Motion Perception

运动感知
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:研究表明,视觉感知学习(VPL)与修改更高级别的决策区域中的神经活动有关。然而,前额叶和视觉皮层在VPL中的因果作用尚不清楚.这里,我们研究了前额叶和视觉皮层的阳极经颅直流电刺激(tDCS)如何在早期和后期调节VPL,以及多个脑区的作用.
    方法:关于连贯运动方向识别任务的感知学习包括早期和后期。经过早期训练,参与者需要不断训练才能到达高原;一旦到达高原,参与者进入了后期阶段。60名参与者被随机分为五组。不管前期和后期的训练,四组在右背外侧前额叶皮层(rDLPFC)和右颞中区(rMT)接受多目标tDCS,rDLPFC上的单目标tDCS,和rMT或假刺激的单目标tDCS,一组在同侧大脑区域受到刺激(即,左MT)。
    结果:与假刺激相比,rDLPFC或rMT上的多目标和两个单目标tDCS提高了后测性能,并在早期加速了学习。然而,多目标tDCS和两个单目标tDCS为VPL带来了等效的好处。此外,当阳极tDCS应用于同侧大脑区域时,这些有益效果不存在.在后期,上述对多靶标或单靶标tDCS诱导的VPL的促进作用消失。
    结论:这项研究表明前额叶和视觉皮层在通过阳极tDCS进行视觉运动知觉学习中的因果作用,但未能通过同时刺激前额叶和视觉皮层来发现更大的有益效果。未来的研究应该研究多个大脑区域之间的功能关联,以进一步促进VPL。
    BACKGROUND: Research has shown that visual perceptual learning (VPL) is related to modifying neural activity in higher level decision-making regions. However, the causal roles of the prefrontal and visual cortexes in VPL are still unclear. Here, we investigated how anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the prefrontal and visual cortices modulates VPL in the early and later phases and the role of multiple brain regions.
    METHODS: Perceptual learning on the coherent motion direction identification task included early and later stages. After early training, participants needed to continuously train to reach a plateau; once the plateau was reached, participants entered a later stage. Sixty participants were randomly divided into five groups. Regardless of the training at the early and later stages, four groups received multitarget tDCS over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (rDLPFC) and right middle temporal area (rMT), single-target tDCS over the rDLPFC, and single-target tDCS over the rMT or sham stimulation, and one group was stimulated at the ipsilateral brain region (i.e., left MT).
    RESULTS: Compared with sham stimulation, multitarget and two single-target tDCS over the rDLPFC or rMT improved posttest performance and accelerated learning during the early period. However, multitarget tDCS and two single-target tDCS led to equivalent benefits for VPL. Additionally, these beneficial effects were absent when anodal tDCS was applied to the ipsilateral brain region. For the later period, the above facilitating effects on VPL induced by multitarget or single-target tDCS disappeared.
