word learning

单词学习
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人们普遍认为,言语感知和言语产生是紧密相连的,每种模式都可以帮助学习其他模式。这种积极的关系通常被概括为感知和生产是“同一枚硬币的两面”。\"有,的确,许多情况表明了这种相互支持的关系。然而,越来越多的研究显示出非常不同的结果,在模式独立运行的情况下,甚至是彼此对立。我们回顾了现在相当大的文献,这些文献证明了语音产生对语音感知学习的负面影响,在多个级别(特别是在词法和副词法级别)。通过将显示这种模式的情况与发生更多积极互动的情况进行比较,我们提供了为什么发现不同结果的初步说明,确定导致生产对感知的正面或负面影响的因素。审查澄清了这两种模式之间存在的复杂关系:它们确实是联系在一起的,但是他们的关系比他们是同一枚硬币的两面所暗示的要复杂得多。
    A widely held belief is that speech perception and speech production are tightly linked, with each modality available to help with learning in the other modality. This positive relationship is often summarized as perception and production being \"two sides of the same coin.\" There are, indeed, many situations that have shown this mutually supportive relationship. However, there is a growing body of research showing very different results, with the modalities operating independently, or even in opposition to each other. We review the now-sizeable literature demonstrating the negative effect that speech production can have on perceptual learning of speech, at multiple levels (particularly at the lexical and sublexical levels). By comparing the situations that show this pattern with ones in which more positive interactions occur, we provide an initial account of why the different outcomes are found, identifying factors that lead to either positive or negative effects of production on perception. The review clarifies the complex relationship that exists between the two modalities: They are indeed linked, but their relationship is more complicated than is suggested by the notion that they are two sides of the same coin.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目前的证据表明,患有发育性语言障碍(DLD)的儿童在单词学习活动中受益于间隔检索。相对于使用替代单词学习程序的回忆,单词回忆是相当好的。然而,绝对召回可以进一步提高;许多研究报告说,只有不到三分之二的单词是学习的,即使在学习活动期间有间隔检索的帮助。在本文中,我们确定了间隔检索的详细信息,这些详细信息不太了解,以通过检索实践促进更有效的学习。
    我们讨论了一些因素的重要性,例如:(a)将即时检索与间隔检索试验相结合;(b)确定相对于相等的间距,逐渐增加间距是否具有短期收益;(c)发现成功检索的数量足以确保以后的回忆;(d)使用间隔检索以避免在以后的回忆测试中语音细节受到侵蚀;(e)记录良好的儿童与特定单词形式学习困难的DLD我们还推测了一些可能的原因,为什么间隔检索首先是有益的。
    尽管许多患有DLD的儿童通过包含间隔检索的程序在单词学习中取得了收益,这个过程中有许多细节可以改变它的成功。在我们更好地理解间隔检索有效性的界限之前,我们不会充分利用这个有希望的单词学习程序。
    间隔的检索活动可能是对临床医生和教育工作者可用来帮助儿童学习单词的资源的重要补充。随着对这里讨论的问题有了更深入的了解,我们应该能够更多地使用间隔检索。
    UNASSIGNED: Current evidence shows that children with developmental language disorder (DLD) benefit from spaced retrieval during word learning activities. Word recall is quite good relative to recall with alternative word learning procedures. However, recall on an absolute basis can be improved further; many studies report that fewer than two-thirds of the words are learned, even with the assistance of spaced retrieval during the learning activities. In this article we identify details of spaced retrieval that are less well understood in an effort to promote more effective learning through retrieval practice.
    UNASSIGNED: We discuss the importance of factors such as: (a) integrating immediate retrieval with spaced retrieval trials; (b) determining whether gradual increases in spacing have more than short-term benefits relative to equal spacing; (c) discovering the number of successful retrievals sufficient to ensure later recall; (d) using spaced retrieval to avoid erosion of phonetic details on later recall tests; and (e) whether the well-documented difficulties with learning word forms might be tied to a particular subgroup of children with DLD. We also speculate on some of the possible reasons why spaced retrieval is beneficial in the first place.
    UNASSIGNED: Although many children with DLD make gains in word learning through procedures that incorporate spaced retrieval, there are numerous details involved in the process that can alter its success. Until we have a better understanding of the boundaries of spaced retrieval\'s effectiveness, we will not be taking full advantage of this promising addition to word learning procedures.
