Fixation, Ocular

固定,眼
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:眼动追踪技术不仅揭示了注视时视觉信息的获取,而且有可能揭示学习使用多功能假手所涉及的潜在认知过程。它还揭示了在标准化任务和自我选择任务期间观察到的凝视行为。该研究的目的是探索在假肢康复中两个不同时间点使用眼动跟踪来跟踪多功能手的学习进度。
    方法:三名截肢者接受了具有新控制策略的多功能手的控制训练。首先收集控制训练的详细描述。他们戴着TobiiPro2眼动眼镜,在一天的训练和一年的随访中(由于插座问题,随访中缺少受试者3的数据)后,执行了一组标准化任务(要求每个任务切换到不同的手柄)。他们还执行了自己选择的任务(可以自由地对任何物体使用任何抓地力),并被指示以他们通常在家中的方式执行任务。使用TobiiProLab分析了凝视覆盖的视频,并提取了以下指标:固定持续时间,扫视幅度,眼手延迟,固定计数和第一次固定的时间。
    结果:在控制训练期间,受试者学会了3到4个握把。有些抓握更容易,而其他人则更加困难,因为他们忘记或混淆了转换策略。在为期一年的随访中,性能时间的减少,固定持续时间,眼手延迟,在受试者1和2中观察到固定计数,表明控制多功能手的能力得到改善,认知负荷减少。在两个受试者中都观察到扫视幅度增加,这表明控制假手的难度降低了。在标准化任务期间,所有三个受试者的第一次固定都在所有对象的多功能手上。在自己选择的任务中,最初的注视主要是首先在物体上。
    结论:来自对照训练的定性数据和来自临床标准化任务的定量眼动追踪数据为学习控制多功能手的认知加工提供了丰富的探索。许多假肢使用者更喜欢多功能手,通过这项研究,我们证明了具有可靠评估方法的有针对性的假肢训练方案将有助于为测量多功能手的功能益处奠定基础。
    BACKGROUND: Eye tracking technology not only reveals the acquisition of visual information at fixation but also has the potential to unveil underlying cognitive processes involved in learning to use a multifunction prosthetic hand. It also reveals gaze behaviours observed during standardized tasks and self-chosen tasks. The aim of the study was to explore the use of eye tracking to track learning progress of multifunction hands at two different time points in prosthetic rehabilitation.
    METHODS: Three amputees received control training of a multifunction hand with new control strategy. Detailed description of control training was collected first. They wore Tobii Pro2 eye-tracking glasses and performed a set of standardized tasks (required to switch to different grips for each task) after one day of training and at one-year-follow-up (missing data for Subject 3 at the follow up due to socket problem). They also performed a self-chosen task (free to use any grip for any object) and were instructed to perform the task in a way how they would normally do at home. The gaze-overlaid videos were analysed using the Tobii Pro Lab and the following metrics were extracted: fixation duration, saccade amplitude, eye-hand latency, fixation count and time to first fixation.
    RESULTS: During control training, the subjects learned 3 to 4 grips. Some grips were easier, and others were more difficult because they forgot or were confused with the switching strategies. At the one-year-follow-up, a decrease in performance time, fixation duration, eye-hand latency, and fixation count was observed in Subject 1 and 2, indicating an improvement in the ability to control the multifunction hand and a reduction of cognitive load. An increase in saccade amplitude was observed in both subjects, suggesting a decrease in difficulty to control the prosthetic hand. During the standardized tasks, the first fixation of all three subjects were on the multifunction hand in all objects. During the self-chosen tasks, the first fixations were mostly on the objects first.