    CONCLUSIONS: This study suggested the causal role of the prefrontal and visual cortices in visual motion perceptual learning by anodal tDCS but failed to find greater beneficial effects by simultaneously stimulating the prefrontal and visual cortices. Future research should investigate the functional associations between multiple brain regions to further promote VPL.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    大量研究探索了生物运动(BM)人群的显着社会意义,主要集中在均匀分布的。然而,现实世界的BM人群通常表现出分层结构,而不是统一的安排。如何处理这种结构化的BM人群仍然是一个问题。这项研究调查了结构化BM人群在工作记忆(WM)中的表现,认识到WM在我们涉及BM的社交互动中的关键作用。我们提出了基于群体的集成假设,并通过成员识别任务对其进行了测试。要求参与者辨别所呈现的BM是否属于八个BM的先前记忆显示,每个人都有不同的行走方向。借鉴突出的格式塔原则作为组织线索,我们通过在实验1和2中分别应用接近度和相似性线索,在BM人群中构建了结构化组。在实验3中,我们通过增加子集之间的相似性来故意削弱刺激结构的可见性,探索结果的稳健性。始终如一,我们的发现表明,与子集的平均方向一致的BM更有可能被认为是记忆刺激的一部分.这表明WM固有地根据组织线索将结构化的BM人群组织成单独的集合。实质上,我们的结果阐明了WM内BM人群的分组和集成编码机制的同时操作。
    Massive studies have explored biological motion (BM) crowds processing for their remarkable social significance, primarily focused on uniformly distributed ones. However, real-world BM crowds often exhibit hierarchical structures rather than uniform arrangements. How such structured BM crowds are processed remains a subject of inquiry. This study investigates the representation of structured BM crowds in working memory (WM), recognizing the pivotal role WM plays in our social interactions involving BM. We propose the group-based ensemble hypothesis and test it through a member identification task. Participants were required to discern whether a presented BM belonged to a prior memory display of eight BM, each with distinct walking directions. Drawing on prominent Gestalt principles as organizational cues, we constructed structured groups within BM crowds by applying proximity and similarity cues in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively. In Experiment 3, we deliberately weakened the visibility of stimuli structures by increasing the similarity between subsets, probing the robustness of results. Consistently, our findings indicate that BM aligned with the mean direction of the subsets was more likely to be recognized as part of the memory stimuli. This suggests that WM inherently organizes structured BM crowds into separate ensembles based on organizational cues. In essence, our results illuminate the simultaneous operation of grouping and ensemble encoding mechanisms for BM crowds within WM.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    感知生物运动(BM)对于人类生存和社会交往至关重要。许多研究报道了自闭症谱系障碍的BM感知受损,其特点是社会互动不足。患有注意力缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)的儿童通常在社交互动中表现出类似的困难。然而,很少有研究调查患有ADHD的儿童的BM感知。这里,我们比较了处理局部运动学和全局构型线索的能力差异,BM感知的两个基本能力,在典型的发展和多动症儿童之间。我们进一步调查了使用社会反应量表测量的BM感知和社交互动技能之间的关系,并检查了潜在因素的贡献(例如性别,年龄,注意,和智力)到BM感知。结果显示,患有ADHD的儿童表现出非典型的BM感知。局部和全局BM处理显示出明显的特征。本地BM处理能力与社交互动技能有关,而全球BM加工能力随着年龄的增长而显著提高。严重的,ADHD儿童的一般BM感知(即局部和全局BM处理)可能受到持续注意能力的影响。这种关系主要是由推理智力介导的。这些发现阐明了ADHD中非典型的BM感知以及与BM感知相关的潜在因素。此外,这项研究提供了新的证据,表明BM感知是社会认知的标志,并促进了我们对局部和全局处理在BM感知和社会认知障碍中的潜在作用的理解.
    Perceiving biological motion (BM) is crucial for human survival and social interaction. Many studies have reported impaired BM perception in autism spectrum disorder, which is characterised by deficits in social interaction. Children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) often exhibit similar difficulties in social interaction. However, few studies have investigated BM perception in children with ADHD. Here, we compared differences in the ability to process local kinematic and global configurational cues, two fundamental abilities of BM perception, between typically developing and ADHD children. We further investigated the relationship between BM