    UNASSIGNED: Spaced retrieval activities can be an important addition to the resources that clinicians and educators have available to assist children in their word learning. With a deeper understanding of the issues discussed here, we should be able to put spaced retrieval to even greater use.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    许多失聪和听力困难(DHH)的儿童比典型听力(TH)的儿童表现出较差的听觉单词学习能力。这项研究的目的是测试检索练习对听力困难(HH)儿童单词学习的影响。
    26名TH儿童和16名HH儿童完成了言语感知和词汇测试,和一个快速的单词学习游戏,在没有检索的情况下学习新单词,立即检索,和间隔的检索条件。
    线性混合效应模型表明,学习条件和时间预测了单词识别中48%的方差,年龄预测了15%的单词回忆方差。我们观察到TH儿童和HH儿童在单词识别和回忆方面没有差异。儿童在即时和间隔检索练习条件下学习和记住的单词比在无检索条件下更多。
    检索实践对于单词学习和保留比被动暴露(不检索)更有效。DHH儿童的临床和教育实践可以结合使用有针对性的暴露和自然方法检索新词的明确指导。未来的研究应该评估现实生活干预中的检索实践,以告知临床和教育实践。
    UNASSIGNED: Many children who are deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) show poorer auditory word-learning skills than children with typical hearing (TH). The goal of this study was to test the effect of retrieval practice on word learning in children who are hard of hearing (HH).
    UNASSIGNED: Twenty-six TH children and 16 children who are HH completed a speech perception and a vocabulary test, and a rapid word-learning game to learn new words under no retrieval, immediate retrieval, and spaced retrieval conditions.
    UNASSIGNED: Linear mixed-effect models revealed that learning condition and time predicted 48% of the variance in word recognition, and age predicted 15% of word recall variance. We observed no differences in word recognition and recall between TH children and children who are HH. Children learned and remembered more words in the immediate and spaced retrieval practice conditions than in the no retrieval condition.
    UNASSIGNED: Retrieval practice is more effective for word learning and retention than passive exposure (no retrieval). Clinical and educational practices for children who are DHH could combine explicit instruction using targeted exposures and retrieval of new words with naturalistic approaches. Future studies should assess retrieval practice in real-life interventions to inform clinical and educational practices.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    儿童在语言学习过程中听到的单词频率分布是高度偏斜的(Zipfian)。先前的研究表明,这种偏斜的环境在需要学习者发现必须学习的单元的任务中赋予了可学习性优势,如在分词或跨情境学习中。这种促进作用已归因于学习较低频率项目中高频项目的上下文促进,并在偏斜分布的增加的可预测性(较低的熵)下更好地学习。这里,我们问Zipfian分布是否除了发现单位之外还能促进学习,正如在可预测性账户下预期的那样。我们在一项学习任务中测试了儿童对新颖单词引用映射的学习,其中每个映射在训练期间都是孤立地呈现的,并且不需要被证明。我们将统一环境中的学习与具有不同熵水平的两个偏斜环境进行了比较。儿童的学习在两个倾斜的环境中总体上更好,即使是低频项目。这些结果将Zipfian分布的促进作用扩展到其他学习任务,并表明它们可以在发现单元之外促进语言学习。
    The word-frequency distributions children hear during language learning are highly skewed (Zipfian). Previous studies suggest that such skewed environments confer a learnability advantage in tasks that require the learner to discover the units that have to be learned, as in word-segmentation or cross-situational learning. This facilitative effect has been attributed to contextual facilitation from high frequency items in learning lower frequency items, and to better learning under the increased predictability (lower entropy) of skewed distributions. Here, we ask whether Zipfian distributions facilitate learning beyond the discovery of units, as expected under the predictability account. We tested children\'s learning of novel word-referent mappings in a learning task where each mapping was presented in isolation during training, and did not need to be dicovered. We compared learning in a uniform environment to two skewed environments with different entropy levels. Children\'s learning was overall better in the two skewed environments, even for low frequency items. These results extend the facilitative effect of Zipfian distributions to additional learning tasks and show they can facilitate language learning beyond the discovery of units.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    准确地捕捉孩子的单词学习能力对于提高我们对语言发展的理解至关重要。研究人员已经证明,利用更复杂的统计方法,如混合效应回归和分层线性建模,可以更全面地理解儿童单词学习能力中观察到的变异性。在当前的论文中,我们展示了以人为中心的数据分析方法如何为儿童单词学习能力的异质性提供更多见解,同时也帮助研究人员努力得出个人层面的结论。使用先前发布的数据,其中32名通常发育中的婴儿和32名后期说话的婴儿完成了新名词概括(NNG)任务,以评估单词学习偏差(即,形状和材料偏差),我们将这种以人为本的方法与三种传统的统计方法进行了比较:(1)对机会的t检验,(2)方差分析(ANOVA),(3)混合效应回归。每次比较,我们提出了一个以人为中心的方法提出的新问题,并展示了相应分析的结果如何在我们对儿童单词学习能力的理解上带来更大的细微差别。以人为本的方法,然后,被证明是有价值的工具,应该添加到现代研究人员使用的越来越复杂的统计程序中。
