Psychophysiology

心理生理学
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究检查了精神病患者较高的人是否比精神病患者较低的人经历了较少的自我报告和心理生理伤害感受压力。我们还通过自我报告和心理生理措施检查了精神病是否会影响对他人疼痛的同理心。使用青年精神病量表(YPI)筛选了三百六十九名学生(18-78岁;M=26,SD=9.34)的精神病特征。分层抽样用于招募49名成年人,这些成年人居住在精神病谱系中最高(n=23)和最低(n=26)的20%。使用皮肤电导反应(SCR)和自我报告反应,参与者对单独调整的气压强度和其他疼痛图像做出了反应,并完成了自我报告的精神病和移情措施(三例精神病措施,TriPm;人际反应指数,IRI)。与精神病患者较低的人相比,精神病患者较高的人自我报告的伤害性压力较小,然而,我们没有发现SCR对伤害性压力的任何差异。然而,当看着其他人痛苦时,与精神病患者较低的患者相比,高精神病患者组的SCR和自我报告的共情能力较低.我们的结果表明,精神病特征与同情他人疼痛的问题有关,以及对伤害性压力的感知。我们还表示对双重伤害理论的支持,该理论已受到越来越多的关注。因此,精神病干预应侧重于认识和同情他人的痛苦。
    The present study examined whether people higher in psychopathy experienced less self-reported and psychophysiological nociceptive pressure than people lower in psychopathy. We also examined whether psychopathy affects empathy for others\' pain via self-reported and psychophysiological measures. Three hundred and sixty-nine students (18-78 years; M = 26, SD = 9.34) were screened for psychopathic traits using the Youth Psychopathy Inventory (YPI). Stratified sampling was used to recruit 49 adults residing in the highest (n = 23) and lowest (n = 26) 20% of the psychopathy spectrum. Using skin conductance response (SCR) and self-report responses, participants responded to individually adjusted intensities of pneumatic pressure and others\' pain images and completed self-reported psychopathy and empathy measures (Triarchic Psychopathy Measure, TriPm; Interpersonal Reactivity Index, IRI). People higher in psychopathy self-reported feeling less nociceptive pressure compared to people lower in psychopathy, yet we did not find any differences in SCR to nociceptive pressure. However, when viewing other people in pain, the high psychopathy group displayed lower SCR and lower self-reported empathy compared to those lower in psychopathy. Our results suggest psychopathic traits relate to problems empathising with others\' pain, as well as the perception of nociceptive pressure. We also show support for the theory of dual harm which has been receiving increasing attention. Consequently, psychopathy interventions should focus both on recognising and empathising with the pain of others.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    叙事是唤起情绪的有效工具,和生理测量提供了一种客观评估情绪反应的手段-使它们成为研究情绪过程的潜在强大工具。然而,结合情感叙事和生理测量的程度研究在设计和应用上差异很大,使识别以前的工作变得具有挑战性,巩固调查结果,并设计有效的实验。我们的范围审查探讨了听觉情绪叙事和生理措施在研究中的使用,检查范式,研究人群,并代表情感。按照PRISMA-ScR清单,我们在5个数据库中搜索了同行评审的实验研究,这些研究使用口头叙述来诱导情绪,并报告了自主生理指标.在筛选的3466本书和审查的653篇文章中,共纳入110项研究。我们的探索揭示了各种应用和实验范式;情感叙事与生理措施配对已用于研究不同的主题和人群,包括神经典型和临床组。尽管无与伦比的设计和有时相互矛盾的结果排除了关于设计新研究时使用哪些生理措施的一般性建议,作为一个整体,研究表明,这些工具对于研究情绪是有价值的。我们的评论概述了采用叙述和生理措施进行情绪研究的研究,并强调了报告实践中的弱点和我们关于生理指标作为情绪指标的鲁棒性和特异性的知识差距。我们讨论研究设计考虑因素和透明报告,促进未来在研究情绪时使用情绪叙事和生理措施。
    Narratives are effective tools for evoking emotions, and physiological measurements provide a means of objectively assessing emotional reactions - making them a potentially powerful pair of tools for studying emotional processes. However, extent research combining emotional narratives and physiological measurement varies widely in design and application, making it challenging to identify previous work, consolidate findings, and design effective experiments. Our scoping review explores the use of auditory emotional narratives and physiological measures in research, examining paradigms, study populations, and represented emotions. Following the PRISMA-ScR Checklist, we searched five databases for peer-reviewed experimental studies that used spoken narratives to induce emotion and reported autonomic