Visual perception

视觉感知
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    建筑物是我们物理环境的组成部分,对于建筑元素的组织完整性具有美学意义。虽然格式塔原则在设计教育中是必不可少的,它们与建筑特征的关系仍未得到充分研究。本研究探讨了格式塔原理和复杂性水平如何通过使用问卷调查和眼动追踪来影响建筑立面的评估。二十四个二维黑白立面图纸,使用选定的格式塔原则(相似性和接近性)进行操作,以实现不同级别的复杂性(低,中高),向79名参与者展示。结果表明,在选定的格式塔原则中,美学等级和复杂性水平之间存在负线性关系。此外,正如预期的那样,参与者的注视次数最多,最短的固定持续时间,和最低的美学评级为更高的复杂性水平。涉及格式塔原则的结果表明,基于接近度的设计获得了更高的美学评价,要求更少的时间,引起的注视次数减少,并导致较短的固定持续时间。相反,基于相似性的设计获得了较低的美学评级,要求更多的时间,引起更多的注视,并导致更长的固定持续时间。这些发现提供了对建筑审美体验的见解,并为未来的研究方向提供了依据。
    Buildings are an integral part of our physical environment and have aesthetic significance with respect to the organizational integrity of architectural elements. While Gestalt principles are essential in design education, their relationship with architectural features remains understudied. The present study explored how Gestalt principles and complexity levels influence evaluations of building façades through the use of questionnaires and eye tracking. Twenty-four two-dimensional black and white façade drawings, manipulated using selected Gestalt principles (similarity and proximity) to achieve different levels of complexity (low, medium & high), were presented to 79 participants. The results suggested a negative linear relationship between aesthetic ratings and complexity levels across selected Gestalt principles. In addition, as expected, participants had the highest number of fixations, shortest fixation durations, and lowest aesthetic ratings for higher levels of complexity. Results involving Gestalt principles revealed that proximity-based designs received higher aesthetic ratings, demanded less time, elicited lower number of fixations, and resulted in shorter fixation durations. Conversely, similarity-based designs received lower aesthetic ratings, demanded more time, elicited higher number of fixations, and resulted in longer fixation durations. These findings offer insights into architectural aesthetic experiences and inform future research directions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    为了研究四个视觉元素的影响,即文本,颜色,image,和形状,关于中国消费者观察营养标签时的视觉感知,以及提高他们对营养信息的关注,这项研究通过眼球运动实验检查了包含这些元素的营养标签的视觉效果,问卷调查,主观评价,等研究方法。目的是确定最佳设计解决方案。结果显示,参与者对圆形x图像组表现出最高的注意力,其次是颜色组。因此,卓越的图像设计和合适的配色方案可以显着提高消费者在浏览过程中的注意力。本研究为食品营养标签的重新设计提供了有价值的参考和指导,同时也为视觉感知在其他领域的应用提供了研究见解。
    To investigate the impact of four visual elements, namely text, colour, image, and shape, on the visual perception of Chinese consumers when observing nutrition labels, as well as to enhance their attention towards nutritional information, this study examines the visual effects of nutrition labels incorporating these elements through eye movement experiments, questionnaire surveys, subjective evaluations, and other research methods. The aim is to determine the optimal design solution. The results revealed that participants displayed the highest level of attention towards the round x image group, followed by the colour group. Thus, exceptional image design and a suitable colour scheme can significantly enhance consumers\' attention during browsing. This study offers valuable references and guidance for the redesign of food nutrition labels, while also presenting research insights for the application of visual perception in other domains.