    Accurately capturing children\'s word learning abilities is critical for advancing our understanding of language development. Researchers have demonstrated that utilizing more complex statistical methods, such as mixed-effects regression and hierarchical linear modeling, can lead to a more complete understanding of the variability observed within children\'s word learning abilities. In the current paper, we demonstrate how a person-centered approach to data analysis can provide additional insights into the heterogeneity of word learning ability among children while also aiding researchers\' efforts to draw individual-level conclusions. Using previously published data with 32 typically developing and 32 late-talking infants who completed a novel noun generalization (NNG) task to assess word learning biases (i.e., shape and material biases), we compare this person-centered method to three traditional statistical approaches: (1) a t-test against chance, (2) an analysis of variance (ANOVA), and (3) a mixed-effects regression. With each comparison, we present a novel question raised by the person-centered approach and show how results from the corresponding analyses can lead to greater nuance in our understanding of children\'s word learning capabilities. Person-centered methods, then, are shown to be valuable tools that should be added to the growing body of sophisticated statistical procedures used by modern researchers.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对象和地点是人类符号表达式中表示的基本空间域,像图纸,它显示了在对象所在的大规模可导航位置信息上描绘小规模对象形状信息的优先级。语言中是否存在类似的对象对位偏差?在六个实验中,成年人和3至4岁的儿童被要求在标签短语中扩展一个新名词,在介词短语中扩展一个新颖的名词,或简单地匹配图片。为了将特定对象和位置信息与更一般的图形和地面信息分离,参与者要么看到既有地点信息(房间)又有物体信息(房间里的一个街区)的场景,或具有两种对象信息的场景,通过呈现内部具有较小的块的开放容器来匹配房间和块的图形-地面关系。虽然成年人在扩展新名词标签和匹配方面都表现出特定的对象过度偏见,他们在介词后扩展新名词时没有表现出这种偏见。幼儿仅在扩展新名词标签时表现出这种偏见。因此,空间域可能会赋予单词学习的特定和基础偏见,这些偏见可能会随着发展而改变,其方式类似于其他关于对象的单词学习偏见,比如形状偏差。这些结果将先前关于绘图中的对象偏差的研究的符号范围扩展到了语言中的对象偏差,它们扩展了先前研究的空间领域,这些研究表征了对象和地方的语言。
    Objects and places are foundational spatial domains represented in human symbolic expressions, like drawings, which show a prioritization of depicting small-scale object-shape information over the large-scale navigable place information in which objects are situated. Is there a similar object-over-place bias in language? Across six experiments, adults and 3- to 4-year-old children were asked either to extend a novel noun in a labeling phrase, to extend a novel noun in a prepositional phrase, or to simply match pictures. To dissociate specific object and place information from more general figure and ground information, participants either saw scenes with both place information (a room) and object information (a block in the room), or scenes with two kinds of object information that matched the figure-ground relations of the room and block by presenting an open container with a smaller block inside. While adults showed a specific object-over-place bias in both extending novel noun labels and matching, they did not show this bias in extending novel nouns following prepositions. Young children showed this bias in extending novel noun labels only. Spatial domains may thus confer specific and foundational biases for word learning that may change through development in a way that is similar to that of other word-learning biases about objects, like the shape bias. These results expand the symbolic scope of prior studies on object biases in drawing to object biases in language, and they expand the spatial domains of prior studies characterizing the language of objects and places.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    无元音词在类型学上非常罕见,尽管它们存在于某些语言中,例如Tashlhiyt(例如,fkt\'给它\')。当前的研究测试了成年英语使用者从短暂的听觉暴露中获得包含三段(CCC)无元音单词的词典是否比不包含无元音单词的词典更困难。还探索了声学语音形式在学习这些类型学上罕见的单词形式中的作用:在实验1中,对参与者进行了仅在清晰语音或随意语音中产生的单词的培训;实验2对参与者进行了两种语音风格产生的词汇项目的培训。当说话风格与参与者一致时,听众能够很好地学习无元音和元音词典,但是当训练由可变的声学语音形式组成时,无元音词典的学习较低。在这两个实验中,对包含新颖项目的训练后单词相似度评级任务的响应表明,接触无元音词典会导致参与者接受新的无元音单词作为可接受的词汇形式。这些结果表明,在类型上最稀有的词汇形式之一-没有元音的单词-可以被幼稚的成年听众迅速获得。然而,声学语音变异调节学习。
