Saccades

扫视
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    众所周知,注意力是由显著对象或事件捕获的。视觉领域中存在的显著信息吸引注意力的概念也是许多有影响力的注意力模型的核心。这些模型通常假定显著性的层次结构,这表明注意力从视野中最突出的项目发展到最不突出的项目。然而,尽管这种说法在各种模型中具有重要意义,对眼球运动的研究挑战了搜索严格遵循这种显著性层次的想法。相反,眼动追踪研究表明,显著性信息具有短暂的影响,只会影响到最突出物体的初始扫视,并且仅在显示开始后迅速执行。虽然这些关于明显眼球运动的发现很重要,它们没有解决在扫视开始之前发生的秘密注意过程。在目前的一系列实验中,我们探讨了是否存在二次捕获的证据-在初次捕获发作后是否可以通过另一个显著项目捕获注意力.为了探索这个,我们利用具有不同显著性水平的多个干扰物的显示。我们的主要问题是,两个具有不同显着性水平的干扰因素是否会比单个干扰干扰搜索更多,高度突出的分心者。在三个实验中,有明确的证据表明,两个干扰物对搜索的干扰比单个干扰物的干扰更大。这一观察表明,在最初捕获之后,发生了下一个最突出的干扰物的二次捕获。这些发现共同支持了隐蔽注意力贯穿显著性层次的观点。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2024APA,保留所有权利)。
    It is well known that attention is captured by salient objects or events. The notion that attention is attracted by salience information present in the visual field is also at the heart of many influential models of attention. These models typically posit a hierarchy of saliency, suggesting that attention progresses from the most to the least salient item in the visual field. However, despite the significance of this claim in various models, research on eye movements challenges the idea that search strictly follows this saliency hierarchy. Instead, eye-tracking studies have suggested that saliency information has a transient impact, only influencing the initial saccade toward the most salient object, and only if executed swiftly after display onset. While these findings on overt eye movements are important, they do not address covert attentional processes occurring before a saccade is initiated. In the current series of experiments, we explored whether there was evidence for secondary capture-whether attention could be captured by another salient item after the initial capture episode. To explore this, we utilized displays with multiple distractors of varying levels of saliency. Our primary question was whether two distractors with different saliency levels would disrupt search more than a single, highly salient distractor. Across three experiments, clear evidence emerged indicating that two distractors interfered more with search than a single salient distractor. This observation suggests that following initial capture, secondary capture by the next most salient distractor occurred. These findings collectively support the idea that covert attention traverses the saliency hierarchy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:良性阵发性位置性眩晕(BPPV)患者可能会混淆哪个管受累,尤其是那些有微妙发现的人。该研究旨在确定是否可以在此类患者中使用视频头脉冲测试作为诊断工具。BPPV的症状评分和治疗效率是该过程的重要组成部分。因此,像“头晕障碍库存”这样的库存在这方面可能是有用的。
    方法:纳入后管和外侧管BPPV患者。在治疗前和治疗后1周进行视频头脉冲测试。注意到前庭眼反射(VOR)的增加,并与另一侧进行比较。还注意到存在校正扫视。此外,比较治疗前和治疗后头晕障碍量表评分。
    结果:57例患者被诊断为后管BPPV,16例患有水平运河BPPV。在后管BPPV患者中,受累运河VOR增益与同一侧的其他运河之间没有差异(P=.639)。涉及的水平运河与相对的水平运河没有区别。与后管BPPV患者相比,侧管BPPV患者在治疗后表现出更显著的改善。
    结论:视频头脉冲测试可能无法用于评估BPPV的受累管;但是,它可以用来评估治疗的效率,尤其是在侧管。
    BACKGROUND:  There may be confusion about which canal is involved in patients with benign paroxysmal positional vertigo (BPPV), especially with those that have subtle findings. The study aimed to determine if video head impulse testing may be used in such patients as a diagnostic tool. Symptom scoring and treatment efficiency in BPPV are essential parts of the process. Therefore, inventories like \"Dizziness Handicap Inventory\" may be useful in this regard.
    METHODS:  Patients with posterior and lateral canal BPPV were included. Video head impulse testing was performed prior to treatment and 1 week after treatment. Vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) gains were noted and compared to the opposite side. The presence of correction saccades was noted as well. Also, pretreatment and posttreatment Dizziness Handicap Inventory scores were compared.
    RESULTS:  Fifty-seven patients were diagnosed with posterior canal BPPV, and sixteen were with horizontal canal BPPV. In patients with posterior canal BPPV, there was no difference between the involved canal VOR gains and the other canals on the same side (P=.639). The involved horizontal canal did not differ from the opposite horizontal canal. Patients with lateral canal BPPV show more significant improvement after treatment compared to patients with posterior canal BPPV.