perception and social interaction skills measured using the Social Responsiveness Scale and examined the contributions of latent factors (e.g. sex, age, attention, and intelligence) to BM perception. The results revealed that children with ADHD exhibited atypical BM perception. Local and global BM processing showed distinct features. Local BM processing ability was related to social interaction skills, whereas global BM processing ability significantly improved with age. Critically, general BM perception (i.e. both local and global BM processing) may be affected by sustained attentional ability in children with ADHD. This relationship was primarily mediated by reasoning intelligence. These findings elucidate atypical BM perception in ADHD and the latent factors related to BM perception. Moreover, this study provides new evidence that BM perception is a hallmark of social cognition and advances our understanding of the potential roles of local and global processing in BM perception and social cognitive disorders.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    飞行昆虫主要依靠视觉运动来检测和跟踪物体。已经有很多关于飞行启发的目标检测算法的研究,但是很少有仅仅基于视觉运动而开发的。令人生畏的困难之一是前景-背景分割的神经和电路机制仍不清楚。我们先前的建模研究提出,叶保持平行路径具有不同的方向选择性,每个人都可以根据相对运动线索,以视网膜方式区分沿自己的首选方向移动的数字。以前的模型,然而,没有解决多个并行路径如何在其共同的下游提供唯一的检测输出。由于路径沿水平轴或垂直轴的优选方向彼此相反,在与物体相反的方向上移动的背景也激活了相应的叶路径。从所有路径到下游的不分青红皂白或无门控投影会将物体与移动的背景混合在一起,使得以前的模型在非平稳背景下失败。这里,我们通过提出单个小叶投影的背景运动相关门控是目标检测的关键来扩展先前的模型。假设大视场小叶板切向细胞执行门控以实现生物启发背景减除。该模型显示能够使用引起平移光流的移动相机或静态相机来实现对视频序列中的移动对象的鲁棒检测。该模型揭示了简洁的苍蝇算法在实际应用中的潜力。
    Flying insects rely mainly upon visual motion to detect and track objects. There has been a lot of research on fly inspired algorithms for object detection, but few have been developed based on visual motion alone. One of the daunting difficulties is that the neural and circuit mechanisms underlying the foreground-background segmentation are still unclear. Our previous modeling study proposed that the lobula held parallel pathways with distinct directional selectivity, each of which could retinotopically discriminate figures moving in its own preferred direction based on relative motion cues. The previous model, however, did not address how the multiple parallel pathways gave the only detection output at their common downstream. Since the preferred directions of the pathways along either horizontal or vertical axis were opposite to each other, the background moving in the opposite direction to an object also activated the corresponding lobula pathway. Indiscriminate or ungated projection from all the pathways to their downstream would mix objects with the moving background, making the previous model fail with non-stationary background. Here, we extend the previous model by proposing that the background motion-dependent gating of individual lobula projections is the key to object detection. Large-field lobula plate tangential cells are hypothesized to perform the gating to realize bioinspired background subtraction. The model is shown to be capable of implementing a robust detection of moving objects in video sequences with either a moving camera that induces translational optic flow or a static camera. The model sheds light on the potential of the concise fly algorithm in real-world applications.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    年轻人认为世界的移动速度比成年人慢,这是一个熟悉的现象。然而,这仍然是一个悬而未决的问题,为什么会这样。利用事件分割理论,使用人工神经网络方法进行脑电图(EEG)波束形成和非线性因果关系估计,我们研究了青少年和成人参与者分割电影时的神经活动。我们展示了当参与者被指示将电影分成有意义的单元时,青少年将传入信息划分为比成年人更少的封装片段或持续时间更长的片段。重要的是,在特定的神经振荡谱中,内侧额叶和下层感知区域之间以及枕部-颞部区域之间的定向交流解释了组间的行为差异。总的来说,这项研究表明,大脑区域之间定向通信的不同组织和大脑区域之间信息的低效传输是理解为什么年轻人认为世界移动缓慢的关键。
