Signal Detection Theory

信号检测理论
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当展示阵容时,证人的任务是指认罪犯或表明罪犯不在场。然后,证人对该决定进行信心判断。但是证人如何做出这些决定和判断?根据绝对判断模型,目击者确定哪个阵容成员提供了最强的记忆匹配,并根据这个MAX阵容成员的绝对实力确定他们的身份决定和信心判断。相反,相对判断模型建议证人确定哪个阵容成员提供了最强的匹配记忆,然后将他们的身份决定和信心判断基于MAX阵容成员相对于其余阵容成员的相对强度。我们采取了一种关键的测试方法来测试这两个模型的预测。正如绝对判断模型所预测的那样,但与相对判断模型的预测相反,证人更有可能正确地拒绝低相似性阵容比高相似性阵容(实验1),与公平阵容相比,更有可能拒绝有偏见的阵容(实验2)。同样,证人拒绝低相似性阵容的信心高于高相似性阵容(实验1),拒绝偏见阵容的信心高于公平阵容(实验2)。只有一个模式与相对模型一致,而与绝对模型不一致:来自偏倚阵容的可疑识别比来自公平阵容的可疑识别具有更大的置信度(实验2)。结果表明,绝对判断模型比相对判断模型更好地预测证人决策,而纯粹的相对判断模型是不可行的。
    When presented with a lineup, the witness is tasked with identifying the culprit or indicating that the culprit is not present. The witness then qualifies the decision with a confidence judgment. But how do witnesses go about making these decisions and judgments? According to absolute-judgment models, witnesses determine which lineup member provides the strongest match to memory and base their identification decision and confidence judgment on the absolute strength of this MAX lineup member. Conversely, relative-judgment models propose that witnesses determine which lineup member provides the strongest match to memory and then base their identification decision and confidence judgment on the relative strength of the MAX lineup member compared to the remaining lineup members. We took a critical test approach to test the predictions of both models. As predicted by the absolute-judgment model, but contrary to the predictions of the relative-judgment model, witnesses were more likely to correctly reject low-similarity lineups than high-similarity lineups (Experiment 1), and more likely to reject biased lineups than fair lineups (Experiment 2). Likewise, witnesses rejected low-similarity lineups with greater confidence than high-similarity lineups (Experiment 1) and rejected biased lineups with greater confidence than fair lineups (Experiment 2). Only a single pattern was consistent with the relative model and inconsistent with the absolute model: suspect identifications from biased lineups were made with greater confidence than suspect identifications from fair lineups (Experiment 2). The results suggest that absolute-judgment models better predict witness decision-making than do relative-judgment models and that pure relative-judgment models are unviable.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    判别性度量d\'在心理学中被广泛用于独立于反应偏差来估计灵敏度。估计d'的常规方法涉及从命中率和误报率的变换。当性能完美时,必须应用校正方法来计算d',但是这些修正扭曲了估计。在三个模拟研究中,我们表明,d'估计中的失真可能来自实验设计的其他属性(试验次数,样本量,样本方差,任务难度),当结合校正方法的应用时,使任何特定实验设计中的d\'失真复杂,并可能在最坏的情况下(I型和II型错误)误导统计推断。为了解决这个问题,我们建议研究人员模拟d'估计来探索设计选择的影响,给定预期或观察到的数据。介绍了一种RShiny应用程序,用于估计d'失真,为研究人员提供识别失真并采取措施将其影响降至最低的手段。
    The discriminability measure d \' is widely used in psychology to estimate sensitivity independently of response bias. The conventional approach to estimate d \' involves a transformation from the hit rate and the false-alarm rate. When performance is perfect, correction methods must be applied to calculate d \' , but these corrections distort the estimate. In three simulation studies, we show that distortion in d \' estimation can arise from other properties of the experimental design (number of trials, sample size, sample variance, task difficulty) that, when combined with application of the correction method, make d \' distortion in any specific experiment design complex and can mislead statistical inference in the worst cases (Type I and Type II errors). To address this problem, we propose that researchers simulate d \' estimation to explore the impact of design choices, given anticipated or observed data. An R Shiny application is introduced that estimates d \' distortion, providing researchers the means to identify distortion and take steps to minimize its impact.