。在启动任务和音节噪声任务之间插入1小时的延迟,好处仍然存在,但仅限于启动音节对。最后,我们证明了这种方法在老年人中的有效性.我们的发现证实了言语产生-感知关系的存在。它们还具有临床相关性,因为它们提高了基于生产的干预措施以提高言语感知能力的可能性。这对于经常在噪声中感知语音时遇到困难的老年人尤其相关。
    Considerable debate exists about the interplay between auditory and motor speech systems. Some argue for common neural mechanisms, whereas others assert that there are few shared resources. In four experiments, we tested the hypothesis that priming the speech motor system by repeating syllable pairs aloud improves subsequent syllable discrimination in noise compared with a priming discrimination task involving same-different judgments via button presses. Our results consistently showed that participants who engaged in syllable repetition performed better in syllable discrimination in noise than those who engaged in the priming discrimination task. This gain in accuracy was observed for primed and new syllable pairs, highlighting increased sensitivity to phonological details. The benefits were comparable whether the priming tasks involved auditory or visual presentation. Inserting a 1-h delay between the priming tasks and the syllable-in-noise task, the benefits persisted but were confined to primed syllable pairs. Finally, we demonstrated the effectiveness of this approach in older adults. Our findings substantiate the existence of a speech production-perception relationship. They also have clinical relevance as they raise the possibility of production-based interventions to improve speech perception ability. This would be particularly relevant for older adults who often encounter difficulties in perceiving speech in noise.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    语音可以定义为人类通过一系列声音进行交流的能力。因此,语音需要能够产生声信号的发射器(扬声器)和能够成功解码发射器产生的声音的接收器(收听者)(即,声学信号)。时间在这种互动的两端都起着核心作用。一方面,语音制作需要精确和快速的协调,通常在毫秒的数量级内,上声道发声器的(即,舌头,下巴,嘴唇,和velum),他们的复合动作,和声带的激活。另一方面,产生的声信号及时展开,在不同的时间尺度上携带信息。该信息必须由接收器解析和整合,以便正确地传输含义。本章描述了表征语音信号的时间模式,并回顾了探索这些模式产生的神经机制及其在语音理解中的作用的研究。
    Speech can be defined as the human ability to communicate through a sequence of vocal sounds. Consequently, speech requires an emitter (the speaker) capable of generating the acoustic signal and a receiver (the listener) able to successfully decode the sounds produced by the emitter (i.e., the acoustic signal). Time plays a central role at both ends of this interaction. On the one hand, speech production requires precise and rapid coordination, typically within the order of milliseconds, of the upper vocal tract articulators (i.e., tongue, jaw, lips, and velum), their composite movements, and the activation of the vocal folds. On the other hand, the generated acoustic signal unfolds in time, carrying information at different timescales. This information must be parsed and integrated by the receiver for the correct transmission of meaning. This chapter describes the temporal patterns that characterize the speech signal and reviews research that explores the neural mechanisms underlying the generation of these patterns and the role they play in speech comprehension.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    众所周知,频率分布会广泛影响心理语言过程。词频在说话时的影响,对话中社会行动的核心,有,相比之下,在很大程度上被忽视了。本研究通过在英国国家语料库(BNC)和弗莱堡多模态交互语料库(FreMIC)的会话部分应用语料库语言方法来探讨这一差距。后者包括记录对话的参与者的连续瞳孔大小测量,一方面,允许系统地调查所含语音和语言的模式,以及它们与说话者和接受者可能产生的并发处理成本的关系。我们在这方面检验了第一个假设,分析说话转弯中的词频分布是否与这些转弯的产生和接收过程中的对话者的处理努力相关。发现转弯通常显示出词频的规则分布模式,在转弯初始位置使用非常频繁的单词,转向中间位置的中频单词,和在最终位置的低频单词。发现说话者的瞳孔大小在谈话过程中趋于增加,到达转弯终点的高潮。值得注意的是,观察到的单词频率在转弯内的减少与观察到的说话者瞳孔大小的增加成反比,但不是在收件人中,随着单词频率的急剧降低,扬声器的瞳孔大小也会急剧增加。我们讨论了这些发现对语音处理理论的影响,车削结构,信息包装。至关重要的是,我们认为,在演讲中发言者的处理努力的加强是由于信息高潮,这意味着从高频发展,低信息词通过中级到低频,高信息词。至少在英语会话中,对话者似乎利用这种模式作为实现对话互动效率的一种方式,在说话轮次中创建定期重复的处理负荷分布,这有助于平滑的转弯过渡,内容预测,和有效的信息传递。
    Frequency distributions are known to widely affect psycholinguistic processes. The effects of word frequency in turns-at-talk, the nucleus of social action in conversation, have, by contrast, been largely neglected. This study probes into this gap by applying corpus-linguistic methods on the conversational component of the British National Corpus (BNC) and the Freiburg Multimodal Interaction Corpus (FreMIC). The latter includes continuous pupil size measures of participants of the recorded conversations, allowing for a systematic investigation of patterns in the contained speech and language on the one hand and their relation to concurrent processing costs they may incur in speakers and recipients on the other hand. We test a first hypothesis in this vein, analyzing whether word frequency distributions within turns-at-talk are correlated with interlocutors\' processing effort during the production and reception of these turns. Turns are found to generally show a regular distribution pattern of word frequency, with highly frequent words in turn-initial positions, mid-range frequency words in turn-medial positions, and low-frequency words in turn-final positions. Speakers\' pupil size is found to tend to increase during the course of a turn at talk, reaching a climax toward the turn end. Notably, the observed decrease in word frequency within turns is inversely correlated with the observed increase in pupil size in speakers, but not in recipients, with steeper decreases in word frequency going along with steeper increases in pupil size in speakers. We discuss the implications of these findings for theories of speech processing, turn structure, and information packaging. Crucially, we propose that the intensification of processing effort in speakers during a turn at talk is owed to an informational climax, which entails a progression from high-frequency, low-information words through intermediate levels to low-frequency, high-information words. At least in English conversation, interlocutors seem to make use of this pattern as one way to achieve efficiency in conversational interaction, creating a regularly recurring distribution of processing load across speaking turns, which aids smooth turn transitions, content prediction, and effective information transfer.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号