Saccades

扫视
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项横断面研究检查了4至16岁患者的眼球运动表现。在进行抑制四个原始反射的治疗之前和之后,获得了眼球运动的测量值,不对称补品颈部反射,对称滋补颈部反射,迷宫滋补反射和莫罗反射。随后,将四个原始反射的得分与五个变量的结果进行了比较:固定维持,%平均扫视大小,运动性短途旅行,短途旅行期间的注视和平均注视持续时间。比较显示,由于四个原始反射的抑制,固定维持的证据以及平均扫视大小显着减少。眼运动性也显着增加,而每次扫视的固定次数和平均固定时间也显着减少。在所有测试中,双眼的值之间的视觉平衡得到改善。一种叫做VisagraphTMIII的装置,测量眼球运动,用于数据收集。这些结果表明,眼球运动的改善反映了其他成熟过程的参与,例如原始反射的出现和抑制,整个重组是未来阅读和注意力过程的关键。
    This cross-sectional study examined eye movement performance in patients aged 4 to 16 years. Measurements of eye movements were obtained before and after performing therapy for inhibition of four primitive reflexes, asymmetric tonic neck reflex, symmetric tonic neck reflex, labyrinthine tonic reflex and Moro reflex. Subsequently the scores of the four primitive reflexes were compared with the results of five variables: fixation maintenance, % mean saccade size, motility excursions, fixations during excursions and mean duration of fixations. The comparisons showed a significant reduction in evidence of fixation maintenance as well as mean saccade size due to the inhibition of the four primitive reflexes. There was also a significant increase in ocular motility while fixations per saccade and average duration of fixations also decreased significantly. Visual balance between values of both eyes improved in all tests. A device called VisagraphTM III, which measures eye movements, was used for data collection. These results suggest that the oculomotor improvements reflect the involvement of other maturational processes such as the emergence and inhibition of primitive reflexes, the whole reorganization being key to future reading and attentional processes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最先进的眼睛跟踪器通过测量和解释人们阅读文本时的注视路径,为诊断阅读问题提供有价值的信息。视野缺陷等异常情况,然而,可能会严重混淆当今大多数现有的解读阅读凝视模式的方法。我们的目标是研究视野缺陷如何影响阅读凝视路径模式,因此,这种神经病理学的影响可以明确地纳入更全面的阅读诊断方法中。横截面,非随机化,设计了包括45名患有各种神经系统疾病的患者和30名正常对照的初步临床研究。参与者使用两个单词和数字阅读测试进行了眼科/神经心理学和眼动仪检查。结果表明,使用眼动仪表明,患有脑损伤和视野改变的患者需要更多的时间来完成阅读文本测试,方法是固定更多的次数(p<0.001);固定时间更长(p=0.03);这些患者的扫视次数更多(p=0.04)。我们的研究表明,抱怨阅读困难的神经系统疾病和视野改变的患者在眼球运动特征方面存在客观差异。这些发现应被视为偏见因素,值得进一步调查。
    State-of-the-art eye trackers provide valuable information for diagnosing reading problems by measuring and interpreting people\'s gaze paths as they read through text. Abnormal conditions such as visual field defects, however, can seriously confound most of today\'s existing methods for interpreting reading gaze patterns. Our objective was to research how visual field defects impact reading gaze path patterns, so the effects of such neurological pathologies can be explicitly incorporated into more comprehensive reading diagnosis methodologies. A cross-sectional, non-randomized, pilot clinical study including 45 patients with various neurologic disorders and 30 normal controls was designed. Participants underwent ophthalmologic/neuropsychologic and eye-tracker examinations using two reading tests of words and numbers. The results showed that the use of the eye tracker showed that patients with brain damage and an altered visual field require more time to complete a reading-text test by fixating a greater number of times (p < 0.001); with longer fixations (p = 0.03); and a greater number of saccades in these patients (p = 0.04). Our study showed objective differences in eye movement characteristics in patients with neurological diseases and an altered visual field who complained of reading difficulties. These findings should be considered as a bias factor and deserve further investigation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们在这项研究中的目的是通过分析眼部行为来了解我们如何执行视觉空间比较任务,并研究黄斑或周边视觉的限制如何干扰眼部行为和任务执行。两组18名正常或矫正视力的健康参与者进行了视觉空间比较任务(基本视觉空间感知[EVSP]测试的计算机化版本)(Pisella等人。,2013年),带有凝视视面罩,模拟管状视觉(第一组)或黄斑暗点(第二组)。在这些病理状况的模拟之后,所有参与者还在全视角下进行了EVSP测试,能够直接比较它们的动眼行为和性能。在动眼行为方面,与全视图条件相比,在没有周边视觉的情况下,要比较的两个物体之间的交替扫视次数较少,而在没有黄斑视力的情况下,物体内探查扫视的数量减少。除了中线判断外,周围视力的缺失不影响准确性,但是缺乏中央视觉会损害所有视觉空间子测试的准确性。除了确认黄斑在视觉空间比较任务中的关键作用,这些实验提供了重要的见解,感觉障碍如何改变眼球运动行为,无论是否影响表现准确性。