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究旨在研究基于视觉实践的职业治疗(VPOT)计划对发育性阅读障碍(DD)儿童的阅读和运动技能的影响。42名儿童被纳入研究。此外,在VPOT之前,对参与者应用了朗读和阅读理解测试2(ORSRC-2)和Bruininininks-Oseretsky运动能力测试2-简短表格(BOT2-BF)。根据研究设计,VPOT被应用于每周两个疗程,为期8周。在此期间,B组为对照组。在这八周结束时,对两组进行评估测试.然后,A组为对照组,B组为干预组,并将VPOT应用于B组。在另外8周结束时,对两组进行了第三次评估测试.当检查最终的ORSRC-2结果时,VPOT被认为是提高阅读技能的有效计划。此外,当检查最终的BOT2-BF结果时,确定VPOT可有效改善运动技能(p<0.05)。我们认为,重要的是进行全面的研究,例如VPOT计划,以解决DD儿童的身体和学习活动中的问题。
    This study aimed to examine the effects of a visual praxis-based occupational therapy (VPOT) programme on reading and motor skills for children with developmental dyslexia (DD). Forty-two children were included in the study. Additionally, before VPOT, the Reading-Aloud and Reading-Comprehension Test 2 (ORSRC-2) and the Bruininks-Oseretsky Motor-Proficiency-Test-2-Brief Form (BOT2-BF) were applied to the participants. According to the study design, VPOT was applied to two sessions per week for 8 weeks to group A. During this period, group B was accepted as the control group. At the end of these 8 weeks, evaluation tests were applied to both groups. Then, group A was defined as the control group and Group B as the intervention group, and VPOT was applied to Group B. At the end of another 8 weeks, evaluation tests were applied to both groups for the third time. When the final ORSRC-2 results were examined, VPOT was found to be an effective programme for improving reading skills. Additionally, when the final BOT2-BF results were examined, VPOT was determined to be effective in improving motor skills (p < 0.05). We believe that it is important to carry out comprehensive studies such as the VPOT programme to solve problems in the physical and learning activities of children with DD.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    面孔和身体为社交互动和交流提供了关键线索。它们的结构编码依赖于配置处理,如刺激反转对两面的有害影响所暗示的(即,面部反转效应-FIE)和身体(身体反转效应-BIE)。在刺激开始(N170)后约170ms处达到峰值的ocipi-temporal负面事件相关电位(ERP)成分始终由人脸和身体引起,并受到这些刺激反转的影响。尽管已知情绪表达可以增强结构编码(导致情绪的N170分量比中性面孔更大),对身体情感表达知之甚少。因此,本研究结合FIE和BIE研究了不同情绪表达对结构编码的影响。使用128通道脑电图(EEG)记录了三个ERP组件(P1,N170,P2),当参与者被呈现(直立和倒置)面孔和身体传达四种可能的情绪(幸福,悲伤,愤怒,恐惧)或没有情绪(中性)。结果表明,反转和情绪表达独立地影响所有ERP组件(P1,N170,P2)的准确性和幅度。特别是,人脸在结构编码阶段(N170)表现出情绪表达的特定影响,而P2振幅(代表自上而下的概念化)被情绪身体感知改变。此外,参与者执行的任务(即,隐式vs.情感信息的显式处理)对准确性和ERP组件的影响不同。这些结果支持视觉感知的综合理论,因此,有利于社会刺激处理中涉及的两个神经认知途径(一个用于结构编码,一个用于情绪表达分析)的功能独立性。讨论了结果,突出了两个途径之间独立性的神经认知和计算优势。
    Faces and bodies provide critical cues for social interaction and communication. Their structural encoding depends on configural processing, as suggested by the detrimental effect of stimulus inversion for both faces (i.e., face inversion effect - FIE) and bodies (body inversion effect - BIE). An occipito-temporal negative event-related potential (ERP) component peaking around 170 ms after stimulus onset (N170) is consistently elicited by human faces and bodies and is affected by the inversion of these stimuli. Albeit it is known that emotional expressions can boost structural encoding (resulting in larger N170 components for emotional than for neutral faces), little is known about body emotional