social behavior

社会行为
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对顺序道德行为(SMB)的实验研究发现,从事最初的道德(或不道德)行为有时会导致道德平衡(即,在积极和消极行为之间切换),有时转向道德一致性(即,保持积极或消极行为的一致模式)。在两个荟萃分析中,我们提出了SMB研究和测试主持人的第一个全面综合,以确定道德平衡和道德一致性最有可能发生的条件。荟萃分析1(k=217效应大小,N=31,242)表明,从事最初的积极行为只会可靠地导致道德许可(即,平衡)在测量消极目标行为的研究中(对冲\'g=0.25,95%CI[0.16,0.44]),并且仅在使用亲社会要求(对冲\'g=-0.44,95%CI[-0.59,-0.29])的上门研究中产生正一致性。荟萃分析2(k=132效应大小,N=14,443)揭示,从事最初的消极行为只能可靠地导致道德补偿(即,平衡)在测量积极目标行为参与度的研究中(对冲=0.27,95%CI[0.18,0.37])。我们没有发现在任何情况下可靠的负面一致性影响的证据。这些结果不能用当前的SMB效应理论来解释,因此,需要进一步的研究来更好地理解在观察到的条件下推动道德平衡和一致性的机制。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2024APA,保留所有权利)。
    Experimental research on sequential moral behavior (SMB) has found that engaging in an initial moral (or immoral) behavior can sometimes lead to moral balancing (i.e., switching between positive and negative behavior) and sometimes to moral consistency (i.e., maintaining a consistent pattern of positive or negative behavior). In two meta-analyses, we present the first comprehensive syntheses of SMB studies and test moderators to identify the conditions under which moral balancing and moral consistency are most likely to occur. Meta-Analysis 1 (k = 217 effect sizes, N = 31,242) revealed that engaging in an initial positive behavior only reliably resulted in moral licensing (i.e., balancing) in studies that measured engagement in negative target behaviors (Hedges\' g = 0.25, 95% CI [0.16, 0.44]) and only resulted in positive consistency in foot-in-the-door studies using prosocial requests (Hedges\' g = -0.44, 95% CI [-0.59, -0.29]). Meta-Analysis 2 (k = 132 effect sizes, N = 14,443) revealed that engaging in an initial negative behavior only reliably resulted in moral compensation (i.e., balancing) in studies that measured engagement in positive target behaviors (Hedges\' g = 0.27, 95% CI [0.18, 0.37]). We found no evidence for reliable negative consistency effects in any conditions. These results cannot be readily explained by current theories of SMB effects, and so further research is needed to better understand the mechanisms that drive moral balancing and consistency under the conditions observed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:亲社会性是指有意使他人受益的自愿行为。大多数现有文献表明,与年轻人相比,老年人倾向于采取更多的亲社会行为,而一些研究表明,在某些条件下,老年人可能不是那么亲社会。本研究旨在通过进行定性系统评价和定量荟萃分析来总结混合发现并量化亲社会的年龄差异。
    方法:基于5个数据库进行文献检索。遵循系统审查和荟萃分析指南的首选报告项目,该审查在PROSPERO(CRD4202233373)注册。
    结果:基于51项研究的定性综合,老年人(n=109,911)比年轻人(n=68,501)更亲社会。46项研究的荟萃分析进一步支持这种年龄效应(Hedges\'g=0.31,95%置信区间[0.24,0.37]),这种年龄效应可能会受到亲社会类型的调节。我们在分享中发现了适度的年龄效应(对冲=0.53),但在帮助中年龄效应不显著(对冲=0.11),安慰(对冲\'g=-0.20),或混合亲社会(对冲\'g=0.15)。此外,只有当老年人的社会经济地位高于年轻人时,年龄效应才显著.
    结论:未来的研究应该开发更全面的亲社会措施,检查更多影响衰老和亲社会的变量,并研究亲社会的神经机制,以全面了解亲社会的年龄差异。
    OBJECTIVE: Prosociality refers to voluntary behaviors that intend to benefit others. Most of the existing literature suggests that older adults tend to act more prosocially compared to younger adults, whereas some studies show that older adults might not be that prosocial under certain conditions. The current study aimed to summarize the mixed findings and quantify the age difference in prosociality by conducting a qualitative systematic review and a quantitative meta-analysis.