    CONCLUSIONS: The qualitative data from control training and the quantitative eye tracking data from clinical standardized tasks provided a rich exploration of cognitive processing in learning to control a multifunction hand. Many prosthesis users prefer multifunction hands and with this study we have demonstrated that a targeted prosthetic training protocol with reliable assessment methods will help to lay the foundation for measuring functional benefits of multifunction hands.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Dataset
    3D-ARM-Gaze是一个公共数据集,旨在提供自然的手臂运动以及视觉和凝视信息,当物体从精确控制的范围内到达时,舒适的坐姿。参与者参与在虚拟环境中以各种位置和方向拾取和放置物体,通过特定的程序,通过从躯干和肩膀的视觉反馈将参与者引导到预定的中立姿势,从而使探索的工作空间最大化,同时确保一致的坐姿。这些实验设置能够以高的中值成功率(达到>98%的对象)和最小的代偿运动捕获自然的手臂运动。该数据集重新分组了来自20名健康参与者的超过250万个样本,这些参与者进行了14,000次单次拾取和放置运动(每个参与者700个)。虽然最初旨在探索基于自然眼手和手臂协调的新型假体控制策略,这个数据集也将有助于对核心感觉运动控制感兴趣的研究人员,人形机器人,人机交互,以及凝视引导计算机视觉相关解决方案的开发和测试。
    3D-ARM-Gaze is a public dataset designed to provide natural arm movements together with visual and gaze information when reaching objects in a wide reachable space from a precisely controlled, comfortably seated posture. Participants were involved in picking and placing objects in various positions and orientations in a virtual environment, whereby a specific procedure maximized the workspace explored while ensuring a consistent seated posture by guiding participants to a predetermined neutral posture via visual feedback from the trunk and shoulders. These experimental settings enabled to capture natural arm movements with high median success rates (>98% objects reached) and minimal compensatory movements. The dataset regroups more than 2.5 million samples recorded from 20 healthy participants performing 14 000 single pick-and-place movements (700 per participant). While initially designed to explore novel prosthesis control strategies based on natural eye-hand and arm coordination, this dataset will also be useful to researchers interested in core sensorimotor control, humanoid robotics, human-robot interactions, as well as for the development and testing of associated solutions in gaze-guided computer vision.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究考察了全球人口老龄化对老年人健康和情绪健康的影响,强调创新的技术解决方案,以满足他们的多样化需求。由于衰老,身体和心理功能的变化,伴随着需要关注的情感挑战,被突出显示。凝视估计和互动艺术被用来开发一个为老年用户量身定制的互动系统,通过眼球运动简化交互,以减少技术障碍并提供舒缓的艺术体验。通过采用多感官刺激,该系统旨在唤起积极的情绪,促进有意义的活动,促进主动衰老。命名为“通过眼睛的自然节奏”,它允许用户通过眼球运动与以自然为主题的环境进行交互。通过问卷调查和专家访谈的用户反馈是在老年人环境的公共演示中收集的,以验证系统在提供可用性方面的有效性。快乐,和老年人的互动体验。主要发现包括:(1)增强了老年用户的注视估计界面的可用性。(2)通过以自然为主题的互动艺术增加乐趣和参与度。(3)通过凝视估计和交互艺术的整合对主动衰老的积极影响。这些发现强调了技术在改善老年人应对老龄化挑战的福祉和生活质量方面的潜力。
    The impact of global population aging on older adults\' health and emotional well-being is examined in this study, emphasizing innovative technological solutions to address their diverse needs. Changes in physical and mental functions due to aging, along with emotional challenges that necessitate attention, are highlighted. Gaze estimation and interactive art are utilized to develop an interactive system tailored for elderly users, where interaction is simplified through eye movements to reduce technological barriers and provide a soothing art experience. By employing multi-sensory stimulation, the system aims to evoke positive emotions and facilitate meaningful activities, promoting active aging. Named \"Natural Rhythm through Eyes\", it allows for users to interact with nature-themed environments via eye movements. User feedback via questionnaires and expert interviews was collected during public demonstrations