    That younger individuals perceive the world as moving slower than adults is a familiar phenomenon. Yet, it remains an open question why that is. Using event segmentation theory, electroencephalogram (EEG) beamforming and nonlinear causal relationship estimation using artificial neural network methods, we studied neural activity while adolescent and adult participants segmented a movie. We show when participants were instructed to segment a movie into meaningful units, adolescents partitioned incoming information into fewer encapsulated segments or episodes of longer duration than adults. Importantly, directed communication between medial frontal and lower-level perceptual areas and between occipito-temporal regions in specific neural oscillation spectrums explained behavioral differences between groups. Overall, the study reveals that a different organization of directed communication between brain regions and inefficient transmission of information between brain regions are key to understand why younger people perceive the world as moving slow.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Systematic Review
    目的:检查视觉功能之间的关系(即,对比敏感度,视野,色觉,和运动知觉)和认知障碍,包括任何“认知障碍”的定义,“轻度认知障碍,或痴呆症。
    方法:系统评价和荟萃分析。
    方法:任何设置;有(病例)或没有(对照)认知障碍的参与者。
    方法:我们检索了4个数据库(至2024年1月),并纳入了已发表的比较病例和对照组视觉功能的研究。在数据可用的情况下,计算95%CI的标准化平均差(SMD)。当病例为痴呆症患者时,数据足以进行荟萃分析。JoannaBriggs研究所的清单用于质量评估。
    结果:纳入51项研究/69份报告。横断面证据表明,痴呆症患者的对比敏感度功能和色觉比对照组更差:通过字母图上的对比敏感度(对数单位)来测量,SMD-1.22(95%CI-1.98,-0.47),或者在不同的空间频率下,-0.90(-1.21,-0.60);通过伪等色平板,-1.04(-1.59,-0.49);颜色排列,-1.30(-2.31,-0.29);或匹配测试,-0.51(-0.78,-0.24)。他们在运动知觉测试中的表现也较差,-1.20(-1.73,-0.67),和视野:平均偏差,-0.87(-1.29,-0.46),和模式标准偏差,-0.69(-1.24,-0.15)。当病例仅限于临床诊断为阿尔茨海默病的参与者时,结果相似。偏倚的来源包括研究人群或认知障碍的设置和定义缺乏明确性。2项纳入的纵向研究随访约10年,质量良好,但报告结果不一致。
    结论:在缺乏纵向数据的情况下,横断面研究表明,认知障碍患者的视觉功能比正常认知者差。需要额外的纵向数据来了解视功能不良是否先于认知障碍和视觉功能的最相关方面,痴呆病理学,和认知领域。
    OBJECTIVE: To examine relationships between visual function (ie, contrast sensitivity, visual field, color vision, and motion perception) and cognitive impairment, including any definition of \"cognitive impairment,\" mild cognitive impairment, or dementia.
    METHODS: Systematic review and meta-analyses.
    METHODS: Any settings; participants with (cases) or without (controls) cognitive impairment.
    METHODS: We searched 4 databases (to January 2024) and included published studies that compared visual function between cases and controls. Standardized mean differences (SMD) with 95% CIs were calculated where data were available. Data were sufficient for meta-analyses when cases were people with dementia. The Joanna Briggs Institute checklists were used for quality assessment.
    RESULTS: Fifty-one studies/69 reports were included. Cross-sectional evidence shows that people with dementia had worse contrast sensitivity function and color vision than controls: measured by contrast sensitivity (log units) on letter charts, SMD -1.22 (95% CI -1.98, -0.47), or at varied spatial frequencies, -0.92 (-1.28, -0.57); and by pseudoisochromatic plates, -1.04 (-1.59, -0.49); color arrangement, -1.30 (-2.31, -0.29); or matching tests, -0.51 (-0.78, -0.24). They also performed more poorly on tests of motion perception, -1.20 (-1.73, -0.67), and visual field: mean deviation, -0.87 (-1.29, -0.46), and pattern standard deviation, -0.69 (-1.24, -0.15). Results were similar when cases were limited to participants with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer disease. Sources of bias included lack of clarity on study populations or settings and definitions of cognitive impairment. The 2 included longitudinal studies with follow-ups of approximately 10 years were of good quality but reported inconsistent results.