    METHODS: Literature search was conducted based on 5 databases. The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines were followed and this review was registered at PROSPERO (CRD42022333373).
    RESULTS: Based on the qualitative synthesis of 51 studies, older adults (n = 109,911) were more prosocial than younger adults (n = 68,501). The meta-analysis of 46 studies further supported this age effect (Hedges\' g = 0.31, 95% confidence interval [0.24, 0.37]), and this age effect might be moderated by the types of prosociality. We discovered a moderate age effect in sharing (Hedges\' g = 0.53), but a nonsignificant age effect in helping (Hedges\' g = 0.11), comforting (Hedges\' g = -0.20), or mixed prosociality (Hedges\' g = 0.15). Additionally, the age effect was only significant when older adults had higher socioeconomic status than younger adults.
    CONCLUSIONS: Future research should develop more comprehensive measures of prosociality, examine more variables that influence aging and prosociality, and investigate the neural mechanism(s) of prosociality to achieve a thorough understanding of the age difference in prosociality.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    以其独特的群体结构,蚂蚁之间的竞争(膜翅目:Formicidae)可能特别激烈,在适当的情况下,殖民地可能愿意牺牲大量个人来获得资源或领土。在这次审查中,我们涵盖了蚂蚁竞争升级为战斗的情况,战斗战略和战术,以及这些战斗的分析方法。殖民地选择战斗的趋势可能因物种和情况而异,我们会详细回顾一下。因为他们的团体规模很大,蚂蚁冲突可以遵循与许多其他物种不同的模式,通过各种专业的改编和战斗策略,例如专门的工人阶层和需要迅速招募大量同胞。这些相同的大群体规模也可以使蚂蚁战斗适合数学分析,特别是在兰切斯特的法律的背景下,考虑总数如何影响对抗的结果。然而,动态行为通常会破坏现实场景中理想化的数学预测,即使这些仍然可以阐明这种行为的解释。我们还系统地报道了关于蚂蚁群体之间战斗的文献,提出了其他一些关于具有独特殖民地组织的物种的有趣研究,如军蚁和削叶蚁。
    With their unique colony structure, competition between ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) can be particularly intense, with colonies potentially willing to sacrifice large number of individuals to obtain resources or territory under the right circumstances. In this review, we cover circumstances in which ant competition escalates into combat, battle strategies and tactics, and analysis methods for these battles. The trends for when colonies choose to fight can vary greatly dependent on the species and situation, which we review in detail. Because of their large group sizes, ant conflicts can follow different patterns than many other species, with a variety of specialist adaptations and battle strategies, such as specialized worker classes and the need to rapidly recruit large number of compatriots. These same large group sizes also can make ant fighting amenable to mathematical analysis, particularly in the context of Lanchester\'s laws that consider how total numbers influence the outcome of a confrontation. Yet, dynamic behavior can often disrupt idealized mathematical predictions in real-world scenarios, even though these can still shed light on the explanations for such behavior. We also systematically cover the literature on battles between groups of ants, presenting several other interesting studies on species with unique colony organization, such as army ants and leafcutter ants.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们在日常生活中的行动和决定受到社会互动的严重影响,它们是涉及行动的动态反馈回路,reactions,以及个体主体之间的内部认知过程。社会互动诱导人际同步,发生在不同的生物行为水平,包括行为,生理,和神经活动。超扫描-一种同时测量多个大脑区域活动的神经成像技术-提供了强大的第二人称神经科学工具,用于研究交互式社会行为期间神经过程的相位对齐。神经同步,通过超扫描显示,是一种称为大脑间同步的现象-据称,通过在持续的共享互动中促进对彼此的社会行为的适当预期和反应来促进社会互动的过程。在这次审查中,我探索了治疗性双脑方法,该方法使用非侵入性脑刺激以基于第二人称神经科学调节社交互动的脑间同步为目标。人工诱导大脑之间的同步是物理疗法的潜在辅助技术,心理治疗,和疼痛治疗-受到治疗师和患者之间的社交互动的强烈影响。个性化刺激参数的双脑方法必须考虑时间,空间,和振荡因素。多数据融合分析,大脑间可塑性的评估,一个闭环系统,脑-脑接口可以支持个性化刺激。