physiological measures. Among 3466 titles screened and 653 articles reviewed, 110 studies were included. Our exploration revealed a variety of applications and experimental paradigms; emotional narratives paired with physiological measures have been used to study diverse topics and populations, including neurotypical and clinical groups. Although incomparable designs and sometimes contradictory results precluded general recommendations as regards which physiological measures to use when designing new studies, as a whole, the body of work suggests that these tools can be valuable to study emotions. Our review offers an overview of research employing narratives and physiological measures for emotion study, and highlights weaknesses in reporting practices and gaps in our knowledge concerning the robustness and specificity of physiological measures as indices of emotion. We discuss study design considerations and transparent reporting, to facilitate future using emotional narratives and physiological measures in studying emotions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在面对面的社交互动中,情感表达提供了对互动伴侣精神状态的见解。这些信息对于推断行动意图和对另一个人的行动做出反应至关重要。在这里,我们研究了在实时互动中面部情绪表情如何影响主观体验以及对社交行为的生理和行为反应。32名参与者在完全沉浸在虚拟现实中的同时与虚拟代理进行了互动。特工在对参与者进行食欲(拳头撞击)或厌恶(拳头)的社交行为之前,表现出愤怒或快乐的面部表情。参与者对这些行动做出了回应,要么通过往复拳头撞击,要么通过捍卫拳头。对于所有的互动,主观经验是使用评级来衡量的。此外,生理反应(皮肤电活动,记录心电图)和参与者的反应时间。相对于食欲行为,厌恶行为被认为更具唤醒性,而不那么令人愉悦。此外,相对于快乐的表情,愤怒的表情增加了心率。至关重要的是,观察面部情绪表达与动作的交互作用。与厌恶行为相比,愤怒的表情使食欲变得更加愉悦。此外,与愤怒的表情相比,快乐的皮肤对厌恶行为的传导性反应增加,而当代理人表现出愤怒的表情时,与食欲行为相比,反应时间更快。这些结果表明,观察者使用面部情绪表达来产生对特定动作的期望。因此,本研究表明,观察者将来自面部情感表达的信息与社交互动中的动作相结合。
    In face-to-face social interactions, emotional expressions provide insights into the mental state of an interactive partner. This information can be crucial to infer action intentions and react towards another person\'s actions. Here we investigate how facial emotional expressions impact subjective experience and physiological and behavioral responses to social actions during real-time interactions. Thirty-two participants interacted with virtual agents while fully immersed in Virtual Reality. Agents displayed an angry or happy facial expression before they directed an appetitive (fist bump) or aversive (punch) social action towards the participant. Participants responded to these actions, either by reciprocating the fist bump or by defending the punch. For all interactions, subjective experience was measured using ratings. In addition, physiological responses (electrodermal activity, electrocardiogram) and participants\' response times were recorded. Aversive actions were judged to be more arousing and less pleasant relative to appetitive actions. In addition, angry expressions increased heart rate relative to happy expressions. Crucially, interaction effects between facial emotional expression and action were observed. Angry expressions reduced pleasantness stronger for appetitive compared to aversive actions. Furthermore, skin conductance responses to aversive actions were increased for happy compared to angry expressions and reaction times were faster to aversive compared to appetitive actions when agents showed an angry expression. These results indicate that observers used facial emotional expression to generate expectations for particular actions. Consequently, the present study demonstrates that observers integrate information from facial emotional expressions with actions during social interactions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们对运动员心理物理状态的微观变化感兴趣,这些变化将峰值运动与生理崩溃分开。