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    研究人员对高配错误的原因提出异议,低NoGo目标检测任务,比如持续关注响应任务(SART)。一些研究人员认为SART中的错误是由于感知解耦,参与者不知道刺激身份。这种缺乏外部意识会导致错误的反应。其他研究人员认为,SART中的大多数错误是由于反应宽大处理,不是感知脱钩。响应延迟可能会使最初不知道刺激身份的参与者,感知解耦,意识到刺激的身份,或在感知上重新耦合。如果,然而,将刺激呈现时间缩短到刺激识别所需的最小值,并且用结构化的掩模破坏刺激,那么即使有反应延迟,也应该没有时间使感知重新耦合。从感知脱钩的角度来看,在这种情况下,响应延迟对性能没有影响。或者,如果响应偏差是关键的,即使在这种情况下,响应延迟也可能影响性能。在这项研究中,我们缩短了刺激呈现时间,并增加了一个结构化的面具。我们检查了响应延迟是否影响SART和SART响应格式反转的任务的性能。我们预计响应延迟只会影响信号检测理论偏差,C,在SART中,回应宽大处理是一个问题。在反向格式化的SART中,由于偏见预计不会宽大处理,我们预计反应延迟对反应偏差没有影响或影响最小.这些预测得到了验证。响应偏差在理解SART性能方面更为重要,而不是感知解耦,如果它发生在SART中,这是罕见的。
    Researchers dispute the cause of errors in high Go, low No Go target detection tasks, like the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART). Some researchers propose errors in the SART are due to perceptual decoupling, where a participant is unaware of stimulus identity. This lack of external awareness causes an erroneous response. Other researchers suggest the majority of the errors in the SART are instead due to response leniency, not perceptual decoupling. Response delays may enable a participant who is initially unaware of stimulus identity, perceptually decoupled, to become aware of stimulus identity, or perceptually recoupled. If, however, the stimulus presentation time is shortened to the minimum necessary for stimulus recognition and the stimulus is disrupted with a structured mask, then there should be no time to enable perception to recouple even with a response delay. From the perceptual decoupling perspective, there should be no impact of a response delay on performance in this case. Alternatively if response bias is critical, then even in this case a response delay may impact performance. In this study, we shortened stimulus presentation time and added a structured mask. We examined whether a response delay impacted performance in the SART and tasks where the SART\'s response format was reversed. We expected a response delay would only impact signal detection theory bias, c, in the SART, where response leniency is an issue. In the reverse formatted SART, since bias was not expected to be lenient, we expected no impact or minimal impact of a response delay on response bias. These predictions were verified. Response bias is more critical in understanding SART performance, than perceptual decoupling, which is rare if it occurs at all in the SART.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在许多决策任务中,我们有一系列的替代选择,我们面临的问题是如何利用我们潜在的信念和偏好对每一个选择做出单一的选择。认知和决策模型通常假定信念和偏好被提炼成每种选择的标量潜在强度,但对人们如何利用这些潜在的优势来选择单一的替代方案进行建模也是至关重要的。大多数模型遵循两个传统之一来建立这种联系。现代心理物理学和记忆研究者利用信号检测理论,假设潜在的强度受到噪音的干扰,并选择最高的结果信号。相比之下,许多现代决策理论建模和机器学习方法都使用softmax函数(该函数基于Luce的选择公理;Luce,1959)给予非最大强度替代品一定的权重。尽管这两种选择理论很突出,目前的方法很少解决它们之间的联系,和选择一个或另一个似乎更多的动机在传统的相关文献比理论或经验的原因更喜欢一个理论。当前工作的目标是通过阐明这两个模型中的哪一个可以更好地表征m-alternative决策任务中的潜在过程来重新审视这一主题。特别关注内存任务。在一组视觉记忆实验中,我们证明,在相同的实验设计中,softmax参数β在m个备选方案中变化,而信号检测模型的参数d'是稳定的。一起,我们的发现表明,用信号检测链路模型代替softmax将在任务结构的变化中产生更普遍的预测。更有野心,信号检测模型参数在不同任务之间的不变性表明,这些模型的参数假设可能不仅仅是数学上的便利,但反映了人类决策的一些真实的东西。