in elderly settings to validate the system\'s effectiveness in providing usability, pleasure, and interactive experience for the elderly. Key findings include the following: (1) Enhanced usability of the gaze estimation interface for elderly users. (2) Increased enjoyment and engagement through nature-themed interactive art. (3) Positive influence on active aging through the integration of gaze estimation and interactive art. These findings underscore technology\'s potential to enhance well-being and quality of life for older adults navigating aging challenges.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    适应闪烁/动态噪声可提高短暂呈现刺激的视敏度(Arnold等人。,2016)。这里,我们调查了这种适应是否直接影响我们看到细节的能力,或者通过改变固定眼球运动和瞳孔大小,或者通过减少视觉拥挤。在早期工作之后,视力是在未适应或适应60Hz闪烁噪声模式的观察者中测量的。参与者报告了白色翻滚T目标的方向(四个替代强制选择[4AFC],)。目标孤立地呈现110毫秒,或者侧翼为随机定向的T(例如,)后跟一个孤立的(+)或侧翼的(+++)面具,分别。当观察者执行任务(有和没有适应)时,我们测量了固定稳定性(使用红外眼睛跟踪器)。适应闪烁后,侧翼视型的视力略有改善(约8.4%)(平均,-0.038±0.063logMAR;p=0.015;BF10=3.66),但用孤立字母测量时没有(平均值,-0.008±0.055logMAR;p=0.5;BF10=0.29)。视力改善的幅度与个体(不适应)对拥挤的敏感性(拥挤与不拥挤的视力之比;r=-0.58,p=0.008,BF10=7.70)有关,但与固定稳定性和瞳孔大小无关。确认以前的报告,闪烁改善了短暂呈现的刺激的敏锐度,但我们表明,这只是拥挤的信件的情况。这些改善可能是由于对瞬态低空间频率(SF)图像结构的灵敏度衰减所致(Arnold等人。,2016;Tagoh等人。,2022),可能,例如,减少低SF对高SF的掩蔽。我们还建议这种衰减可以减少后向掩蔽,从而减少中央凹拥挤。
    Adaptation to flickering/dynamic noise improves visual acuity for briefly presented stimuli (Arnold et al., 2016). Here, we investigate whether such adaptation operates directly on our ability to see detail or by changing fixational eye movements and pupil size or by reducing visual crowding. Following earlier work, visual acuity was measured in observers who were either unadapted or who had adapted to a 60-Hz flickering noise pattern. Participants reported the orientation of a white tumbling-T target (four-alternative forced choice [4AFC], ⊤⊣⊥⊢). The target was presented for 110 ms either in isolation or flanked by randomly oriented T\'s (e.g., ⊣⊤⊢) followed by an isolated (+) or flanked (+++) mask, respectively. We measured fixation stability (using an infrared eye tracker) while observers performed the task (with and without adaptation). Visual acuity improved modestly (around 8.4%) for flanked optotypes following adaptation to flicker (mean, -0.038 ± 0.063 logMAR; p = 0.015; BF10 = 3.66) but did not when measured with isolated letters (mean, -0.008 ± 0.055 logMAR; p = 0.5; BF10 = 0.29). The magnitude of acuity improvement was associated with individuals\' (unadapted) susceptibility to crowding (the ratio of crowded to uncrowded acuity; r = -0.58, p = 0.008, BF10 = 7.70) but to neither fixation stability nor pupil size. Confirming previous reports, flicker improved acuity for briefly presented stimuli, but we show that this was only the case for crowded letters. These improvements likely arise from attenuation of sensitivity to a transient low spatial frequency (SF) image structure (Arnold et al., 2016; Tagoh et al., 2022), which may, for example, reduce masking of high SFs by low SFs. We also suggest that this attenuation could reduce backward masking and so reduce foveal crowding.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    检测轻度认知障碍(MCI)的测试应具有较高的敏感性和准确性。以前,我们报道了我们开发的用于检测MCI的认知成分测试(CCT).在这项研究中,我们比较了凝视行为参数,即,凝视时间和凝视移动频率,在MCI参与者和健康对照(HC)的CCT期间,确定这些参数是否能准确检测老年人的MCI.参与者进行了CCT-A,-B,和-C任务具有不同的难度级别,同时佩戴眼动跟踪设备。性能时间,凝视时间,并对凝视频移进行了分析。进行接收器工作特征曲线分析以评估识别准确性。与HC组相比,MCI组完成CCT-C任务的速度明显较慢,并且在样本对象空间和工作空间中的视线移位频率更高。随着CCT变得困难,MCI组中样本对象空间中的凝视固定持续时间增加。我们的发现表明,将CCT与表现时间和凝视模式相结合,可以提高区分有和没有MCI的个体的准确性,并且MCI患者在认知任务期间的凝视行为异常。因此,对注视参数的评估可以提高识别MCI患者的准确性.