    Our actions and decisions in everyday life are heavily influenced by social interactions, which are dynamic feedback loops involving actions, reactions, and internal cognitive processes between individual agents. Social interactions induce interpersonal synchrony, which occurs at different biobehavioral levels and comprises behavioral, physiological, and neurological activities. Hyperscanning-a neuroimaging technique that simultaneously measures the activity of multiple brain regions-has provided a powerful second-person neuroscience tool for investigating the phase alignment of neural processes during interactive social behavior. Neural synchronization, revealed by hyperscanning, is a phenomenon called inter-brain synchrony- a process that purportedly facilitates social interactions by prompting appropriate anticipation of and responses to each other\'s social behaviors during ongoing shared interactions. In this review, I explored the therapeutic dual-brain approach using noninvasive brain stimulation to target inter-brain synchrony based on second-person neuroscience to modulate social interaction. Artificially inducing synchrony between the brains is a potential adjunct technique to physiotherapy, psychotherapy, and pain treatment- which are strongly influenced by the social interaction between the therapist and patient. Dual-brain approaches to personalize stimulation parameters must consider temporal, spatial, and oscillatory factors. Multiple data fusion analysis, the assessment of inter-brain plasticity, a closed-loop system, and a brain-to-brain interface can support personalized stimulation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    催产素给药在为自闭症患者提供个性化支持方面表现出相当大的希望。然而,评估催产素给药对自闭症特征的影响的研究得出的结果不一致.本系统综述和荟萃分析使用最新开发的方法来准确评估效应大小依赖性和发表偏倚的潜在影响,研究了催产素给药对自闭症社会和常规行为的影响。我们的频率荟萃分析得出了催产素给药对自闭症患者社会结果的影响的显著汇总效应大小估计(d=0.22,p<0.001)。常规行为结果的汇总效应大小估计没有统计学意义(d=0.14,p=0.22),随访测试表明效应大小估计在统计学上也不相等(Z=-1.06,p=0.2),假设最小效应大小为0.25。发表偏见的频率论和贝叶斯评估,以及催产素对自闭症社会结果影响的稳健贝叶斯荟萃分析结果,这表明,由于出版偏差,汇总效应大小可能会膨胀。未来的研究应旨在通过预先注册分析计划来减少偏差,并通过更大的样本量来提高精度。
    Oxytocin administration has demonstrated considerable promise for providing individualized support for autistic people. However, studies evaluating the effects of oxytocin administration on autistic characteristics have yielded inconsistent results. This systematic review and meta-analysis investigates the effect of oxytocin administration on social and routinized behaviors in autism using recently developed methods to accurately assess the potential impact of effect size dependency and publication bias. Our frequentist meta-analysis yielded a significant summary effect size estimate for the impact of oxytocin administration on social outcomes in autism (d = 0.22, p < 0.001). The summary effect size estimate for routinized behavior outcomes was not statistically significant (d = 0.14, p = 0.22), with a follow up test indicating that the effect size estimate was not either statistically equivalent (Z = -1.06, p = 0.2), assuming a smallest effect size of interest of 0.25. Frequentist and Bayesian assessments for publication bias, as well as results from Robust Bayesian meta-analysis of oxytocin effects on social outcomes in autism, indicated that summary effect sizes might be inflated due to publication bias. Future studies should aim to reduce bias by preregistering analysis plans and to increase precision with larger sample sizes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    与他人的互动需要社会调整(即,不断适应不断变化的社会情况)。混合证据表明,帕金森病(PwPD)患者在丘脑底核深部脑刺激(STN-DBS)后的社会调节发生了积极和消极的变化。迄今为止,然而,不存在对这些变化的荟萃分析.因此,本研究旨在回顾STN-DBS对PwPD社会适应影响的证据.为此,在MEDLINE进行了系统的文献检索。使用随机效应模型和具有95%置信区间(CI)的标准化平均差(SMD)进行荟萃分析。MINORS工具用于评估研究的方法学质量。最初的文献检索确定了13124篇文章,其中1,550个全文被评估为合格性。最终纳入了八项研究;对于七篇文章,有足够的数据进行荟萃分析。大多数研究发现手术前的社会适应损害有轻度损害。荟萃分析显示,术后6个月(SMD=0.25;95CI=-0.03,0.53;P=0.08)和12个月以上(SMD=0.26;95CI=-0.03,0.55;P=0.07),社会适应没有显著变化,但有统计学改善趋势。87.5%的研究方法学质量中等,12.5%良好。虽然在大多数研究中报道了手术前社会适应的轻度损害,数据表明STN-DBS可能对该结果产生有益影响.然而,还没有足够的数据来得出确切的结论。作为日常运作的一项关键技能,在PwPD的STN-DBS试验中,应更经常地将社会适应定义为结局,并应在临床常规中加以考虑.