因此,我们用经典的心理物理学方法测量跑步者的知觉敏锐度,在两个标准刺激上的明显差异(JND)在跑步机速度下运行,对应于70%VO2max和80%VO2max。34名男性跑步者(M年龄=35.26,SD=7.33岁)首先进行了最大跑步机测试,以确定JND试验的标准运动回合的速度。JND试验包括在跑步机上进行四次5分钟的跑步比赛,两次比赛之间休息5分钟。对于第1次和第3次比赛,参与者以标准刺激速度跑,但是对于第2和第4回合,他们调整了速度,以在SS上方/下方的JND处达到一定的用力水平。他们在每个JND回合与之前的标准刺激之间的VO2的最后30秒内实现了差异,即在JND感知到的用力上方(JND-A)和下方(JND-B)。我们使用广义线性模型分析来比较通气阈值组(较低/较高)内和之间的绝对和相对VO2以及总JND幅度的JND-A和JND-B。在70%VO2max和80%VO2max时,JND-A的幅度大于JND-B的绝对单位(70%VO2Δ=2.62;SE=0.37;p<.001;80%VO2Δ=1.67;SE=0.44;p=.002)和相对单位(70%VO2maxΔΔ=4.70;SE=0.66;p<.0096;80%VO2.在70%VO2max试验中,以绝对单位(70%VO2M=3.78,SE=0.31mL·kg-1·min-1;80%VO2M=2.62,SE=0.37mL·kg-1·min-1;p=.020)和相对单位(70%VO2maxM=6.57,VOSE=0.53%2max4.64%)。当生理需求增加时,JND范围缩小,对于物理(速度)和心理(RPE)变量。
    We were interested in micro-variations in an athlete\'s psychophysical state that separate peak exertion from physiological collapse. Thus, we measured perceptual acuity in runners using a classic psychophysical approach, the just noticeable difference (JND) on two standard stimuli runs at treadmill speed corresponding to 70%VO2max and 80%VO2max. Thirty-four male runners (M age = 35.26, SD = 7.33 years) first performed a maximal treadmill test to determine the speed of a standard exercise bout for the JND trials. The JND trials consisted of four 5-minute running bouts on a treadmill with 5-minute rests between bouts. For bouts 1 and 3, participants ran at the standard stimuli pace, but for bouts 2 and 4, they adjusted their speeds to achieve a level of exertion at a JND above/below the SS. They achieved differences in the final 30 seconds of the VO2 between each JND bout and the previous standard stimuli at just above (JND-A) and just below (JND-B) the JND perceived exertions. We used a Generalized Linear Model analysis to compare the JND-A and JND-B within and between ventilatory threshold groups (lower/higher) in absolute and relative VO2 and in terms of the total JND magnitude. The magnitude of JND-A was greater than that of JND-B at 70%VO2max and 80%VO2max in absolute units (70%VO2 Δ = 2.62; SE = 0.37; p < .001; 80%VO2 Δ = 1.67; SE = 0.44; p = .002) and in relative units (70%VO2max Δ = 4.70; SE = 0.66; p < .001; 80%VO2max Δ = 2.96; SE = 0.80; p = .002). The total magnitude was greater in the 70%VO2max trial than 80%VO2max in absolute units (70%VO2 M = 3.78, SE = 0.31 mL·kg-1·min-1; 80%VO2 M = 2.62, SE = 0.37 mL·kg-1·min-1; p = .020) and in relative units (70%VO2max M = 6.57, SE = 0.53%VO2max; 80%VO2max M = 4.71, SE = 0.64%VO2max; p = .030). The JND range narrowed when physiologic demand increased, for both physical (speed) and psychological (RPE) variables.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    据我们所知,牙科学校的学生从未接受过压力评估,焦虑,倦怠,24小时工作日的生理指标,和执行职能在工作后和休假回来后的工作;因此,这项研究已经进行了。
    数据是在帕多瓦大学牙科学院获得的,有16名学生在他们4年级,远离考试。在牙科椅上和工作日进行临床活动时,皮肤电活动(EDA),心率变异性(HRV),记录心率(HR)。使用感知压力量表(PSS-10量表)测量参与者的压力,并使用一般焦虑障碍问卷(GAD-7)和状态特质焦虑量表(STAI-Y-2)测量焦虑,而与Maslach倦怠清单(MBI-HSS)一起倦怠。使用伦敦塔测试(TOL-R)评估执行功能。
    三名学生(2F/1M)的GAD-7得分≥10。5名学生(4F/1M)表现出特质焦虑。85%的参与者报告了中等程度的感知压力。MBI-HSS显示,7名参与者在情绪疲惫方面得分较高,7名参与者在人格解体方面得分较高。TOL-R性能(M=15.85,SD=4.01)低于标准值p<0.00001。第二次测试,假期过后,显示正常值。儿童治疗期间EDA较高(p<0.05),ANOVA显示工作时间内的高HR(p<0.001),男性HRV较高(p<0.001)。
    根据评估的样本量,据报道,作为牙科学生对压力有中等影响,焦虑,和倦怠,同时对执行功能的强烈影响被休息缓冲。
    UNASSIGNED: To the best of our knowledge, dental school students have never been evaluated for stress, anxiety, burnout, physiological indexes during a 24-h working day, and executive function performance post-work and post-work after returning from vacation; therefore, this research has been conducted.