    Tests to detect mild cognitive impairment (MCI) should have high sensitivity and accuracy. Previously, we reported on a cognitive composition test (CCT) that we developed to detect MCI. In this study, we compared gaze behavior parameters, namely, gaze fixation duration and gaze-shift frequency, during the CCT in participants with MCI and healthy controls (HC) to determine whether these parameters would accurately detect MCI in older adults. Participants performed CCT-A, -B, and -C tasks with varying difficulty levels while wearing eye-tracking devices. Performance time, gaze fixation duration, and gaze-shift frequency were analyzed. Receiver operating characteristic curve analysis was performed to assess the identification accuracy. The MCI group was significantly slower in completing the CCT-C task and had a higher gaze-shift frequency into both the sample object space and workspace than the HC group. Gaze fixation duration in the sample object space increased in the MCI group as the CCT became difficult. Our findings indicated that combining the CCT with performance time and gaze pattern improved the accuracy of distinguishing between individuals with and without MCI and that patients with MCI have abnormal gaze behavior during cognitive tasks. Therefore, evaluation of gaze parameters may improve the accuracy of identifying patients with MCI.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    眼动追踪技术是用于不同数学认知研究领域的潜在工具。此外,迫切需要更多的研究来提供有关注册眼科数据质量的详细信息。本研究旨在说明眼动追踪在数学认知检查中的适用性。特别关注完成计算机化心算任务的小学生。结果表明,当小学生从事这项特定任务时,眼动跟踪设备有效地捕获了高质量的眼动数据。此外,已发现任务表现与眼睛固定次数之间存在显着负相关。最后,已发现“感兴趣区域”之间的眼球运动差异,指示与算术计算的不同组件相关联的不同视觉跟踪。这项研究强调了未来研究在计算机化计算任务中使用眼动跟踪设备作为评估工具来探索复杂的视觉和认知过程的广泛可能性。
    Eye tracking technology is a high-potential tool for different mathematic cognition research areas. Moreover, there is a dire need for more studies that provide detailed information on the quality of registered eye data. This study aimed to illustrate the applicability of eye tracking in the examination of mathematical cognition, focusing specifically on primary school students completing a computerized mental arithmetic task. Results suggested that the eye tracking device effectively captured high-quality eye movement data when primary school children engaged in this specific task. Furthermore, significant negative correlations have been found between task performance and number of eye fixations. Finally, eye movements distinctions between \"Areas of Interest\" have been found, indicating different visual tracking associated with different components of arithmetic calculations. This study underscores the extensive possibilities for future research employing eye tracking devices during computerized calculation tasks as assessment tools to explore the complex visual and cognitive processes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:眼球震颤固定抑制的评估可用作急性前庭综合征患者的额外诊断工具,以区分中枢或外周原因。我们研究了医生使用眼球震颤模拟模型检测固定抑制的能力。
    方法:我们使用眼球震颤模拟器来测量眼球震颤固定抑制试验的准确性。由20名耳鼻喉科医师和神经科医师在6170项试验中随机评估了固定抑制,根据他们的神经病理学经验分为两组,一个初学者和一个有经验的团体。模拟器呈现随机眼球震颤慢速(SPV)降低,并呈现3种情况,其中眼球震颤频率改变,振幅,或者两者兼而有之。
    结果:识别固定抑制的截止范围为1.2至14°/s眼球震颤速度差。基线眼球震颤越强烈,更难的是固定抑制的检测。在所有3种不同条件下,与新手相比,专家组的截止值没有显着差异(P>.05)。两者,新手和专家,检测到的频率变化比眼球震颤幅度的差异更容易。对于由专家辨别<2°/s的小眼球震颤速度差异,测试灵敏度非常低(19%-65%)。
    结论:在我们的研究中,专家和新手在通过视觉固定检测眼球震颤抑制方面没有差异。检查者只能在低强度基线眼球震颤时检测到较大的抑制作用。总的来说,临床固定抑制试验的灵敏度和准确性较低,强烈建议使用视频眼镜检查装置辅助.
    BACKGROUND:  Assessment of nystagmus fixation suppression can be used as an additional diagnostic tool for patients with an acute vestibular syndrome to distinguish between a central or peripheral cause. We investigated the ability of physicians to detect fixation suppression using a nystagmus simulation model.