    Our aim in this study was to understand how we perform visuospatial comparison tasks by analyzing ocular behavior and to examine how restrictions in macular or peripheral vision disturb ocular behavior and task performance. Two groups of 18 healthy participants with normal or corrected visual acuity performed visuospatial comparison tasks (computerized version of the elementary visuospatial perception [EVSP] test) (Pisella et al., 2013) with a gaze-contingent mask simulating either tubular vision (first group) or macular scotoma (second group). After these simulations of pathological conditions, all participants also performed the EVSP test in full view, enabling direct comparison of their oculomotor behavior and performance. In terms of oculomotor behavior, compared with the full view condition, alternation saccades between the two objects to compare were less numerous in the absence of peripheral vision, whereas the number of within-object exploration saccades decreased in the absence of macular vision. The absence of peripheral vision did not affect accuracy except for midline judgments, but the absence of central vision impaired accuracy across all visuospatial subtests. Besides confirming the crucial role of the macula for visuospatial comparison tasks, these experiments provided important insights into how sensory disorder modifies oculomotor behavior with or without consequences on performance accuracy.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    许多研究试图确定阅读障碍的根本原因。不同的阅读障碍理论提出了语音,注意,或视觉缺陷。虽然研究已经使用眼动追踪来研究阅读障碍,之前只有两项研究使用移动窗口范式来探索阅读障碍阅读中的感知跨度,没有人在视觉搜索中这样做。本研究使用阅读和视觉搜索任务分析了感知范围,以识别阅读障碍中与语言无关的注意力障碍。我们发现模棱两可的证据表明,阅读障碍者的感知范围受到损害,而视觉搜索没有损害的证据。然而,阅读障碍参与者确实在视觉搜索任务中表现出缺陷,与对照组相比,搜索精度较低,扫视较短。这些结果为视觉提供了支持,而不是注意力或语音,诵读困难的帐户。
    Many studies have attempted to identify the root cause of dyslexia. Different theories of dyslexia have proposed either a phonological, attentional, or visual deficit. While research has used eye-tracking to study dyslexia, only two previous studies have used the moving-window paradigm to explore the perceptual span in dyslexic reading, and none have done so in visual search. The present study analysed the perceptual span using both reading and visual search tasks to identify language-independent attentional impairments in dyslexics. We found equivocal evidence that the perceptual span was impaired in dyslexic reading and no evidence of impairment in visual search. However, dyslexic participants did show deficits in the visual search task, with lower search accuracy and shorter saccades compared with controls. These results lend support for a visual, rather than attentional or phonological, account of dyslexia.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:良性阵发性位置性眩晕(BPPV)患者可能会混淆哪个管受累,尤其是那些有微妙发现的人。该研究旨在确定是否可以在此类患者中使用视频头脉冲测试作为诊断工具。BPPV的症状评分和治疗效率是该过程的重要组成部分。因此,像“头晕障碍库存”这样的库存在这方面可能是有用的。
    方法:纳入后管和外侧管BPPV患者。在治疗前和治疗后1周进行视频头脉冲测试。注意到前庭眼反射(VOR)的增加,并与另一侧进行比较。还注意到存在校正扫视。此外,比较治疗前和治疗后头晕障碍量表评分。
    结果:57例患者被诊断为后管BPPV,16例患有水平运河BPPV。在后管BPPV患者中,受累运河VOR增益与同一侧的其他运河之间没有差异(P=.639)。涉及的水平运河与相对的水平运河没有区别。与后管BPPV患者相比,侧管BPPV患者在治疗后表现出更显著的改善。
    结论:视频头脉冲测试可能无法用于评估BPPV的受累管;但是,它可以用来评估治疗的效率,尤其是在侧管。
    BACKGROUND:  There may be confusion about which canal is involved in patients with benign paroxysmal positional vertigo (BPPV), especially with those that have subtle findings. The study aimed to determine if video head impulse testing may be used in such patients as a diagnostic tool. Symptom scoring and treatment efficiency in BPPV are essential parts of the process. Therefore, inventories like \"Dizziness Handicap Inventory\" may be useful in this regard.