    Interactions with others need social adjustment (i.e., the constant accommodation to changing social situations). Mixed evidence indicates positive as well as negative changes in social adjustment after subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation (STN-DBS) in people with Parkinson\'s Disease (PwPD). To date, however, no meta-analysis of these changes exists. Thus, the study aim was to review evidence of the effects of STN-DBS on social adjustment in PwPD. For this purpose, a systematic literature search in MEDLINE was conducted. The meta-analysis was performed using a random effects model and standardized mean differences (SMDs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). The MINORS tool was used to assess the methodological quality of the studies. The initial literature search identified 13,124 articles, of which 1,550 full texts were assessed for eligibility. Eight studies were finally included; for seven articles sufficient data for a meta-analysis was available. Most studies found mild impairment in social adjustment impairment pre-surgery. The meta-analysis revealed no significant changes but a statistical trend towards improvement in social adjustment up to six months (SMD = 0.25; 95%CI=-0.03,0.53; P = 0.08) and over 12 months (SMD = 0.26; 95%CI=-0.03,0.55; P = 0.07) post-surgery. Methodological quality was moderate in 87.5% of the studies and good in 12.5%. While mild impairment in social adjustment pre-surgery was reported in most studies, the data indicate that STN-DBS might yield beneficial effects toward this outcome. However, not enough data yet exists to draw firm conclusions. As a crucial skill for everyday functioning, social adjustment should be more often defined as an outcome in STN-DBS trials in PwPD and should be considered in clinical routines.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    张量,与智能手机使用相关的普遍社交行为,涉及用户忽略他们的对话伙伴与他们的手机互动。尽管不断探索其与错过恐惧概念(FOMO)的联系,现有文献中的研究结果表现出明显的不一致性。为了解决这个差距,这项研究采用了系统综述和荟萃分析,以审查phubbing行为与FOMO之间的复杂关系.全面系统的审查,时间跨度至2023年12月10日,包括PubMed等数据库,Scopus,WebofScience,ProQuest,谷歌学者。由此产生的数据集包括27项符合条件的研究,纳入来自15个国家的20415名参与者的见解。使用纽卡斯尔渥太华量表对研究质量进行了严格的评估,而统计分析是使用RStudio精心进行的。显示出强烈的正关联,吐痰行为与FOMO显著相关(效应大小[ES]=0.43,95%CI:0.36,0.49,I2:97.5%,τ2:0.05)。使用修剪和填充方法校正检测到的出版偏差,纳入了另外16项研究,加强研究结果的稳健性。适度分析揭示了位置的显著影响(p<0.01),收入水平(p<0.01),抽样法(p<0.01),phubbing秤(p<0.01),与FOMO量表和类型有关(p<0.01)的估计关系。单变量荟萃回归强调了样本量的实质性影响(R2=11.81%,p<0.01),而多元元回归说明了出版年份的综合影响,学习质量评分,样本量,平均年龄,和女性比例在估计关系上(k=19,R2=52.85%,I2=93.78%,p<0.05)。此外,事后影响力分析,通过留一法进行,为考试提供了更多的深度。
    Phubbing, a pervasive social behavior linked to smartphone usage, involves users neglecting their conversation partners to engage with their phones. Despite consistent exploration of its association with the concept of fear of missing out (FOMO), findings in the existing literature exhibit notable inconsistency. To address this gap, this study employs a systematic review and meta-analysis to scrutinize the intricate relationship between phubbing behavior and FOMO. A comprehensive systematic review, spanning up to December 10, 2023, encompassed databases such as PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, ProQuest, and Google Scholar. The resulting dataset comprised 27 eligible studies, incorporating insights from 20,415 participants across 15 countries. Rigorous evaluation of study quality was executed using the Newcastle Ottawa Scale, while statistical analyses were meticulously conducted using R Studio. Revealing a robust positive association, phubbing behavior was significantly linked to FOMO (effect size[ES] = 0.43, 95% CI: 0.36, 0.49, I2: 