    CONCLUSIONS: In the lack of longitudinal data, cross-sectional studies indicate that individuals with cognitive impairment have poorer visual function than those with normal cognition. Additional longitudinal data are needed to understand whether poor visual function precedes cognitive impairment and the most relevant aspects of visual function, dementia pathologies, and domains of cognition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尽管动物群体的自我组织对集体行为有着深远的影响,理解基本原理并将其应用于群体机器人技术仍然不完整。在这里,我们提出了一种对运动显著性(MS)感知的启发式度量,以从第一人称视角量化邻居的相对运动变化。利用三个大型鸟群数据集,我们探讨了这种对MS的感知如何与领导者-追随者(LF)关系的结构相关,并进一步对过去的MS感知和未来的速度一致性变化率进行个体水平的相关分析。我们观察到真实羊群中正相关的患病率,这表明个体将加速与具有较高MS的邻居的速度收敛。这一经验发现激励我们在群体模型中引入基于自适应MS(AMS)交互的概念。最后,我们在102个微型机器人的群中实现了AMS。群体实验表明,AMS在增强群体的自组织能力方面具有显着优势,可以从密闭环境中顺利撤离。
    Despite the profound implications of self-organization in animal groups for collective behaviors, understanding the fundamental principles and applying them to swarm robotics remains incomplete. Here we propose a heuristic measure of perception of motion salience (MS) to quantify relative motion changes of neighbors from first-person view. Leveraging three large bird-flocking datasets, we explore how this perception of MS relates to the structure of leader-follower (LF) relations, and further perform an individual-level correlation analysis between past perception of MS and future change rate of velocity consensus. We observe prevalence of the positive correlations in real flocks, which demonstrates that individuals will accelerate the convergence of velocity with neighbors who have higher MS. This empirical finding motivates us to introduce the concept of adaptive MS-based (AMS) interaction in swarm model. Finally, we implement AMS in a swarm of ~102 miniature robots. Swarm experiments show the significant advantage of AMS in enhancing self-organization of the swarm for smooth evacuations from confined environments.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究旨在探索弱视中观察到的视觉运动缺陷的潜在机制。
    24名弱视(25.8±3.8岁;15名男性)和22名正常参与者(25.8±2.1岁;8名男性)参加了这项研究。指示参与者使用鼠标在计算机屏幕上连续跟踪随机移动的高斯目标。在实验1中,参与者以六种不同的目标大小执行了跟踪任务。在实验2和3中,他们被要求用调整到个体阈值的对比度跟踪目标。跟踪性能由计算为目标和鼠标位移之间的互相关的核函数表示。高峰,延迟,提取并比较两组的籽粒宽度。
    在实验1中,目标尺寸对内核峰有显着影响(F(1.649,46.170)=200.958,P=4.420×10-22)。在最小的目标尺寸下,弱视组的峰值明显低于正常组(0.089±0.023vs.0.107±0.020,t(28)=-2.390,P=0.024),与弱视眼对比敏感度相关(r=0.739,P=0.002)。在实验2和3中,有同样可见的刺激,两组之间的内核仍存在差异(P均<0.05)。
    当刺激能见度得到补偿时,弱视参与者的追踪表现仍然明显较差.
    UNASSIGNED: This study aimed to explore the underlying mechanisms of the observed visuomotor deficit in amblyopia.
    UNASSIGNED: Twenty-four amblyopic (25.8 ± 3.8 years; 15 males) and 22 normal participants (25.8 ± 2.1 years; 8 males) took part in the study. The participants were instructed to continuously track a randomly moving Gaussian target on a computer screen using a mouse. In experiment 1, the participants performed the tracking task at six different target sizes. In experiments 2 and 3, they were asked to track a target with the contrast adjusted to individual\'s threshold. The tracking performance was represented by the kernel function calculated as the cross-correlation between the target and mouse displacements. The peak, latency, and width of the kernel were extracted and compared between the two groups.
    UNASSIGNED: In experiment 1, target size had a significant effect on the kernel peak (F(1.649, 46.170) = 200.958, P = 4.420 × 10-22). At the smallest target size, the peak in the amblyopic group was significantly lower than that in the normal group (0.089 ± 0.023 vs. 0.107 ± 0.020, t(28) = -2.390, P = 0.024) and correlated with the contrast sensitivity function (r = 0.739, P = 0.002) in the amblyopic eyes. In experiments 2 and 3, with equally visible stimuli, there were still differences in the kernel between the two groups (all Ps < 0.05).