expressions. Thus, the current study investigated the effects of different emotional expressions on structural encoding in combination with FIE and BIE. Three ERP components (P1, N170, P2) were recorded using a 128-channel electroencephalogram (EEG) when participants were presented with (upright and inverted) faces and bodies conveying four possible emotions (happiness, sadness, anger, fear) or no emotion (neutral). Results demonstrated that inversion and emotional expressions independently affected the Accuracy and amplitude of all ERP components (P1, N170, P2). In particular, faces showed specific effects of emotional expressions during the structural encoding stage (N170), while P2 amplitude (representing top-down conceptualisation) was modified by emotional body perception. Moreover, the task performed by participants (i.e., implicit vs. explicit processing of emotional information) differently influenced Accuracy and ERP components. These results support integrated theories of visual perception, thus speaking in favour of the functional independence of the two neurocognitive pathways (one for structural encoding and one for emotional expression analysis) involved in social stimuli processing. Results are discussed highlighting the neurocognitive and computational advantages of the independence between the two pathways.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:这项多中心研究旨在评估六种颜色测量设备的视觉-仪器一致性,并使用专家观察者的视觉色差数据集优化三个色差方程。
    方法:共有来自5个国家16个地点的154名专家观察员参加了会议,使用幅度估计对26对人造牙齿样本进行视觉缩放。三个色差方程(ΔE*ab,ΔE00和CAM16-UCS)进行了测试。使用特定于设备的权重对所有三个方程进行优化,并使用标准化残差平方和(STRESS)指数来评估视觉-仪器一致性。
    结果:ΔE*ab公式显示的STRESS值为18至40,视觉-仪器一致性在60%至82%之间。ΔE00公式显示应力值从26到32,代表68%到74%的视觉仪器一致性。CAM16-UCS显示应力值在32-39之间,视觉仪器一致性在61-68%之间。优化后,所有三个公式的应力值都降低了,ΔE\'显示平均视觉-仪器一致性为79%,ΔE00为78%。CAM16-UCS在优化后显示出76%的平均视觉-仪器一致性。
    结论:色差方程的优化显着改善了视觉-仪器一致性,使器件性能黯然失色。优化的ΔE'公式显示出最佳的整体性能,结合了计算简单性和出色的视觉工具一致性。
    OBJECTIVE: This multicenter study aimed to evaluate visual-instrumental agreement of six color measurement devices and optimize three color difference equations using a dataset of visual color differences (∆V) from expert observers.
    METHODS: A total of 154 expert observers from 16 sites across 5 countries participated, providing visual scaling on 26 sample pairs of artificial teeth using magnitude estimation. Three color difference equations (ΔE*ab, ∆E00, and CAM16-UCS) were tested. Optimization of all three equations was performed using device-specific weights, and the standardized residual sum of squares (STRESS) index was used to evaluate visual-instrumental agreement.
    RESULTS: The ΔE*ab formula exhibited STRESS values from 18 to 40, with visual-instrumental agreement between 60 % and 82 %. The ∆E00 formula showed STRESS values from 26 to 32, representing visual-instrumental agreement of 68 % to 74 %. CAM16-UCS demonstrated STRESS values from 32 - 39, with visual-instrumental agreement between 61-68 %. Following optimization, STRESS values decreased for all three formulas, with ΔE\' demonstrating average visual-instrumental agreement of 79 % and ∆E00 of 78 %. CAM16-UCS showed average visual-instrumental agreement of 76 % post optimization.