97.5%, τ2: 0.05). Correcting for detected publication bias using the Trim and Fill method, an additional 16 studies were included, fortifying the robustness of the findings. Moderation analysis uncovered significant influences of location (p < 0.01), income level (p < 0.01), sampling method (p < 0.01), phubbing scale (p < 0.01), and FOMO scale and type (p < 0.01) on the estimated relationship. Univariate meta-regression highlighted the substantial impact of sample size (R2 = 11.81%, p < 0.01), while multivariate meta-regression illuminated the combined effects of publication year, study quality score, sample size, mean age, and female proportion on the estimated relationship (k = 19, R2 = 52.85%, I2 = 93.78%, p < 0.05). Furthermore, post hoc influential analysis, conducted through the leave-one-out method, offered additional depth to the examination.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目前还没有关于在早期发展研究中使用社交机器人的越来越多的文献的系统综述。设计适当的幼儿研究方法对于扩大我们对幼儿社会和认知发展的理解至关重要。本范围综述系统地研究了有关使用社交机器人研究2至35个月婴儿的社会和认知发展的现有文献。此外,它旨在确定研究重点,调查结果,并报告了在研究中使用机器人时的差距和挑战。我们纳入了1990年至2023年5月29日发表的实证研究。我们在PsychINFO中搜索文献,ERIC,WebofScience,和PsyArXiv。29项研究符合纳入标准,并使用范围审查方法进行了映射。我们的研究结果表明,大多数研究都是定量的,在实验室环境中进行的实验设计,其中儿童在一对一的情况下接触物理存在或虚拟机器人。我们发现机器人被用来研究四个主要概念:动画概念,行动理解,模仿,和早期的对话技巧。许多研究集中在幼儿是否将机器人视为代理人或社会伙伴。研究表明,幼儿可以在某些情况下学习和理解社交机器人,但并非总是如此。例如,儿童对社交机器人的理解通常是由行为互动和偶然的机器人促进的。这项范围审查强调了设计社交机器人的必要性,这些机器人可以参与早期发展研究的互动和偶然的社会行为。
    There is currently no systematic review of the growing body of literature on using social robots in early developmental research. Designing appropriate methods for early childhood research is crucial for broadening our understanding of young children\'s social and cognitive development. This scoping review systematically examines the existing literature on using social robots to study social and cognitive development in infants and toddlers aged between 2 and 35 months. Moreover, it aims to identify the research focus, findings, and reported gaps and challenges when using robots in research. We included empirical studies published between 1990 and May 29, 2023. We searched for literature in PsychINFO, ERIC, Web of Science, and PsyArXiv. Twenty-nine studies met the inclusion criteria and were mapped using the scoping review method. Our findings reveal that most studies were quantitative, with experimental designs conducted in a laboratory setting where children were exposed to physically present or virtual robots in a one-to-one situation. We found that robots were used to investigate four main concepts: animacy concept, action understanding, imitation, and early conversational skills. Many studies focused on whether young children regard robots as agents or social partners. The studies demonstrated that young children could learn from and understand social robots in some situations but not always. For instance, children\'s understanding of social robots was often facilitated by robots that behaved interactively and contingently. This scoping review highlights the need to design social robots that can engage in interactive and contingent social behaviors for early developmental research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们通过利用大型跨国公司关于领导者涌现的数据库(k=120个样本,N=32,579)和领导者有效性(k=116,N=42,487)以扩展Judge等人。(2002)“大五人格与领导力”的经典元分析。通过测试植根于文化认可的内隐领导理论和社会分析理论的新颖假设,我们提供三个独特的见解。首先,在集体主义社会(重视与一个群体相互依存的文化)中,五因素模型特征-尤其是领导者外向性和和睦性-是领导者有效性的更强预测因子,这与在这种文化中加强社会协调的理论需要是一致的。第二,提出了一个理论模型,以指定领导者大五特质效应是由领导者行为介导的(证实了“考虑”中介外向性和和睦性,而启动结构是在责任心的中介,外向,和开放性)。第三,特质诚实-谦逊有力地预测了超越五大特质的领导者有效性,扩大特质方法。讨论了理解人格特质何时以及为什么可以预测领导能力的新含义。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2024APA,保留所有权利)。