    UNASSIGNED: Data were acquired at the Dental School of the University of Padua on 16 students in their 4th year, far from the exam session. While performing clinical activity on the dental chair and during a working day, electrodermal activity (EDA), heart rate variability (HRV), and heart rate (HR) were recorded. Participants\' stress was measured with the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10 scale) and anxiety with the General Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire (GAD-7) and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-Y-2), while burnout with the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI-HSS). Executive functions were evaluated using the Tower of London test (TOL-R).
    UNASSIGNED: Three students (2F/1M) had a GAD-7 score ≥ 10. Five students (4F/1M) showed trait anxiety. Moderate levels of perceived stress were reported in 85% of participants. MBI-HSS showed that 7 participants scored high on emotional exhaustion and 7 on depersonalization. TOL-R performance (M = 15.85, SD = 4.01) was below the normative value p < 0.00001. A second test, after the holidays, showed normal values. EDA was higher during children\'s treatment (p < 0.05), ANOVA showed high HR during working time (p < 0.001), and HRV was higher in males (p < 0.001).
    UNASSIGNED: Based on the sample size evaluated, it is reported that being a dental student has a moderate impact on stress, anxiety, and burnout while a strong impact on executive functions buffered by rest.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    疼痛是一种自然发生的现象,它通过强加无意识来不断抑制运动表现,神经生理学改变(例如,皮质脊髓变化)以及有意识的,心理生理压力(例如,共同的努力要求)。虽然,几项研究表明,疼痛会导致较低的任务产出,以设定的感知努力强度,没有研究对此进行过测试。因此,这项研究调查了通过注射高渗盐水引起的肌肉疼痛对功率输出的影响,心理生理学,脑氧合,固定感知努力运动过程中的感知变化。十名参与者完成了三次访问(一次熟悉+两次固定感知努力试验)。固定的感知努力循环对应于高于气体交换阈值15%(平均RPE=15;硬)。在30分钟的固定感知努力练习之前,参与者接受了随机的,股外侧肌双侧注射高渗或等渗盐水。功率输出,心肺,脑氧合,和感知标记(例如,情感效价)在运动过程中记录。线性混合模型回归评估了条件和时间效应以及条件×时间相互作用。显著的条件效应表明高渗条件下功率输出显著降低(t_107=2.08,p=0.040,β=4.77瓦,95CI[0.27至9.26瓦])。同时所有生理变量(例如,心率,氧气吸收,分钟通气)没有明显的条件影响。从基线观察到脱氧血红蛋白变化的条件效应(t_107=-3.29,p=.001,β=-1.50ΔµM,95CI[-2.40至-0.61ΔµM])和情感效价(t_127=6.12,p=.001,β=0.93,95CI[0.63,1.23])。结果推断疼痛会影响固定感知努力运动的自我调节,因为功率输出的差异主要发生在高渗与等渗盐水给药后疼痛评分较高时。
    Pain is a naturally occurring phenomenon that consistently inhibits exercise performance by imposing unconscious, neurophysiological alterations (e.g., corticospinal changes) as well as conscious, psychophysiological pressures (e.g., shared effort demands). Although, several studies indicate that pain would elicit lower task outputs for a set intensity of perceived effort, no study has tested this. Therefore, this study investigated the impact of elevated muscle pain through a hypertonic saline injection on the power output, psychophysiological, cerebral oxygenation, and perceptual changes during fixed perceived effort exercise. Ten participants completed three visits (one familiarisation + two fixed perceived effort trials). Fixed perceived effort cycling corresponded to 15% above gas exchange threshold (mean RPE = 15; hard). Before the 30-minute fixed perceived