    CONCLUSIONS:  Video head impulse testing may not be used to estimate the involved canal in BPPV; however, it may be used to evaluate the efficiency of the treatment, especially in the lateral canal.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们使用纯粹的扫视反应研究了在感知决策任务中是否相对于目标或固定发生干扰物抑制。先前的研究表明,在区分目标和干扰物的过程中,对目标的扫视偏离了分心器。一旦被歧视,干扰者被抑制,轨迹偏离了干扰物。扫视偏差大小根据目标干扰物之间的距离提供了对目标干扰物竞争的敏感度量。在以自我为中心的参考系中计划扫视时(相对于固定表示的位置),基于对象的抑制已被证明发生在非中心参考系中(对象相对于彼此独立地表示)。通过改变目标和干扰物的自我中心和分配中心距离,我们发现,在主动决策过程中,只有以自我为中心的距离才会导致扫视轨迹向分心者转移。当感知决策过程完成时,干扰者被抑制了,自我中心距离和分配中心距离都独立地导致扫视轨迹远离干扰物。这与独立的基于空间和对象的抑制机制一致。因此,我们建议使用分配中心图在皮层视觉区域保持干扰抑制,然后将其输入动眼区域进行扫视计划。
    We investigated whether distractor inhibition occurs relative to the target or fixation in a perceptual decision-making task using a purely saccadic response. Previous research has shown that during the process of discriminating a target from distractor, saccades made to a target deviate towards the distractor. Once discriminated, the distractor is inhibited, and trajectories deviate away from the distractor. Saccade deviation magnitudes provide a sensitive measure of target-distractor competition dependent on the distance between them. While saccades are planned in an egocentric reference frame (locations represented relative to fixation), object-based inhibition has been shown to occur in an allocentric reference frame (objects represented relative to each other independent of fixation). By varying the egocentric and allocentric distances of the target and distractor, we found that only egocentric distances contributed to saccade trajectories shifts towards the distractor during active decision-making. When the perceptual decision-making process was complete, and the distractor was inhibited, both ego- and allocentric distances independently contributed to saccade trajectory shifts away from the distractor. This is consistent with independent spatial and object-based inhibitory mechanisms. Therefore, we suggest that distractor inhibition is maintained in cortical visual areas with allocentric maps which then feeds into oculomotor areas for saccade planning.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对未来刺激的期望增加了一旦实际出现就采取行动的准备,并导致适当运动反应的延迟减少。真实世界事件在空间和/或时间上都是不确定的,但是这种不确定性本身是可以预期的。在存在预期的空间和时间不确定性的情况下,哪一个应该由电机系统优先考虑可能取决于上下文。因此,我们研究了在准备扫视眼球运动过程中预期的空间和时间不确定性的相对权重。反应时间任务在警告和命令性视觉刺激之间具有可变的前周期。提示与刺激相关的预期时间和/或空间不确定性。我们发现在命令性刺激开始之前,瞳孔扩张随着预期的时间不确定性而增加,但未因空间不确定性而改变。在命令性刺激发作后,这两种类型的预期不确定性都会影响扫视延迟。最大眼速仅由预期的空间不确定性调制。总之,预期的时间和空间不确定性对运动反应的准备和执行没有相同的影响。根据任务期间不断发展的预期不确定性环境,可以对相关信息进行优先排序。
    Expectation of a future stimulus increases the preparedness to act once it actually appears and results in reduced latency of the appropriate motor response. Real world events are uncertain both spatially and/or temporally but this uncertainty could itself be expected. In the presence of both expected spatial and temporal uncertainty, which one should be prioritized by the motor system could depend on the context. Therefore, we investigated the relative weight of expected spatial and temporal uncertainty during the preparation of a saccadic eye movement. A reaction time task was used with a variable foreperiod between a warning and an imperative visual stimuli. Expected temporal and/or spatial uncertainty associated with the stimulus was cued. We found that before imperative stimulus onset, pupil dilation increased with expected temporal uncertainty but was unaltered by spatial uncertainty. After imperative stimulus onset, both types of expected uncertainty affected saccade latency. Maximum eye velocity was modulated by expected spatial uncertainty only. In conclusion, expected temporal and spatial uncertainty do not have the same impact on preparation and execution of a motor response. There could be a prioritization of the relevant information as a function of the evolving expected uncertainty context during the task.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当使用扫视眼球运动(扫视)与视觉世界互动时,视觉刺激的感知位置变得有偏见,一种叫做预兆误定位的现象。然而,这种改变的视觉空间感知的潜在神经机制及其与其他周围感知现象的潜在联系尚未建立。使用四只雄性猕猴的神经外区域的电生理记录,结合计算模型,我们能够根据统计模型捕获的跨时空敏感性的周期动力学,量化扫视目标(ST)周围的空间偏差.这种方法可以预测ST周围的空间偏差,与行为数据一致,并揭示了表征偏差背后的精确神经元反应成分。这些发现还确立了ST附近的敏感性增加对于具有远离ST的感受野的神经元在驱动ST空间偏差中的关键作用。此外,我们证明了,通过为视觉目标表示分配更多资源,视觉区域增强了它们对ST位置的表示,即使以空间表示中的瞬态失真为代价。周围ST表示的这种潜在神经基础也支持跨神经元在创建刺激位置感知中的一般作用。