    CONCLUSIONS: Optimization of color difference equations notably improved visual-instrumental agreement, overshadowing device performance. The optimzed ΔE\' formula demonstrated the best overall performance combining computational simplicty with outstanding visual-instrumental agreement.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在动物模型中的发现和发展是其在临床听力学和神经学中实施的基础。在我们的实验室里,在龙猫中使用圆窗ECochG录音可以更好地了解听觉传出功能。在以前的作品中,我们提供了视觉注意力和工作记忆过程中听觉神经和耳蜗反应的皮质调节的证据。然而,在视听交叉模式刺激期间,这些对听觉通路最外围结构的自上而下的认知机制是否也是活跃的,目前尚不清楚.这里,我们引入了一种新技术,无线ECochG记录听觉神经的复合动作电位(CAP),耳蜗微音(CM),在跨模态(视觉和听觉)刺激的范例中,清醒的龙猫中的圆窗噪声(RWN)。我们比较了从无线ECochG系统记录的四只清醒龙猫获得的ECochG数据与六只麻醉动物的有线ECochG记录。尽管使用无线系统进行的ECochG实验的信噪比低于有线记录,它们的质量足以比较清醒交叉模态条件下的ECochG电位。我们发现,与单独的听觉刺激(点击和音调)相比,对视听刺激的CAP和CM幅度没有显着差异。另一方面,自发听觉神经活动(RWN)通过视觉交叉模态刺激进行调节,这表明视觉交叉模态模拟可以调节自发而非诱发的听觉神经活动。然而,鉴于10只动物的有限样本(4只无线和6只有线),这些结果应谨慎解释。需要未来的实验来证实这些结论。此外,我们介绍了在动物模型中使用无线ECochG作为转化研究的有用工具。
    The discovery and development of electrocochleography (ECochG) in animal models has been fundamental for its implementation in clinical audiology and neurotology. In our laboratory, the use of round-window ECochG recordings in chinchillas has allowed a better understanding of auditory efferent functioning. In previous works, we gave evidence of the corticofugal modulation of auditory-nerve and cochlear responses during visual attention and working memory. However, whether these cognitive top-down mechanisms to the most peripheral structures of the auditory pathway are also active during audiovisual crossmodal stimulation is unknown. Here, we introduce a new technique, wireless ECochG to record compound-action potentials of the auditory nerve (CAP), cochlear microphonics (CM), and round-window noise (RWN) in awake chinchillas during a paradigm of crossmodal (visual and auditory) stimulation. We compared ECochG data obtained from four awake chinchillas recorded with a wireless ECochG system with wired ECochG recordings from six anesthetized animals. Although ECochG experiments with the wireless system had a lower signal-to-noise ratio than wired recordings, their quality was sufficient to compare ECochG potentials in awake crossmodal conditions. We found non-significant differences in CAP and CM amplitudes in response to audiovisual stimulation compared to auditory stimulation alone (clicks and tones). On the other hand, spontaneous auditory-nerve activity (RWN) was modulated by visual crossmodal stimulation, suggesting that visual crossmodal simulation can modulate spontaneous but not evoked auditory-nerve activity. However, given the limited sample of 10 animals (4 wireless and 6 wired), these results should be interpreted cautiously. Future experiments are required to substantiate these conclusions. In addition, we introduce the use of wireless ECochG in animal models as a useful tool for translational research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:橡胶手错觉(RHI)是研究身体所有权的一种公认方法:鉴于视觉上的充分一致性,感官,和本体感受刺激,个人的经验是他或她自己的橡胶手。
    目的:研究对橡胶手的威胁的影响。
    方法:我们创建了一个典型的RHI范例,但增加了威胁性的疼痛:用刷子抚摸个人的真实手和橡胶手,同步(RHI诱导条件)或异步(控制条件),但是只有橡皮手被针头刺破,造成了威胁性的疼痛状况。在一组23个典型发育个体中记录事件相关电位(ERP)。使用低分辨率电磁层析成像对ERP影响进行源定位。
    结果:当将笔触同步应用于真实手和橡胶手时,个体在实验期间一致报告了RHI的经验。ERP分析表明,同步刷行程比异步刷行程在100-200ms范围内产生更高的正面ERP幅度,这被解释为反映了RHI。在威胁疼痛的情况下,ERP在额中央电极显示出更大的阳性,源位于辅助运动区(SMA)。
    结论:SMA激活可以反映对反身运动活动的控制机制,促进可能的威胁相关响应。进一步的研究应以相关的方式解决ERP的影响以及RHI对标准和威胁刺激的程度,以进一步阐明神经生理学发现的功能意义。
    BACKGROUND: The rubber hand illusion (RHI) is a well-established method for studying body ownership: Given adequate concordance of visual, sensory, and proprioceptive stimuli, the individual experiences a rubber hand as his or her own.