effort exercise, participants received a randomised, bilateral hypertonic or isotonic saline injection in the vastus lateralis. Power output, cardiorespiratory, cerebral oxygenation, and perceptual markers (e.g., affective valence) were recorded during exercise. Linear mixed model regression assessed the condition and time effects and condition × time interactions. Significant condition effects showed that power output was significantly lower during hypertonic conditions (t_107= 2.08,p=.040,β=4.77 Watts,95%CI [0.27 to 9.26 Watts]). Meanwhile all physiological variables (e.g., heart rate, oxygen uptake, minute ventilation) demonstrated no significant condition effects. Condition effects were observed for deoxyhaemoglobin changes from baseline (t_107= -3.29,p=.001,β=-1.50 ΔµM,95%CI [-2.40 to-0.61 ΔµM]) and affective valence (t_127= 6.12,p=.001,β=0.93,95%CI [0.63,1.23]). Results infer that pain impacts the self-regulation of fixed perceived effort exercise, as differences in power output mainly occurred when pain ratings were higher after hypertonic versus isotonic saline administration.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:本研究旨在探讨不同情绪调节策略的有效性,特别是重新评估和压制,与适应性和适应不良人格特征有关。
    背景:人格会影响情绪并影响情绪调节。在可用的监管策略中,重新评估(重新解释情况)被描述为一种有效的策略,而抑制(不表现出经验的情绪)会带来更高的生理和认知成本。鲜为人知,然而,关于人格对这些效率的影响。
    方法:我们测试了102名参与者的人格结构(均值=20.75,SDage=2.15),基于五因素模型和适应不良人格特质模型。经验,表现力,在参与者重新评估时,在观看带有情感的正面和负面图像期间记录了生理唤醒,压制,或者在不调节情绪的情况下观看图像。
    结果:我们确定了两个自适应人格集群(“自适应弹性”和“抗弹性”)和两个适应不良人格集群(“自适应弹性”和“控制不足”)。主要发现是适应不良人格的情感体验,在适应不良的弹性特征中,重新评估是有效的,而没有一种策略能缓解受控制的情况。
    结论:这项研究,系统地对比了情绪调节策略的个性和效率,是对人格如何影响情绪调节过程的理解的首次尝试之一。
    OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to explore the efficiency of different emotion regulation strategies, specifically reappraisal and suppression, in relation to adaptive and maladaptive personality profiles.
    BACKGROUND: Personality conditions emotions and influences emotion regulation. Of the available regulation strategies, reappraisal (reinterpreting the situation) is described as an efficient strategy, whereas suppression (not displaying the experienced emotion) carries higher physiological and cognitive costs. Little is known, however, about the influence of personality on these efficiencies.
    METHODS: We tested the personality structure of 102 participants (Meanage = 20.75, SDage = 2.15), based on the Five-Factor Model and the Maladaptive Personality Trait Model. Experience, expressivity, and physiological arousal were recorded during the viewing of emotionally charged positive and negative images while participants reappraised, suppressed, or viewed the images without regulating their emotions.
    RESULTS: We identified two clusters for adaptive personality (\"Adaptive Resilient\" and \"Anti-resilient\") and two for maladaptive personality (\"Maladaptive Resilient\" and \"Under-controlled\"). The major finding was for emotional experience in maladaptive personalities, where reappraisal was efficient in the Maladaptive Resilient profile, while none of the strategies brought relief in the Under-controlled profile.