    In many decision tasks, we have a set of alternative choices and are faced with the problem of how to use our latent beliefs and preferences about each alternative to make a single choice. Cognitive and decision models typically presume that beliefs and preferences are distilled to a scalar latent strength for each alternative, but it is also critical to model how people use these latent strengths to choose a single alternative. Most models follow one of two traditions to establish this link. Modern psychophysics and memory researchers make use of signal detection theory, assuming that latent strengths are perturbed by noise, and the highest resulting signal is selected. By contrast, many modern decision theoretic modeling and machine learning approaches use the softmax function (which is based on Luce\'s choice axiom; Luce, 1959) to give some weight to non-maximal-strength alternatives. Despite the prominence of these two theories of choice, current approaches rarely address the connection between them, and the choice of one or the other appears more motivated by the tradition in the relevant literature than by theoretical or empirical reasons to prefer one theory to the other. The goal of the current work is to revisit this topic by elucidating which of these two models provides a better characterization of latent processes in m -alternative decision tasks, with a particular focus on memory tasks. In a set of visual memory experiments, we show that, within the same experimental design, the softmax parameter β varies across m -alternatives, whereas the parameter d \' of the signal-detection model is stable. Together, our findings indicate that replacing softmax with signal-detection link models would yield more generalizable predictions across changes in task structure. More ambitiously, the invariance of signal detection model parameters across different tasks suggests that the parametric assumptions of these models may be more than just a mathematical convenience, but reflect something real about human decision-making.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    情感识别是亲社会互动的核心,能够推断心理和情感状态。已知犯有性犯罪的人表现出社会情感缺陷,文献中发现的四个动态风险评估维度之一。很少有研究集中在情感识别上。现有文献,专门针对在监狱中犯有性犯罪的个人,显示对比结果。有些人发现了一个全球性的(跨越所有情绪)或特定的(例如,愤怒,恐惧)情感识别中的缺陷。相比之下,其他人发现在监狱中犯有性犯罪的人和犯有非性犯罪的人之间没有区别。此外,尚未在表现出社会情感缺陷的法医住院患者中进行此类研究。这项研究旨在调查112名男性参与者对动态面部表情的识别,这些参与者分为三组:犯有性犯罪的法医住院患者(n=37),犯有非性犯罪的法医住院病人(n=25),和社区成员(n=50),使用信号检测理论指数:灵敏度(d')和响应偏差(c)。此外,与反应时间相关的措施,情感标签反射时间,任务容易,还收集了容易反射时间。非参数分析(Kruskall-Wallis\'H,随后是Mann-Whitney的U与Dunn-Bonferroni更正)强调,与社区成员相比,这两个法医住院组表现出情感识别缺陷。犯有性犯罪的法医住院患者在选择惊喜标签方面比社区成员更为保守。他们还花费了更多的时间来对刺激做出反应并选择情感标签。尽管情感识别缺陷,两个法医住院组报告的刺激容易性比社区成员多。
    Emotion recognition is central in prosocial interaction, enabling the inference of mental and affective states. Individuals who have committed sexual offenses are known to exhibit socio-affective deficits, one of the four dynamic risk assessment dimensions found in the literature. Few research focused on emotion recognition. The available literature, exclusively on individuals in prison who have committed sexual offenses, showed contrasting results. Some found a global (across all emotions) or specific (e.g., anger, fear) deficit in emotion recognition. In contrast, others found no difference between individuals in prison who have committed sexual offenses and those who have committed non-sexual offenses. In addition, no such study has been undertaken among forensic inpatients who exhibit socio-affective