    METHODS:  Patients with posterior and lateral canal BPPV were included. Video head impulse testing was performed prior to treatment and 1 week after treatment. Vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) gains were noted and compared to the opposite side. The presence of correction saccades was noted as well. Also, pretreatment and posttreatment Dizziness Handicap Inventory scores were compared.
    RESULTS:  Fifty-seven patients were diagnosed with posterior canal BPPV, and sixteen were with horizontal canal BPPV. In patients with posterior canal BPPV, there was no difference between the involved canal VOR gains and the other canals on the same side (P=.639). The involved horizontal canal did not differ from the opposite horizontal canal. Patients with lateral canal BPPV show more significant improvement after treatment compared to patients with posterior canal BPPV.
    CONCLUSIONS:  Video head impulse testing may not be used to estimate the involved canal in BPPV; however, it may be used to evaluate the efficiency of the treatment, especially in the lateral canal.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们使用纯粹的扫视反应研究了在感知决策任务中是否相对于目标或固定发生干扰物抑制。先前的研究表明,在区分目标和干扰物的过程中,对目标的扫视偏离了分心器。一旦被歧视,干扰者被抑制,轨迹偏离了干扰物。扫视偏差大小根据目标干扰物之间的距离提供了对目标干扰物竞争的敏感度量。在以自我为中心的参考系中计划扫视时(相对于固定表示的位置),基于对象的抑制已被证明发生在非中心参考系中(对象相对于彼此独立地表示)。通过改变目标和干扰物的自我中心和分配中心距离,我们发现,在主动决策过程中,只有以自我为中心的距离才会导致扫视轨迹向分心者转移。当感知决策过程完成时,干扰者被抑制了,自我中心距离和分配中心距离都独立地导致扫视轨迹远离干扰物。这与独立的基于空间和对象的抑制机制一致。因此,我们建议使用分配中心图在皮层视觉区域保持干扰抑制,然后将其输入动眼区域进行扫视计划。
    We investigated whether distractor inhibition occurs relative to the target or fixation in a perceptual decision-making task using a purely saccadic response. Previous research has shown that during the process of discriminating a target from distractor, saccades made to a target deviate towards the distractor. Once discriminated, the distractor is inhibited, and trajectories deviate away from the distractor. Saccade deviation magnitudes provide a sensitive measure of target-distractor competition dependent on the distance between them. While saccades are planned in an egocentric reference frame (locations represented relative to fixation), object-based inhibition has been shown to occur in an allocentric reference frame (objects represented relative to each other independent of fixation). By varying the egocentric and allocentric distances of the target and distractor, we found that only egocentric distances contributed to saccade trajectories shifts towards the distractor during active decision-making. When the perceptual decision-making process was complete, and the distractor was inhibited, both ego- and allocentric distances independently contributed to saccade trajectory shifts away from the distractor. This is consistent with independent spatial and object-based inhibitory mechanisms. Therefore, we suggest that distractor inhibition is maintained in cortical visual areas with allocentric maps which then feeds into oculomotor areas for saccade planning.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对未来刺激的期望增加了一旦实际出现就采取行动的准备,并导致适当运动反应的延迟减少。真实世界事件在空间和/或时间上都是不确定的,但是这种不确定性本身是可以预期的。在存在预期的空间和时间不确定性的情况下,哪一个应该由电机系统优先考虑可能取决于上下文。因此,我们研究了在准备扫视眼球运动过程中预期的空间和时间不确定性的相对权重。反应时间任务在警告和命令性视觉刺激之间具有可变的前周期。提示与刺激相关的预期时间和/或空间不确定性。我们发现在命令性刺激开始之前,瞳孔扩张随着预期的时间不确定性而增加,但未因空间不确定性而改变。在命令性刺激发作后,这两种类型的预期不确定性都会影响扫视延迟。最大眼速仅由预期的空间不确定性调制。总之,预期的时间和空间不确定性对运动反应的准备和执行没有相同的影响。根据任务期间不断发展的预期不确定性环境,可以对相关信息进行优先排序。