    OBJECTIVE: To study the effects of a threat to the rubber hand.
    METHODS: We created a typical RHI paradigm but added threatening pain: Both the real hand of an individual and the rubber hand were stroked with a brush, either synchronously (RHI-inducing condition) or asynchronously (control condition), but only the rubber hand was then pricked with a needle to create a threatening pain condition. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded in a group of 23 typically-developed individuals. ERP effects were source-localized using low-resolution electromagnetic tomography.
    RESULTS: The individuals consistently reported experience of the RHI during the experiment when the brush strokes were applied synchronously to both the real hand and the rubber hand. ERP analysis revealed that synchronous brush stroking gave rise to higher amplitude of frontal ERPs in the 100-200 ms range than asynchronous brush stroking, which was interpreted as reflecting the RHI. In the threatening pain condition, ERPs showed a greater positivity at frontocentral electrodes, source localized in the supplementary motor area (SMA).
    CONCLUSIONS: SMA activation could reflect a control mechanism over reflexive motor activity, facilitating a possible threat-related response. Further studies should address ERP effects and the extent of the RHI to standard and threat stimuli in a correlative fashion to further elucidate the functional significance of the neurophysiological findings.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:具有不可逆视野丧失的青光眼患者经常经历生活质量下降,行动不便,和心理健康挑战。感知学习(PL)和经颅电刺激(tES)已成为视力康复的有希望的干预措施,显示出恢复剩余视觉功能的潜力。使用ElectricAI经颅刺激(GREAT)的青光眼康复项目旨在研究结合PL和tES是否比单独使用两种方法更有效地最大化青光眼患者的视觉功能。此外,这项研究将评估这些干预措施对脑神经活动的影响,血液生物标志物,移动性,心理健康,生活质量,害怕跌倒。
    方法:该研究采用了三臂,双盲,随机化,优势控制设计。参与者以1:1:1的比例随机分配到以下三组中的一组:(1)真实PL和真实tES,(2)真实PL和假TES,和(3)安慰剂PL和假tES。每个参与者每个区块经历10个会话(每个约1小时),总共三个街区。在六个时间点进行评估:基线,临时1,临时2,干预后,干预后1个月,干预后2个月。主要结果是汉弗莱视野分析仪测量的24-2视野的平均偏差。次要结果包括阈值上视野的检出率,平衡和步态功能,以及电生理和生物反应。这项研究还调查了神经递质代谢的变化,生物标志物,自我感知的生活质量,干预前后的心理状况。
    结论:GREAT项目是第一个评估PL和tES在青光眼康复中的有效性的研究。我们的研究结果将全面评估这些治疗方法对广泛的大脑和视觉相关指标的影响,包括视野,神经活动,生物标志物,移动性,心理健康,害怕跌倒,和生活质量。
    背景:ClinicalTrials.govNCT05874258。2023年5月15日注册。
    BACKGROUND: Glaucoma patients with irreversible visual field loss often experience decreased quality of life, impaired mobility, and mental health challenges. Perceptual learning (PL) and transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) have emerged as promising interventions for vision rehabilitation, showing potential in restoring residual visual functions. The Glaucoma Rehabilitation using ElectricAI Transcranial stimulation (GREAT) project aims to investigate whether combining PL and tES is more effective than using either method alone in maximizing the visual function of glaucoma patients. Additionally, the study will assess the impact of these interventions on brain neural activity, blood biomarkers, mobility, mental health, quality of life, and fear of falling.