    METHODS:  We used a nystagmus simulator to measure the accuracy of the nystagmus fixation suppression test. Fixation suppression was assessed randomly in 6170 trials by 20 otorhinolaryngologists and neurologists, segregated into 2 groups based on their neurootological experience, a beginner and an experienced group. The simulator presented random nystagmus slow velocity (SPV) reductions and presented 3 conditions with either changed nystagmus frequency, amplitude, or both.
    RESULTS:  The cutoff for the discernment of fixation suppression ranged from 1.2 to 14°/s nystagmus velocity difference. The more intense the baseline nystagmus was, the more difficult was the detection of fixation suppression. There was not significant difference (P >.05) in the cutoff values in the experts group compared to the novices for all 3 different conditions. Both, novices and experts, detected frequency changes easier than differences of the nystagmus amplitude. Test sensitivity was very low (19%-65%) for discernment of small nystagmus velocity differences of <2°/s by experts.
    CONCLUSIONS:  In our study, there was no difference between experts and novices in detection of nystagmus suppression by visual fixation. The examiners could only detect large suppression effects at low-intensity baseline nystagmus. Overall, the sensitivity and accuracy of a clinical fixation suppression test is low and the assistance with a video-oculography device is highly recommended.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当一个人的瞳孔大小无意识地适应被观察个体的瞳孔大小并被认为反映了唤醒的转移时,就会发生瞳孔传染。重要的是,在估计瞳孔传染时,低水平的刺激特性需要控制,以确保对瞳孔变化的观察是由于唤醒的内部变化而不是刺激之间的外部差异。这里,自然的儿童的脸描绘无论是小或大的学生被呈现给一组儿童和青少年与广泛的自闭症特征,其中三分之一被诊断为自闭症。我们检查了瞳孔感染通过瞳孔大小变化反映自主神经系统反应的程度,心率和皮肤电导反应。我们的第二个目的是确定对刺激的唤醒反应与自闭症特征程度之间的关联。结果表明,瞳孔感染和伴随的心率变化,但不是皮肤电导变化,当凝视仅限于面部刺激的眼睛区域时,很明显。当参与者的视线被限制在眼睛区域时,瞳孔传染与自闭症特征之间也存在正相关。研究结果增加了对瞳孔传染及其与自闭症关联的潜在机制的更广泛理解。
    Pupillary contagion occurs when one\'s pupil size unconsciously adapts to the pupil size of an observed individual and is presumed to reflect the transfer of arousal. Importantly, when estimating pupil contagion, low level stimuli properties need to be controlled for, to ensure that observations of pupillary changes are due to internal change in arousal rather than the external differences between stimuli. Here, naturalistic images of children\'s faces depicting either small or large pupils were presented to a group of children and adolescents with a wide range of autistic traits, a third of whom had been diagnosed with autism. We examined the extent to which pupillary contagion reflects autonomic nervous system reaction through pupil size change, heart rate and skin conductance response. Our second aim was to determine the association between arousal reaction to stimuli and degree of autistic traits. Results show that pupil contagion and concomitant heart rate change, but not skin conductance change, was evident when gaze was restricted to the eye region of face stimuli. A positive association was also observed between pupillary contagion and autistic traits when participants\' gaze was constrained to the eye region. Findings add to a broader understanding of the mechanisms underlying pupillary contagion and its association with autism.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究的目的是使用参数有限元建模和实验设计(DOE)方法研究眼球和视神经(ON)形态和组织刚度对凝视引起的视神经头变形的影响。
    开发了一种自定义软件,用于使用10个形态学参数生成眼睛的有限元模型:硬脑膜半径,巩膜,脉络膜,视网膜,毛管和乳头周围边界组织厚度,前层组织深度,筛板(LC)深度,在半径上,和弯曲。使用中心复合面为中心的设计(1045模型)来预测每种形态因子及其相互作用对13度内收诱导的LC菌株的影响。随后,进行了进一步的DOE分析(1045个模型),以研究从初始DOE研究中确定的前5个形态参数与5个关键组织硬度之间的影响和潜在的相互作用.