deficits. This study aims to investigate the recognition of dynamic facial expressions of emotion in 112 male participants divided into three groups: forensic inpatients who have committed sexual offenses (n = 37), forensic inpatients who have committed non-sexual offenses (n = 25), and community members (n = 50), using the Signal Detection Theory indices: sensitivity (d\') and response bias (c). In addition, measures related to reaction time, emotion labeling reflection time, task easiness, and easiness reflection time were also collected. Non-parametric analyses (Kruskall-Wallis\' H, followed by Mann-Whitney\'s U with Dunn-Bonferroni correction) highlighted that the two forensic inpatient groups exhibited emotion recognition deficits when compared to community members. Forensic inpatients who have committed sexual offenses were more conservative in selecting the surprise label than community members. They also took significantly more time to react to stimuli and to select an emotional label. Despite emotion recognition deficits, the two forensic inpatient groups reported more stimuli easiness than community members.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    先前的研究报告了在将同时阵容与两个不同的阵容程序进行比较时,在区分性和响应偏差方面,结果存在不同的模式,顺序停止规则阵容和英国阵容。在单个大样本实验中,我们比较了同时呈现的六项摄影阵容中的可辨别性和响应偏差,有一个停止规则,或者在进行识别并包括重新访问物品的能力之前,依次要求对物品进行两圈完整的检查,类似于英国的阵容程序。与顺序停止规则阵容相比,同时阵容的可识别性更大,尽管这两个程序之间的经验辨别性没有显著差异。将同时阵容与连续两个阵容以及连续两个圈阵容与连续停止规则阵容进行比较时,可判别性没有显着差异。对于连续的两圈阵容,回应是最宽容的,紧随其后的是同时阵容,其次是顺序阵容。这些结果表明,顺序项目呈现可能不会对辨别性和响应偏差产生很大影响。相反,序贯停止规则阵容和英国阵容中的可辨别性和反应偏差是由于序贯项目呈现与这些程序的其他方面的相互作用所致.
    Previous research has reported diverging patterns of results with respect to discriminability and response bias when comparing the simultaneous lineup to two different lineup procedures in which items are presented sequentially, the sequential stopping rule lineup and the UK lineup. In a single large sample experiment, we compared discriminability and response bias in six-item photographic lineups presented either simultaneously, sequentially with a stopping rule, or sequentially requiring two full laps through the items before making an identification and including the ability to revisit items, analogous to the UK lineup procedure. Discriminability was greater for the simultaneous lineup compared to the sequential stopping rule lineup, despite a non-significant difference in empirical discriminability between the procedures. There was no significant difference in discriminability when comparing the simultaneous lineup to the sequential two lineup and the sequential two lap lineup to the sequential stopping rule lineup. Responding was most lenient for the sequential two lap lineup, followed by the simultaneous lineup, followed by the sequential lineup. These results imply that sequential item presentation may not exert a large effect in isolation on discriminability and response bias. Rather, discriminability and response bias in the sequential stopping rule lineup and UK lineup result from the interaction of sequential item presentation with other aspects of these procedures.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    侦查犬在支持军队方面发挥着关键作用,国土安全和边境保护。一些爆炸物探测任务对狗的身体要求很高,先前的研究表明,这可能导致嗅觉检测灵敏度降低。为了进一步评估运动强度对嗅觉敏感性的影响,我们开发了一种新的嗅觉模式,使我们能够测量嗅觉检测阈值,而狗在跑步机上以两种不同的运动强度运动。狗(n=3)在10-3(v/v)稀释度下对1-溴辛烷的嗅觉检测降低,在更大的运动强度下降低。最低浓度的狗的命中率从低强度行走时的0.87±0.04下降到中等强度小跑时的0.45±0.06以下。这种下降与中等强度运动的持续时间有相互作用,因此,狗在8km/h会话的前10分钟内进行了接近100%的检测,但在20分钟后显示0%的检测。高气味浓度(10-2)的命中率在低(1±0.00)和中等(0.91±0.04)运动强度下都相对稳定。此处开发的范例和设备可能有助于进一步了解狗与操作相关的嗅觉检测阈值下降的原因。