    When interacting with the visual world using saccadic eye movements (saccades), the perceived location of visual stimuli becomes biased, a phenomenon called perisaccadic mislocalization. However, the neural mechanism underlying this altered visuospatial perception and its potential link to other perisaccadic perceptual phenomena have not been established. Using the electrophysiological recording of extrastriate areas in four male macaque monkeys, combined with a computational model, we were able to quantify spatial bias around the saccade target (ST) based on the perisaccadic dynamics of extrastriate spatiotemporal sensitivity captured by a statistical model. This approach could predict the perisaccadic spatial bias around the ST, consistent with behavioral data, and revealed the precise neuronal response components underlying representational bias. These findings also establish the crucial role of increased sensitivity near the ST for neurons with receptive fields far from the ST in driving the ST spatial bias. Moreover, we showed that, by allocating more resources for visual target representation, visual areas enhance their representation of the ST location, even at the expense of transient distortions in spatial representation. This potential neural basis for perisaccadic ST representation also supports a general role for extrastriate neurons in creating the perception of stimulus location.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究调查了在文化景观和旅游相关视觉刺激的背景下,眼动追踪指标与情感体验之间的关系。53名参与者参与了两个实验:43名在数据收集阶段,10名在模型验证阶段。记录眼球运动并分析数据,以确定四个眼球跟踪指标-平均扫视次数(ANS)之间的相关性。总停留固定(TDF),固定计数(FC),和平均瞳孔扩张(APD)-和19种不同的情感体验,随后分为三类:阳性,中性,和消极。该研究检查了整个建筑,历史,经济,和生活景观,以及旅游的三个主要阶段:进入,核心,和离开。研究结果表明,建筑和历史景观需要更高水平的视觉和认知参与,尤其是在核心阶段。逐步回归分析确定了情绪体验的四个关键眼动追踪预测因子,能够开发预测模型。这项研究强调了眼动追踪技术在捕捉和预测对不同景观类型的情绪反应方面的有效性,为优化乡村旅游环境和增强游客的情感体验提供有价值的见解。
    This study investigates the relationship between eye-tracking metrics and emotional experiences in the context of cultural landscapes and tourism-related visual stimuli. Fifty-three participants were involved in two experiments: forty-three in the data collection phase and ten in the model validation phase. Eye movements were recorded and the data were analyzed to identify correlations between four eye-tracking metrics-average number of saccades (ANS), total dwell fixation (TDF), fixation count (FC), and average pupil dilation (APD)-and 19 distinct emotional experiences, which were subsequently grouped into three categories: positive, neutral, and negative. The study examined the variations in eye-tracking metrics across architectural, historic, economic, and life landscapes, as well as the three primary phases of a tour: entry, core, and departure. Findings revealed that architectural and historic landscapes demanded higher levels of visual and cognitive engagement, especially during the core phase. Stepwise regression analysis identified four key eye-tracking predictors for emotional experiences, enabling the development of a prediction model. This research underscores the effectiveness of eye-tracking technology in capturing and predicting emotional responses to different landscape types, offering valuable insights for optimizing rural tourism environments and enhancing visitors\' emotional experiences.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    接受野(RF)的预测性重映射被认为是在眼球运动期间增强感知稳定性的关键机制之一。虽然已经在几个皮质区域观察到RF重新映射,它在早期视觉皮层中的作用及其对神经元调谐特性的影响一直知之甚少。这里,当受试者执行提示扫视任务时,我们从视觉区域V2跟踪数百个神经元中的重新映射RF。我们发现,在所有记录的皮质层和细胞类型的神经元中,区域V2中的重映射很普遍。此外,我们的结果表明,由于未调节的抑制,重新定位的RFs不仅维持了特征选择性,而且暂时增强了特征选择性.一起来看,这些发现揭示了早期视觉皮层重新映射的动态和普遍性,迫使我们在扫视眼球运动期间修改当前的感知稳定性模型。