    在10个形态参数的DOE分析中,5个最重要的因素是弯曲,硬脑膜半径,在半径上,巩膜厚度,LC深度结合生物力学参数的进一步DOE分析强调了硬脑膜和LC刚度的重要性。较大的硬脑膜半径和硬脑膜增加了LC应变,但其他主要因素具有相反的作用。值得注意的是,硬脑膜半径与硬脑膜刚度之间存在显著的相互作用,在半径上,和弯曲。
    这项研究强调了形态因素对眼球运动过程中LC变形的重大影响,关键的形态效应比组织僵硬更明显。
    UNASSIGNED: The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of globe and optic nerve (ON) morphologies and tissue stiffnesses on gaze-induced optic nerve head deformations using parametric finite element modeling and a design of experiment (DOE) approach.
    UNASSIGNED: A custom software was developed to generate finite element models of the eye using 10 morphological parameters: dural radius, scleral, choroidal, retinal, pial and peripapillary border tissue thicknesses, prelaminar tissue depth, lamina cribrosa (LC) depth, ON radius, and ON tortuosity. A central composite face-centered design (1045 models) was used to predict the effects of each morphological factor and their interactions on LC strains induced by 13 degrees of adduction. Subsequently, a further DOE analysis (1045 models) was conducted to study the effects and potential interactions between the top five morphological parameters identified from the initial DOE study and five critical tissue stiffnesses.
    UNASSIGNED: In the DOE analysis of 10 morphological parameters, the 5 most significant factors were ON tortuosity, dural radius, ON radius, scleral thickness, and LC depth. Further DOE analysis incorporating biomechanical parameters highlighted the importance of dural and LC stiffness. A larger dural radius and stiffer dura increased LC strains but the other main factors had the opposite effects. Notably, the significant interactions were found between dural radius with dural stiffness, ON radius, and ON tortuosity.
    UNASSIGNED: This study highlights the significant impact of morphological factors on LC deformations during eye movements, with key morphological effects being more pronounced than tissue stiffnesses.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们使用纯粹的扫视反应研究了在感知决策任务中是否相对于目标或固定发生干扰物抑制。先前的研究表明,在区分目标和干扰物的过程中,对目标的扫视偏离了分心器。一旦被歧视,干扰者被抑制,轨迹偏离了干扰物。扫视偏差大小根据目标干扰物之间的距离提供了对目标干扰物竞争的敏感度量。在以自我为中心的参考系中计划扫视时(相对于固定表示的位置),基于对象的抑制已被证明发生在非中心参考系中(对象相对于彼此独立地表示)。通过改变目标和干扰物的自我中心和分配中心距离,我们发现,在主动决策过程中,只有以自我为中心的距离才会导致扫视轨迹向分心者转移。当感知决策过程完成时,干扰者被抑制了,自我中心距离和分配中心距离都独立地导致扫视轨迹远离干扰物。这与独立的基于空间和对象的抑制机制一致。因此,我们建议使用分配中心图在皮层视觉区域保持干扰抑制,然后将其输入动眼区域进行扫视计划。
    We investigated whether distractor inhibition occurs relative to the target or fixation in a perceptual decision-making task using a purely saccadic response. Previous research has shown that during the process of discriminating a target from distractor, saccades made to a target deviate towards the distractor. Once discriminated, the distractor is inhibited, and trajectories deviate away from the distractor. Saccade deviation magnitudes provide a sensitive measure of target-distractor competition dependent on the distance between them. While saccades are planned in an egocentric reference frame (locations represented relative to fixation), object-based inhibition has been shown to occur in an allocentric reference frame (objects represented relative to each other independent of fixation). By varying the egocentric and allocentric distances of the target and distractor, we found that only egocentric distances contributed to saccade trajectories shifts towards the distractor during active decision-making. When the perceptual decision-making process was complete, and the distractor was inhibited, both ego- and allocentric distances independently contributed to saccade trajectory shifts away from the distractor. This is consistent with independent spatial and object-based inhibitory mechanisms. Therefore, we suggest that distractor inhibition is maintained in cortical visual areas with allocentric maps which then feeds into oculomotor areas for saccade planning.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号