    Expectation of a future stimulus increases the preparedness to act once it actually appears and results in reduced latency of the appropriate motor response. Real world events are uncertain both spatially and/or temporally but this uncertainty could itself be expected. In the presence of both expected spatial and temporal uncertainty, which one should be prioritized by the motor system could depend on the context. Therefore, we investigated the relative weight of expected spatial and temporal uncertainty during the preparation of a saccadic eye movement. A reaction time task was used with a variable foreperiod between a warning and an imperative visual stimuli. Expected temporal and/or spatial uncertainty associated with the stimulus was cued. We found that before imperative stimulus onset, pupil dilation increased with expected temporal uncertainty but was unaltered by spatial uncertainty. After imperative stimulus onset, both types of expected uncertainty affected saccade latency. Maximum eye velocity was modulated by expected spatial uncertainty only. In conclusion, expected temporal and spatial uncertainty do not have the same impact on preparation and execution of a motor response. There could be a prioritization of the relevant information as a function of the evolving expected uncertainty context during the task.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当使用扫视眼球运动(扫视)与视觉世界互动时,视觉刺激的感知位置变得有偏见,一种叫做预兆误定位的现象。然而,这种改变的视觉空间感知的潜在神经机制及其与其他周围感知现象的潜在联系尚未建立。使用四只雄性猕猴的神经外区域的电生理记录,结合计算模型,我们能够根据统计模型捕获的跨时空敏感性的周期动力学,量化扫视目标(ST)周围的空间偏差.这种方法可以预测ST周围的空间偏差,与行为数据一致,并揭示了表征偏差背后的精确神经元反应成分。这些发现还确立了ST附近的敏感性增加对于具有远离ST的感受野的神经元在驱动ST空间偏差中的关键作用。此外,我们证明了,通过为视觉目标表示分配更多资源,视觉区域增强了它们对ST位置的表示,即使以空间表示中的瞬态失真为代价。周围ST表示的这种潜在神经基础也支持跨神经元在创建刺激位置感知中的一般作用。
    When interacting with the visual world using saccadic eye movements (saccades), the perceived location of visual stimuli becomes biased, a phenomenon called perisaccadic mislocalization. However, the neural mechanism underlying this altered visuospatial perception and its potential link to other perisaccadic perceptual phenomena have not been established. Using the electrophysiological recording of extrastriate areas in four male macaque monkeys, combined with a computational model, we were able to quantify spatial bias around the saccade target (ST) based on the perisaccadic dynamics of extrastriate spatiotemporal sensitivity captured by a statistical model. This approach could predict the perisaccadic spatial bias around the ST, consistent with behavioral data, and revealed the precise neuronal response components underlying representational bias. These findings also establish the crucial role of increased sensitivity near the ST for neurons with receptive fields far from the ST in driving the ST spatial bias. Moreover, we showed that, by allocating more resources for visual target representation, visual areas enhance their representation of the ST location, even at the expense of transient distortions in spatial representation. This potential neural basis for perisaccadic ST representation also supports a general role for extrastriate neurons in creating the perception of stimulus location.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究调查了在文化景观和旅游相关视觉刺激的背景下,眼动追踪指标与情感体验之间的关系。53名参与者参与了两个实验:43名在数据收集阶段,10名在模型验证阶段。记录眼球运动并分析数据,以确定四个眼球跟踪指标-平均扫视次数(ANS)之间的相关性。总停留固定(TDF),固定计数(FC),和平均瞳孔扩张(APD)-和19种不同的情感体验,随后分为三类:阳性,中性,和消极。该研究检查了整个建筑,历史,经济,和生活景观,以及旅游的三个主要阶段:进入,核心,和离开。研究结果表明,建筑和历史景观需要更高水平的视觉和认知参与,尤其是在核心阶段。逐步回归分析确定了情绪体验的四个关键眼动追踪预测因子,能够开发预测模型。这项研究强调了眼动追踪技术在捕捉和预测对不同景观类型的情绪反应方面的有效性,为优化乡村旅游环境和增强游客的情感体验提供有价值的见解。