    CONCLUSIONS: This study, which systematically contrasts personality and efficiency of emotion regulation strategies, is one of the first attempts to refine the understanding of how personality influences the emotional regulation process.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    为了识别一个敏感的人,健壮,和易于测量的一组生物标志物来评估应激反应,我们在这里研究了大量相对容易获得反映主观的标记,自主神经系统(ANS),内分泌,和对急性社会压力的炎症反应(n=101)。参与者的子集在第二天(n=48)暴露于另一种社交压力源,同时以相同的方式进行测量。按照标准化程序引起急性社会压力。调查的标志物是自我报告的阳性和阴性影响,心率,皮肤电活动,唾液皮质醇,毛细血管血浆和唾液样本中的十种炎症标志物,包括IL-22,以前没有研究过对人类急性应激的反应。对于自我报告的负面影响,发现了稳健的影响(两天在同一方向上的显着影响),心率,皮肤电活动,血浆IL-5,血浆IL-22,唾液IL-8和唾液IL-10。在这七个标记中,参与者第一天的IL-22应答与第二天的应答呈正相关.我们没有发现唾液和毛细血管血浆应激反应之间的任何十种细胞因子和有些出乎意料的相关性,唾液中的细胞因子反应似乎比毛细血管血浆中的细胞因子反应更明显,更符合以前的文献。总之,确定了7种稳健且易于获得的生物标志物来测量急性应激反应,这些生物标志物应在未来的应激研究中用于检测和检查应激反应性.这包括血浆中的IL-22作为有前途的新型标志物。
    With the purpose of identifying a sensitive, robust, and easy-to-measure set of biomarkers to assess stress reactivity, we here study a large set of relatively easy to obtain markers reflecting subjective, autonomic nervous system (ANS), endocrine, and inflammatory responses to acute social stress (n = 101). A subset of the participants was exposed to another social stressor the next day (n = 48) while being measured in the same way. Acute social stress was induced following standardized procedures. The markers investigated were self-reported positive and negative affect, heart rate, electrodermal activity, salivary cortisol, and ten inflammatory markers both in capillary plasma and salivary samples, including IL-22 which has not been studied in response to acute stress in humans before. Robust effects (significant effect in the same direction for both days) were found for self-reported negative affect, heart rate, electrodermal activity, plasma IL-5, plasma IL-22, salivary IL-8 and salivary IL-10. Of these seven markers, the participants\' IL-22 responses on the first day were positively correlated to those on the second day. We found no correlations between salivary and capillary plasma stress responses for any of the ten cytokines and somewhat unexpectedly, cytokine responses in saliva seemed more pronounced and more in line with previous literature than cytokines in capillary plasma. In sum, seven robust and easy to obtain biomarkers to measure acute stress response were identified and should be used in future stress research to detect and examine stress reactivity. This includes IL-22 in plasma as a promising novel marker.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    合金化是一种普遍的,非人类灵长类动物中普遍存在的活动。除了它的卫生功能,它被认为有助于维持社会纽带和建立群体内部的等级结构。然而,问题是,从社会接触中获得的生理和社会效益是否直接来自身体刺激,或者其他机制是否起作用。我们通过分析在两只成年雄性猕猴中观察到的复杂的社会行为来解决这个问题。这种行为证明了通过模拟修饰的形式进行交互的持续动机的存在,因为动物被安置在由玻璃面板隔开的相邻围栏中,以防止直接的触觉接触。我们发现,这种虚拟修饰会产生与allogrooming相似的生理感觉和社会效应。我们建议这种行为与真实的社会接触相同程度地参与情感和奖励大脑回路,这可能是通过类似于先前在人类中描述的身体错觉或联觉现象的高级过程来实现的。这一观察揭示了非人灵长类动物发明替代品的意外能力,准符号策略,以获得类似于直接身体互动提供的效果,这对维持社会纽带非常重要。