    Predictive remapping of receptive fields (RFs) is thought to be one of the critical mechanisms for enforcing perceptual stability during eye movements. While RF remapping has been observed in several cortical areas, its role in early visual cortex and its consequences on the tuning properties of neurons have been poorly understood. Here, we track remapping RFs in hundreds of neurons from visual area V2 while subjects perform a cued saccade task. We find that remapping is widespread in area V2 across neurons from all recorded cortical layers and cell types. Furthermore, our results suggest that remapping RFs not only maintain but also transiently enhance their feature selectivity due to untuned suppression. Taken together, these findings shed light on the dynamics and prevalence of remapping in the early visual cortex, forcing us to revise current models of perceptual stability during saccadic eye movements.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    单一单位(SU)活动-从一个神经元分离的动作电位-传统上被用来将神经元活动与行为联系起来。然而,最近的研究表明,多单位(MU)活动-在一个微电极附近记录的整体神经活动-也可能包含与任务相关的神经群体动力学的准确估计。这里,使用既定的模型拟合方法,在记忆引导的扫视任务中,我们将SU响应场的空间代码与从头部不受约束的猴子(Macacamulatta)的前眼场(FEF)记录的相应MU响应场进行了比较。总的来说,SU和MU群体都显示出简单的视觉运动转换:视觉响应编码的目标在眼睛坐标,在延迟期间逐渐向扫视运动响应中的未来凝视代码过渡。然而,SU人口显示出额外的二级代码,包括视觉响应中的预测性注视代码和运动响应中的目标代码的保留。Further,当SU被分成规则/快速尖峰神经元时,这些细胞类型在延迟后期显示出不同的空间代码进展,仅在最后的扫视运动响应期间收敛于注视编码。最后,重建MU群体(通过对相同位点内的SU数据求和)未能复制SU或MU模式。这些结果证实了MU活性记录作为基本感觉运动转化的生物标志物的理论和实践潜力(例如,动眼系统中的目标到凝视编码),同时也强调了SU活动对编码更微妙的重要性(例如,预测/记忆)感觉运动行为的方面。显著性陈述多单位记录(来自多个神经元的未分化信号)相对容易记录,并提供神经动力学的简化估计,但是不清楚保留了哪些单单元信号,放大,或丢失。这里,我们比较了单/多单位活动从一个明确的结构(额眼领域)和行为(记忆延迟扫视任务),通过时间跟踪他们的空间代码。在单单位活动中观察到的从目标到凝视编码的渐进转换保留在多单位活动中,但其他认知信号(初始视觉反应内的凝视预测,最终运动响应内的目标记忆,和细胞特异性延迟信号)丢失。这表明多单位活动为健康的感觉运动转化提供了极好的生物标志物,以错过更微妙的认知信号为代价。
    Single-unit (SU) activity-action potentials isolated from one neuron-has traditionally been employed to relate neuronal activity to behavior. However, recent investigations have shown that multiunit (MU) activity-ensemble neural activity recorded within the vicinity of one microelectrode-may also contain accurate estimations of task-related neural population dynamics. Here, using an established model-fitting approach, we compared the spatial codes of SU response fields with corresponding MU response fields recorded from the frontal eye fields (FEFs) in head-unrestrained monkeys (Macaca mulatta) during a memory-guided saccade task. Overall, both SU and MU populations showed a simple visuomotor transformation: the visual response coded target-in-eye coordinates, transitioning progressively during the delay toward a future gaze-in-eye code in the saccade motor response. However, the SU population showed additional secondary codes, including a predictive gaze code in the visual response and retention of a target code in the motor response. Further, when SUs were separated into regular/fast spiking neurons, these cell types showed different spatial code progressions during the late delay period, only converging toward gaze coding during the final saccade motor response. Finally, reconstructing MU populations (by summing SU data within the same sites) failed to replicate either the SU or MU pattern. These results confirm the theoretical and practical potential of MU activity recordings as a biomarker for fundamental sensorimotor transformations (e.g., target-to-gaze coding in the oculomotor system), while also highlighting the importance of SU activity for coding more subtle (e.g., predictive/memory) aspects of sensorimotor behavior.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    单次运动可增强执行功能(EF),并可能与脑血流量(CBF)的增加有关。当前文献中的一个局限性是,鉴于一些证据表明整个月经周期中激素水平的变化会影响生理和心理变量,女性参与者在生物学上的代表性不足。这里,生物学上,女性参与者在月经周期的卵泡期(FOL)和黄体期(LUT)分别完成了单次中等强度运动(估计乳酸阈值的80%).此外,生物学上男性参与者完成了相同持续时间/强度的运动。使用大脑中动脉速度(MCAv)来估计CBF,并通过反扫视任务评估运动前后的EF。结果表明,静息MCAv在LUT比FOL阶段更大;然而,运动介导的MCAv增加在月经周期阶段之间相当,女性和男性参与者之间。从运动前到运动后,反视反应时间可靠地减少了,频率学家和非频率学家的统计数据表明,在FOL和LUT阶段,减少的幅度相等。女性和男性参与者之间。因此,结果表明,月经周期状态不应作为限制女性参与者参与运动和EF检查研究的基础。