    Detection canines serve critical roles to support the military, homeland security and border protection. Some explosive detection tasks are physically demanding for dogs, and prior research suggests this can lead to a reduction in olfactory detection sensitivity. To further evaluate the effect of exercise intensity on olfactory sensitivity, we developed a novel olfactory paradigm that allowed us to measure olfactory detection thresholds while dogs exercised on a treadmill at two different exercise intensities. Dogs (n = 3) showed a decrement in olfactory detection for 1-bromooctane at 10-3 (v/v) dilutions and lower under greater exercise intensity. Dogs\' hit rate for the lowest concentration dropped from 0.87 ± 0.04 when walking at low intensity to below 0.45 ± 0.06 when trotting at moderate intensity. This decline had an interaction with the duration of the session in moderate intensity exercise, whereby dogs performed near 100% detection in the first 10 min of the 8 km/h session, but showed 0% detection after 20 min. Hit rates for high odor concentrations (10-2) were relatively stable at both low (1 ± 0.00) and moderate (0.91 ± 0.04) exercise intensities. The paradigm and apparatus developed here may be useful to help further understand causes of operationally relevant olfactory detection threshold decline in dogs.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在有情绪障碍的患者中,负面情感偏见-系统地对信息进行优先排序和负面解释-很常见。测试这种偏见的翻译认知任务表明,在模棱两可的决策条件下,抑郁症患者对高回报的偏好降低。强调这种偏见的确切机制是,然而,还不明白。因此,我们开发了一套措施,通过测试行为偏见与参与者的奖励敏感性的关系来探索行为偏见的潜在来源,价值敏感度和奖励学习率。148名参与者完成了三项在线行为任务:原始的模糊线索决策任务,探测负面情感偏见,探索奖励敏感度和奖励学习率的概率奖励学习任务,和探测价值敏感性的赌博任务。我们通过动态信号检测理论模型对学习任务进行了建模,并通过期望最大化前景理论模型对赌博任务进行了建模。概率奖励任务的奖励敏感性(β=0.131,p=0.024)和概率奖励任务的设定噪声(β=-0.187,p=0.028)都预测了逻辑回归中的情感偏差得分。负面情感偏见增加,至少在这个特定的任务上,因此,可能在一定程度上是由对奖励的敏感性降低和反应更加多变的组合所驱动的。
    In patients with mood disorders, negative affective biases - systematically prioritising and interpreting information negatively - are common. A translational cognitive task testing this bias has shown that depressed patients have a reduced preference for a high reward under ambiguous decision-making conditions. The precise mechanisms underscoring this bias are, however, not yet understood. We therefore developed a set of measures to probe the underlying source of the behavioural bias by testing its relationship to a participant\'s reward sensitivity, value sensitivity and reward learning rate. One-hundred-forty-eight participants completed three online behavioural tasks: the original ambiguous-cue decision-making task probing negative affective bias, a probabilistic reward learning task probing reward sensitivity and reward learning rate, and a gambling task probing value sensitivity. We modelled the learning task through a dynamic signal detection theory model and the gambling task through an expectation-maximisation prospect theory model. Reward sensitivity from the probabilistic reward task (β = 0.131, p = 0.024) and setting noise from the probabilistic reward task (β = -0.187, p = 0.028) both predicted the affective bias score in a logistic regression. Increased negative affective bias, at least on this specific task, may therefore be driven in part by a combination of reduced sensitivity to rewards and more variable responses.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    识别记忆的模型通常假设决策是彼此独立做出的。然而,越来越多的证据表明,连续的识别反应显示出顺序的依赖性,从而使一个反应增加了从一个试验到下一个试验重复该反应的可能性。在六个实验中,我们使用单词和非单词刺激复制了这种与反应相关的结转效应,并进一步证明了先前试验的内容-无论是感知的还是概念的-也可以使反应偏向当前的测试探针,感知(正交)和概念(语义)相似性提高了连续“旧”响应的概率。最后,在实验3a和3b中对注意力参与的操纵提供了很少的证据,这些对识别决策的遗留影响仅仅是注意力失误的产物。一起来看,当前的研究加强了先前的发现,即承认决定不是独立做出的,并且多种形式的信息在连续的试验中持续存在。