    METHODS: The study employs a three-arm, double-blind, randomized, superiority-controlled design. Participants are randomly allocated in a 1:1:1 ratio to one of three groups receiving: (1) real PL and real tES, (2) real PL and sham tES, and (3) placebo PL and sham tES. Each participant undergoes 10 sessions per block (~ 1 h each), with a total of three blocks. Assessments are conducted at six time points: baseline, interim 1, interim 2, post-intervention, 1-month post-intervention, and 2-month post-intervention. The primary outcome is the mean deviation of the 24-2 visual field measured by the Humphrey visual field analyzer. Secondary outcomes include detection rate in the suprathreshold visual field, balance and gait functions, and electrophysiological and biological responses. This study also investigates changes in neurotransmitter metabolism, biomarkers, self-perceived quality of life, and psychological status before and after the intervention.
    CONCLUSIONS: The GREAT project is the first study to assess the effectiveness of PL and tES in the rehabilitation of glaucoma. Our findings will offer comprehensive assessments of the impact of these treatments on a wide range of brain and vision-related metrics including visual field, neural activity, biomarkers, mobility, mental health, fear of falling, and quality of life.
    BACKGROUND: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT05874258 . Registered on May 15, 2023.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    注意力失误(AL)是反应能力的完全失误,其中表现短暂但完全中断,与微睡眠相反,眼睛保持开放。尽管已经通过行为和生理手段研究了ALs的现象,AL的根本原因在很大程度上仍然难以捉摸。本研究旨在研究在连续的视觉运动任务中行为识别的内源性AL的潜在生理底物,主要是为了回答这个问题:在这项任务中,AL是由于极端的思维游荡还是思维空白?得出来自40名健康非睡眠不足受试者的数据(20M/20F;平均年龄27.1岁,20-45).由于每个受试者需要最少两个AL,因此在分析中仅使用40个受试者中的17个。在研究1和2中,受试者分别进行了50和20分钟的随机2-D连续视觉运动跟踪任务。跟踪性能,眼睛视频,同时记录功能磁共振成像(fMRI)。人类专家目视检查了跟踪性能和眼睛视频记录,以识别并将响应性的失效分类为微睡眠或AL。通过全脑体素fMRI和来自七个关键网络的感兴趣区域(ROI)的血液动力学响应(HR)分析来估计85个AL(17位受试者)相对于响应性跟踪的神经活动变化。还估计了关键ROI内部和之间的功能连通性(FC)的变化。探索的网络是默认模式网络,背侧注意网络,额顶叶网络,感觉运动网络,显著性网络,视觉网络,和工作记忆网络。体素分析显示,重叠的背侧前扣带皮质和辅助运动区区域的血氧水平依赖性活动显着增加,但活动没有显着减少;活动的增加被认为代表了AL后反应性的恢复过程。在相应ROI的HR中也观察到这种增加的活性。重要的是,HR分析显示,默认模式网络的后扣带活动没有增加的趋势,它已被反复证明是思维游荡的强大生物标志物。FC分析显示外部注意力脱钩,这支持了AL的非自愿性质,除了神经恢复过程。其他发现是在ALs发作之前额叶网络中的HR降低,以及默认模式网络和工作内存网络之间的FC减少。这些发现与我们的结论一致,即在我们的任务中观察到的AL是非自愿的思维空白。这在行为上进一步得到了AL的短持续时间(平均1.7s)的支持,这被认为太简短了,不可能是极端走神的例子。这是第一项研究证明,至少在连续的视觉运动任务中,大部分完全丧失了响应能力,如果不是由于微睡眠,由于非自愿的精神空白。
    Attention lapses (ALs) are complete lapses of responsiveness in which performance is briefly but completely disrupted and during which, as opposed to microsleeps, the eyes remain open. Although the phenomenon of ALs has been investigated by behavioural and physiological means, the underlying cause of an AL has largely remained elusive. This study aimed to investigate the underlying physiological substrates of behaviourally identified endogenous ALs during a continuous visuomotor task, primarily to answer the question: Were the ALs during this task due to extreme mind-wandering or mind-blanks? The data from two studies were combined, resulting in data from 40 healthy non-sleep-deprived subjects (20M/20F; mean age 27.1 years, 20-45). Only 17 of the 40 subjects were used in the analysis due to a need for a minimum of two ALs per subject. Subjects performed a random 2-D continuous visuomotor tracking task for 50 and 20 min in Studies 1 and 2, respectively. Tracking performance, eye-video, and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) were recorded simultaneously. A human expert visually inspected the tracking performance and eye-video recordings to identify and categorise lapses of responsiveness as microsleeps or ALs. Changes in neural activity during 85 ALs (17 subjects) relative