    Allogrooming is a widespread, pervasive activity among non-human primates. Besides its hygienic function, it is thought to be instrumental in maintaining social bonds and establishing hierarchical structures within groups. However, the question arises as to whether the physiological and social benefits derived from social touch stem directly from body stimulation, or whether other mechanisms come into play. We address this question by analyzing an elaborate social behavior that we observed in two adult male macaques. This behavior demonstrates the existence of a persistent motivation to interact through a form of simulated grooming, as the animals were housed in adjacent enclosures separated by a glass panel preventing direct tactile contact. We find that such virtual grooming produces similar physiological sensations and social effects as allogrooming. We suggest that this behavior engages affective and reward brain circuits to the same extent as real social touch, and that this is probably achieved through high level processes similar to those involved in bodily illusions or synaesthetic phenomena previously described in humans. This observation reveals the unsuspected capacity of non-human primates to invent alternative, quasi-symbolic strategies to obtain effects similar to those provided by direct bodily interaction, which are so important for maintaining social bonds.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:22q11.2缺失综合征(22q11DS)是精神病和其他精神疾病最可靠的遗传预测因子之一。在这项研究中,我们检查了22q11DS受试者的声音惊吓反应(ASR),推定精神病风险指数。ASR的潜伏期是神经处理速度的假定标志,在精神分裂症中延长(较慢)。ASR指标与精神病风险增加相关,依赖于谷氨酸和多巴胺受体信号,并可作为高精神病风险人群干预措施中的转化生物标志物。
    方法:惊吓幅度,延迟,在31名22q11.2DS的个体和32名健康比较(HC)受试者中,采用标准的声学惊吓范式评估了前脉冲抑制。放置在参与者眼轮匝肌上的表面电极记录了ASR眨眼中的肌电图信号。在初始习惯块中没有可测量的惊吓眨眼的个体被归类为非惊吓者。
    结果:在整个惊吓阶段,22q11DS受试者的ASR大小显著低于HCs,因为22q11DS受试者中,非初学者的比例显著较高.在22q11DS中,ASR对脉冲单独刺激的潜伏期明显慢于HC受试者。由于22q11DS惊吓响应频率和幅度总体较低,无法分析预脉冲抑制。
    结论:22q11DS受试者反应的幅度降低和潜伏期缓慢表明中枢神经系统和神经元反应性降低。这些发现与在22q11DS受试者中观察到的显著认知损伤一致。需要进一步的研究来解开基本神经传递功能障碍之间的联系,心理生理反应,和认知障碍。
    BACKGROUND: 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS) is one of the most robust genetic predictors of psychosis and other psychiatric illnesses. In this study, we examined 22q11DS subjects\' acoustic startle responses (ASRs), which putatively index psychosis risk. Latency of the ASR is a presumptive marker of neural processing speed and is prolonged (slower) in schizophrenia. ASR measures correlate with increased psychosis risk, depend on glutamate and dopamine receptor signaling, and could serve as translational biomarkers in interventions for groups at high psychosis risk.
    METHODS: Startle magnitude, latency, and prepulse inhibition were assessed with a standard acoustic startle paradigm in 31 individuals with 22q11.2DS and 32 healthy comparison (HC) subjects. Surface electrodes placed on participants\' orbicularis oculi recorded the electromyographic signal in ASR eyeblinks. Individuals without measurable startle blinks in the initial habituation block were classified as non-startlers.
    RESULTS: Across the startle session, the ASR magnitude was significantly lower in 22q11DS subjects than HCs because a significantly higher proportion of 22q11DS subjects were non-startlers. Latency of the ASR to pulse-alone stimuli was significantly slower in 22q11DS than HC subjects. Due to the overall lower 22q11DS startle response frequency and magnitudes prepulse inhibition could not be analyzed.
    CONCLUSIONS: Reduced magnitude and slow latency of 22q11DS subjects\' responses suggest reduced central nervous system and neuronal responsiveness. These findings are consistent with significant cognitive impairments observed in 22q11DS subjects. Further research is needed to untangle the connections among basic neurotransmission dysfunction, psychophysiological responsiveness, and cognitive impairment.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号