    A single bout of exercise enhances executive function (EF) and may relate to an increase in cerebral blood flow (CBF). A limitation in the current literature is that biologically female participants are underrepresented given some evidence that changes in hormone levels across the menstrual cycle impact physiological and psychological variables. Here, biologically female participants completed separate single bouts of moderate intensity exercise (80% of estimated lactate threshold) during the follicular (FOL) and luteal (LUT) phases of their menstrual cycle. In addition, biologically male participants completed a same duration/intensity exercise session. Middle cerebral artery velocity (MCAv) was used to estimate CBF and pre- and postexercise EF was assessed via the antisaccade task. Results showed that resting MCAv was larger in the LUT than FOL phase; however, the exercise-mediated increase in MCAv was equivalent between menstrual cycle phases, and between female and male participants. Antisaccade reaction times reliably decreased from pre- to postexercise and frequentist and non-frequentist statistics demonstrated that the magnitude of the decrease was equivalent across FOL and LUT phases, and between female and male participants. Thus, results evince that menstrual cycle status should not serve as a basis limiting biologically female participants\' inclusion in research examining exercise and EF.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们每天都要进行数千次扫视,并在探索环境时迈出数千步。尽管它们在典型的活动状态下共同出现,我们对眼球运动之间的协调知之甚少,行走行为和皮质活动的相关变化。技术限制一直是一个主要障碍,我们在这里通过利用具有三维(3D)位置跟踪的沉浸式无线虚拟现实(VR)环境的优势来克服这些优势,同时记录眼球运动和移动脑电图(EEG)。使用这种方法,参与者不受阻碍地沿着一条清晰的道路行走,水平路径,我们发现,在缓慢和自然的步行速度下,眼球运动的可能性都会影响脚步的节奏,在每一步的脚跟撞击后达到峰值。与以往的研究相比,这种夹带是在一项不需要视觉引导步进的任务中捕获的-这表明运动和视觉运动功能之间存在持续的相互作用.同时进行的EEG记录显示,伴随着脚跟撞击的调制,随着频率范围宽的振荡功率的增加和减少。这些影响的峰值发生在缓慢和自然步行速度的θ和α范围内,分别。一起,我们的数据表明,步进周期的阶段会影响其他行为,如眼球运动,并按照相同的节律模式产生同步EEG的相关调制。这些结果表明,步态是在主动观察者中解释扫视和时频EEG数据时要考虑的重要因素,并证明步态的快速夹带可能会持续到整个日常活动中。
    Every day we make thousands of saccades and take thousands of steps as we explore our environment. Despite their common co-occurrence in a typical active state, we know little about the coordination between eye movements, walking behaviour and related changes in cortical activity. Technical limitations have been a major impediment, which we overcome here by leveraging the advantages of an immersive wireless virtual reality (VR) environment with three-dimensional (3D) position tracking, together with simultaneous recording of eye movements and mobile electroencephalography (EEG). Using this approach with participants engaged in unencumbered walking along a clear, level path, we find that the likelihood of eye movements at both slow and natural walking speeds entrains to the rhythm of footfall, peaking after the heel-strike of each step. Compared to previous research, this entrainment was captured in a task that did not require visually guided stepping - suggesting a persistent interaction between locomotor and visuomotor functions. Simultaneous EEG recordings reveal a concomitant modulation entrained to heel-strike, with increases and decreases in oscillatory power for a broad range of frequencies. The peak of these effects occurred in the theta and alpha range for slow and natural walking speeds, respectively. Together, our data show that the phase of the step-cycle influences other behaviours such as eye movements, and produces related modulations of simultaneous EEG following the same rhythmic pattern. These results reveal gait as an important factor to be considered when interpreting saccadic and time-frequency EEG data in active observers, and demonstrate that saccadic entrainment to gait may persist throughout everyday activities.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号