    This study investigates the relationship between eye-tracking metrics and emotional experiences in the context of cultural landscapes and tourism-related visual stimuli. Fifty-three participants were involved in two experiments: forty-three in the data collection phase and ten in the model validation phase. Eye movements were recorded and the data were analyzed to identify correlations between four eye-tracking metrics-average number of saccades (ANS), total dwell fixation (TDF), fixation count (FC), and average pupil dilation (APD)-and 19 distinct emotional experiences, which were subsequently grouped into three categories: positive, neutral, and negative. The study examined the variations in eye-tracking metrics across architectural, historic, economic, and life landscapes, as well as the three primary phases of a tour: entry, core, and departure. Findings revealed that architectural and historic landscapes demanded higher levels of visual and cognitive engagement, especially during the core phase. Stepwise regression analysis identified four key eye-tracking predictors for emotional experiences, enabling the development of a prediction model. This research underscores the effectiveness of eye-tracking technology in capturing and predicting emotional responses to different landscape types, offering valuable insights for optimizing rural tourism environments and enhancing visitors\' emotional experiences.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    单一单位(SU)活动-从一个神经元分离的动作电位-传统上被用来将神经元活动与行为联系起来。然而,最近的研究表明,多单位(MU)活动-在一个微电极附近记录的整体神经活动-也可能包含与任务相关的神经群体动力学的准确估计。这里,使用既定的模型拟合方法,在记忆引导的扫视任务中,我们将SU响应场的空间代码与从头部不受约束的猴子(Macacamulatta)的前眼场(FEF)记录的相应MU响应场进行了比较。总的来说,SU和MU群体都显示出简单的视觉运动转换:视觉响应编码的目标在眼睛坐标,在延迟期间逐渐向扫视运动响应中的未来凝视代码过渡。然而,SU人口显示出额外的二级代码,包括视觉响应中的预测性注视代码和运动响应中的目标代码的保留。Further,当SU被分成规则/快速尖峰神经元时,这些细胞类型在延迟后期显示出不同的空间代码进展,仅在最后的扫视运动响应期间收敛于注视编码。最后,重建MU群体(通过对相同位点内的SU数据求和)未能复制SU或MU模式。这些结果证实了MU活性记录作为基本感觉运动转化的生物标志物的理论和实践潜力(例如,动眼系统中的目标到凝视编码),同时也强调了SU活动对编码更微妙的重要性(例如,预测/记忆)感觉运动行为的方面。显著性陈述多单位记录(来自多个神经元的未分化信号)相对容易记录,并提供神经动力学的简化估计,但是不清楚保留了哪些单单元信号,放大,或丢失。这里,我们比较了单/多单位活动从一个明确的结构(额眼领域)和行为(记忆延迟扫视任务),通过时间跟踪他们的空间代码。在单单位活动中观察到的从目标到凝视编码的渐进转换保留在多单位活动中,但其他认知信号(初始视觉反应内的凝视预测,最终运动响应内的目标记忆,和细胞特异性延迟信号)丢失。这表明多单位活动为健康的感觉运动转化提供了极好的生物标志物,以错过更微妙的认知信号为代价。
    Single-unit (SU) activity-action potentials isolated from one neuron-has traditionally been employed to relate neuronal activity to behavior. However, recent investigations have shown that multiunit (MU) activity-ensemble neural activity recorded within the vicinity of one microelectrode-may also contain accurate estimations of task-related neural population dynamics. Here, using an established model-fitting approach, we compared the spatial codes of SU response fields with corresponding MU response fields recorded from the frontal eye fields (FEFs) in head-unrestrained monkeys (Macaca mulatta) during a memory-guided saccade task. Overall, both SU and MU populations showed a simple visuomotor transformation: the visual response coded target-in-eye coordinates, transitioning progressively during the delay toward a future gaze-in-eye code in the saccade motor response. However, the SU population showed additional secondary codes, including a predictive gaze code in the visual response and retention of a target code in the motor response. Further, when SUs were separated into regular/fast spiking neurons, these cell types showed different spatial code progressions during the late delay period, only converging toward gaze coding during the final saccade motor response. Finally, reconstructing MU populations (by summing SU data within the same sites) failed to replicate either the SU or MU pattern. These results confirm the theoretical and practical potential of MU activity recordings as a biomarker for fundamental sensorimotor transformations (e.g., target-to-gaze coding in the oculomotor system), while also highlighting the importance of SU activity for coding more subtle (e.g., predictive/memory) aspects of sensorimotor behavior.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号