    Models of recognition memory often assume that decisions are made independently from each other. Yet there is growing evidence that consecutive recognition responses show sequential dependencies, whereby making one response increases the probability of repeating that response from one trial to the next trial. Across six experiments, we replicated this response-related carryover effect using word and nonword stimuli and further demonstrated that the content of the previous trial-both perceptual and conceptual-can also bias the response to the current test probe, with both perceptual (orthographic) and conceptual (semantic) similarity boosting the probability of consecutive \"old\" responses. Finally, a manipulation of attentional engagement in Experiments 3a and 3b provided little evidence these carryover effects on recognition decisions are merely a product of lapses in attention. Taken together, the current study reinforces prior findings that recognition decisions are not made independently, and that multiple forms of information perseverate across consecutive trials.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    事件相关电位(ERP)研究已经确定了与识别记忆相关的两个时间窗口,并将其解释为反映了两个过程:熟悉和回忆。然而,使用相对简单的刺激并获得高识别率,大多数研究集中在命中和正确的拒绝。根据信号检测理论(SDT),这省略了一些信息(未命中和错误警报),这些信息对于理解信号处理是必要的。
    我们使用了困难的视觉识别任务,其中包含不同类别的彩色图片,以获得四种可能的SDT结果中的足够多,并用现代ERP方法对其进行了分析。
    这些结果的非参数分析确定了一个时间窗口(470至670ms),该时间窗口反映了前中央和左后电极簇的活动,指示差分处理。左后聚类显著区分所有STD结果。前端中心集群仅根据受试者的响应区分ERP:是与不。此外,只有左后簇内的电生理活动与辨别指数(d')相关。
    我们表明,当检查所有SDT结果时,识别记忆的ERP反映了一个单一的时间窗口,可以揭示一个自下而上的因素区分项目的历史(即记忆强度),以及指示参与者决策的自上而下因素。
    UNASSIGNED: Event-related potential (ERP) studies have identified two time windows associated with recognition memory and interpreted them as reflecting two processes: familiarity and recollection. However, using relatively simple stimuli and achieving high recognition rates, most studies focused on hits and correct rejections. This leaves out some information (misses and false alarms) that according to Signal Detection Theory (SDT) is necessary to understand signal processing.
    UNASSIGNED: We used a difficult visual recognition task with colored pictures of different categories to obtain enough of the four possible SDT outcomes and analyzed them with modern ERP methods.
    UNASSIGNED: Non-parametric analysis of these outcomes identified a single time window (470 to 670 ms) which reflected activity within fronto-central and posterior-left clusters of electrodes, indicating differential processing. The posterior-left cluster significantly distinguished all STD outcomes. The fronto-central cluster only distinguished ERPs according to the subject\'s response: yes vs. no. Additionally, only electrophysiological activity within the posterior-left cluster correlated with the discrimination index (d\').
    UNASSIGNED: We show that when all SDT outcomes are examined, ERPs of recognition memory reflect a single-time window that may reveal a bottom-up factor discriminating the history of items (i.e. memory strength), as well as a top-down factor indicating participants\' decision.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号