to responsive tracking were estimated by whole-brain voxel-wise fMRI and by haemodynamic response (HR) analysis in regions of interest (ROIs) from seven key networks to reveal the neural signature of ALs. Changes in functional connectivity (FC) within and between the key ROIs were also estimated. Networks explored were the default mode network, dorsal attention network, frontoparietal network, sensorimotor network, salience network, visual network, and working memory network. Voxel-wise analysis revealed a significant increase in blood-oxygen-level-dependent activity in the overlapping dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and supplementary motor area region but no significant decreases in activity; the increased activity is considered to represent a recovery-of-responsiveness process following an AL. This increased activity was also seen in the HR of the corresponding ROI. Importantly, HR analysis revealed no trend of increased activity in the posterior cingulate of the default mode network, which has been repeatedly demonstrated to be a strong biomarker of mind-wandering. FC analysis showed decoupling of external attention, which supports the involuntary nature of ALs, in addition to the neural recovery processes. Other findings were a decrease in HR in the frontoparietal network before the onset of ALs, and a decrease in FC between default mode network and working memory network. These findings converge to our conclusion that the ALs observed during our task were involuntary mind-blanks. This is further supported behaviourally by the short duration of the ALs (mean 1.7 s), which is considered too brief to be instances of extreme mind-wandering. This is the first study to demonstrate that at least the majority of complete losses of responsiveness on a continuous visuomotor task are, if not due to microsleeps, due to involuntary mind-blanks.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    谵妄的视觉注意缺陷特征不佳。研究强调了视觉处理流中的神经解剖学异常,但未能在功能水平上量化这些异常。为了识别这些缺陷,我们进行了一项多中心眼动追踪研究,我们使用一种新型眼动追踪系统记录了来自42名患者的210个疗程,该系统是专门为在(ICU)中自由观看而设计的;每个疗程持续10分钟,并使用ICU中的混淆评估方法(CAM-ICU)标记患者谵妄状态.为了分析这些数据,我们将视觉注意力的任务表述为分层生成过程,该过程产生下一个固定位置的概率分布。然后可以将该分布与所测量的患者固定进行比较,从而产生正确性评分,该正确性评分在谵妄状态之间进行比较。该分析表明,谵妄患者的视觉处理系统在功能上受到统计学上的限制。这是第一项探索谵妄中视觉注意力不集中的潜在机制的研究,并提出了未来研究疾病过程的新目标,该过程会影响四分之一的住院患者,并产生严重的短期和长期后果。
    The visual attentional deficits in delirium are poorly characterized. Studies have highlighted neuro-anatomical abnormalities in the visual processing stream but fail at quantifying these abnormalities at a functional level. To identify these deficits, we undertook a multi-center eye-tracking study where we recorded 210 sessions from 42 patients using a novel eye-tracking system that was made specifically for free-viewing in the (ICU); each session lasted 10 min and was labeled with the delirium status of the patient using the Confusion Assessment Method in ICU (CAM-ICU). To analyze this data, we formulate the task of visual attention as a hierarchical generative process that yields a probabilistic distribution of the location of the next fixation. This distribution can then be compared to the measured patient fixation producing a correctness score which is tallied compared across delirium status. This analysis demonstrated that the visual processing system of patients suffering from delirium is functionally restricted to a statistically significant degree. This is the first study to explore the potential mechanisms underpinning visual inattention in delirium and suggests a new target of future research into a disease process that affects one in four hospitalized patients with severe short and long-term consequences.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号