    We advance the trait approach to leadership by leveraging a large multinational database on leader emergence (k = 120 samples, N = 32,579) and leader effectiveness (k = 116, N = 42,487) to extend Judge et al.\'s (2002) classic meta-analysis of Big Five personality and leadership. By testing novel hypotheses rooted in culturally endorsed implicit leadership theory and socioanalytic theory, we offer three unique insights. First, in collectivist societies (cultures that value interdependence with one\'s group), the five factor model traits-and leader Extraversion and Agreeableness in particular-are stronger predictors of leader effectiveness, consistent with the theorized need for enhanced social coordination in such cultures. Second, a theoretical model is proposed to specify that leader Big Five trait effects are mediated by leader behavior (confirming that Consideration mediates Extraversion and Agreeableness, whereas Initiating Structure mediates Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Openness). Third, trait Honesty-Humility robustly predicts leader effectiveness beyond the Big Five traits, expanding the trait approach. New implications for understanding when and why personality traits predict leadership are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人们对其他人感到忠诚,groups,组织,或日常生活中的道德规范。这种社会承诺可以带来积极的结果,例如提高工作满意度或关系寿命;然而,对承诺的感觉也可能有不利影响。安然或大众汽车等公司最近备受瞩目的欺诈或腐败案件可能受到对该组织或同事的坚定承诺的影响。尽管社会承诺可能会增加不诚实的行为,很少有关于何时以及如何发生这种情况的系统知识。在本项目中,我们审阅了20988篇文章,专注于实验性操纵社会承诺和衡量不诚实行为的研究。我们保留了121篇文章中的445种效应大小,共有来自33个国家的91,683名参与者。我们没有发现社会承诺增加或减少不诚实行为的证据。尽管如此,我们确实发现了证据,表明效果在很大程度上取决于承诺的目标。对其他个人或团体的忠诚会降低诚实行为(g=-0.17[-0.24,-0.11]),而通过诚实宣誓或承诺来致力于诚实规范的感觉会增加诚实行为(g=0.27[0.19,0.36])。分析确定了几个调节变量,并检测到不同效应的某种程度的发表偏差。我们的发现强调了不同形式的社会承诺对不诚实行为的不同影响,并建议不同形式的承诺的组合可能是在组织背景下打击腐败和不诚实行为的可能手段。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2024APA,保留所有权利)。
    People feel committed to other individuals, groups, organizations, or moral norms in many contexts of everyday life. Such social commitment can lead to positive outcomes, such as increased job satisfaction or relationship longevity; yet, there can also be detrimental effects to feeling committed. Recent high-profile cases of fraud or corruption in companies like Enron or Volkswagen are likely influenced by strong commitment to the organization or coworkers. Although social commitment might increase dishonest behavior, there is little systematic knowledge about when and how this may occur. In the present project, we reviewed 20,988 articles, focusing on studies that experimentally manipulated social commitment and measured dishonest behavior. We retained 445 effect sizes from 121 articles featuring a total of 91,683 participants across 33 countries. We found no evidence that social commitment increases or reduces dishonest behavior in general. Nonetheless, we did find evidence that the effect strongly depends on the target of the commitment. Feeling committed to other individuals or groups reduces honest behavior (g = -0.17 [-0.24, -0.11]), whereas feeling committed to honesty norms through honesty oaths or pledges increases honest behavior (g = 0.27 [0.19, 0.36]). The analysis identified several moderating variables and detected some degree of publication bias across effects. Our findings highlight the diverging effects of different forms of social commitment on dishonest behavior and suggest a combination of the different forms of commitment could be a possible means to combat corruption and dishonest behavior in the organizational context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号