    UNASSIGNED: When stimulus visibility was compensated, amblyopic participants still showed significantly poorer tracking performance.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    旋转的运动,作为人机界面中的动态符号,在界面设计和图形设计方面引起了广泛的关注。本研究旨在通过探索视觉系统在每秒5-1800度的速度范围内对简单和复杂旋转图标的感知偏好,为界面设计建立速度基准。该研究对12名参与者进行了两次实验,以检查观察者的速度差异(JNDS)和旋转图标的感知速度。实验一使用恒定刺激方法在八速水平上测量JNDS,达到14.9-29%的范围。在这个基础上,实验二提出了给定范围内的速度序列,并使用评级量表方法来评估观察者对速度序列的主观感知速度。研究结果表明,速度的增加影响了区分速度的能力;分类低,中等,并以10、180和720度/秒的速度识别出高速,分别。发现形状复杂性可以调节视觉系统对实际速度的感知,使得在高于180度/s的转速下,复杂的图标似乎比简单的图标旋转得更快。最重要的是,该研究将定量方法和计量学应用于接口设计,为设计工作流程提供更科学的方法。
    The motion of rotation, which served as a dynamic symbol within human-computer interfaces, has garnered extensive attention in interface and graphic design. This study aimed to establish speed benchmarks for interface design by exploring visual system preferences for the perception of both simple and complex rotating icons within the velocity range of 5-1800 degrees per second. The research conducted two experiments with 12 participants to examine the observers\' just noticeable difference in speed (JNDS) and perceived speed for rotational icons. Experiment one measured the JNDS over eight-speed levels using a constant stimulus method, achieving a range of 14.9-29%. Building on this, experiment two proposed a sequence of speeds within the given range and used a rating scale method to assess observers \' subjective perception of the speed series\' rapidity. The findings indicated that speed increases impacted the ability to differentiate between speeds; key points for categorizing low, medium, and high speeds were identified at 10, 180, and 720 degrees/s, respectively. Shape complexity was found to modulate the visual system\'s perception of actual speed, such that at rotation speeds above 180 degrees/s, complex icons appeared to rotate faster than simpler ones. Most importantly, the study applied quantitative methods and metrology to interface design, offering a more scientific approach to the design workflow.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    视觉系统中感官信息的传输和处理需要时间。对于运动知觉,我们的大脑可以通过外推机制克服这种内在的神经延迟,并准确地预测连续移动物体的当前位置。但是,当运动突然变化并且预测出错时,系统将如何表现?在这里,我们通过研究人类观察者在各种突然运动变化的情况下运动物体的感知位置来解决这个问题。我们开发了一个任务,其中一个酒吧单调地水平移动,然后运动突然停止,反转,或消失-然后-围绕两条垂直固定参考线反转。我们的结果表明,参与者高估了停止杆的位置,但在运动反转条件下没有感觉到超调。当在反向点添加时间间隙时,终点的感知超调与间隙持续时间成比例。我们的模型表明,物体消失时对物体位置的高估并不是其速度的线性函数,而是逐渐淡出。因此,可以在单个过程中调和这些结果,其中皮层运动预测机制和后期感觉瞬态视觉输入之间存在相互作用。
    Transmission and processing of sensory information in the visual system takes time. For motion perception, our brain can overcome this intrinsic neural delay through extrapolation mechanisms and accurately predict the current position of a continuously moving object. But how does the system behave when the motion abruptly changes and the prediction becomes wrong? Here we address this question by studying the perceived position of a moving object with various abrupt motion changes by human observers. We developed a task in which a bar is monotonously moving horizontally, and then motion suddenly stops, reverses, or disappears-then-reverses around two vertical stationary reference lines. Our results showed that participants overestimated the position of the stopping bar but did not perceive an overshoot in the motion reversal condition. When a temporal gap was added at the reverse point, the perceptual overshoot of the end point scaled with the gap durations. Our model suggests that the overestimation of the object position when it disappears is not linear as a function of its speeds but gradually fades out. These results can thus be reconciled in a single process where there is an interplay of the cortical motion prediction mechanisms and the late sensory transient visual inputs.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号