    Vowelless words are exceptionally typologically rare, though they are found in some languages, such as Tashlhiyt (e.g., fkt \'give it\'). The current study tests whether lexicons containing tri-segmental (CCC) vowelless words are more difficult to acquire than lexicons not containing vowelless words by adult English speakers from brief auditory exposure. The role of acoustic-phonetic form on learning these typologically rare word forms is also explored: In Experiment 1, participants were trained on words produced in either only Clear speech or Casual speech productions of words; Experiment 2 trained participants on lexical items produced in both speech styles. Listeners were able to learn both vowelless and voweled lexicons equally well when speaking style was consistent for participants, but learning was lower for vowelless lexicons when training consisted of variable acoustic-phonetic forms. In both experiments, responses to a post-training wordlikeness ratings task containing novel items revealed that exposure to a vowelless lexicon leads participants to accept new vowelless words as acceptable lexical forms. These results demonstrate that one of the typologically rarest types of lexical forms - words without vowels - can be rapidly acquired by naive adult listeners. Yet, acoustic-phonetic variation modulates learning.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们成年后每天学习的大多数新单词在形态上都很复杂;然而,我们对形态学在小说单词学习中的作用了解不多。在这项研究中,我们通过比较以下方面的学习来解决这个问题:(i)后缀新颖的单词(例如轻浮);(ii)以非形态结尾的新颖单词,但频繁的字母块(例如fliban);和(iii)具有非形态的新颖单词,低频结局(如:flibov)。单词是通过句子阅读偶然学到的,同时监测参与者的眼球运动。我们表明,与其他两种类型的新颖词相比,形态学具有促进作用,在学习期间和学习后识别记忆任务中。我们还表明,参与者将含义归因于单词部分(如果轻浮是一种幸福状态,那么flb必须意味着快乐),但是这个过程并不是由后缀的存在特别触发的(在fliban和flybov中,flb也必须意味着快乐),因此表明大脑倾向于对相似的单词和单词部分假设相似的含义。
    The majority of the new words that we learn every day as adults are morphologically complex; yet, we do not know much about the role of morphology in novel word learning. In this study, we tackle this issue by comparing the learning of: (i) suffixed novel words (e.g. flibness); (ii) novel words that end in non-morphological, but frequent letter chunks (e.g. fliban); and (iii) novel words with non-morphological, low-frequency endings (e.g. flibov). Words are learned incidentally through sentence reading, while the participants\' eye movements are monitored. We show that morphology has a facilitatory role compared with the other two types of novel words, both during learning and in a post-learning recognition memory task. We also showed that participants attributed meaning to word parts (if flibness is a state of happiness, then flib must mean happy), but this process was not specifically triggered by the presence of a suffix (flib must also mean happy in fliban and flibov), thus suggesting that the brain tends to assume similar meanings for similar words and word parts.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    婴儿与社交伙伴的互动是丰富的多模式。Dyads回应并协调他们的视觉注意力,手势,发声,演讲,手动操作,和物体的操纵。虽然婴儿通常被描述为活跃的学习者,以前的实验研究通常集中在婴儿如何从研究人员精心制作的刺激中学习。最近研究自然主义的研究,自由流动的互动探索了与语言学习有关的二元行为的有意义的模式。婴儿手动参与和探索物体支持他们的视觉注意力,创造物体的显著和多样化的视图,并引出父母的标签话语。在这一章中,我们讨论了婴儿多模式注意力产生的级联行为如何在塑造他们的学习环境中发挥基本作用,支持实时单词学习和预测以后的词汇量。我们从最近的家庭和跨文化研究中得出,以测试我们的机械途径的有效性,并讨论为什么手对学习如此重要。我们的目标是传达发展科学家研究自然行为并超越我们的“久经考验”范式的迫切需要,像基于屏幕的任务。通过研究自然行为,婴儿的手在早期语言学习中的作用被揭示-尽管这是一种通常未编码的行为,未讨论,甚至在过去几十年的研究中都不允许。当我们在自然环境中研究婴儿时,他们可以向我们展示他们如何了解和探索他们的世界。单词学习是动手的。
    Infants\' interactions with social partners are richly multimodal. Dyads respond to and coordinate their visual attention, gestures, vocalizations, speech, manual actions, and manipulations of objects. Although infants are typically described as active learners, previous experimental research has often focused on how infants learn from stimuli that is well-crafted by researchers. Recent research studying naturalistic, free-flowing interactions has explored the meaningful patterns in dyadic behavior that relate to language learning. Infants\' manual engagement and exploration of objects supports their visual attention, creates salient and diverse views of objects, and elicits labeling utterances from parents. In this chapter, we discuss how the cascade of behaviors created by infant multimodal attention plays a fundamental role in shaping their learning environment, supporting real-time word learning and predicting later vocabulary size. We draw from recent at-home and cross-cultural research to test the validity of our mechanistic pathway and discuss why hands matter so much for learning. Our goal is to convey the critical need for developmental scientists to study natural behavior and move beyond our \"tried-and-true\" paradigms, like screen-based tasks. By studying natural behavior, the role of infants\' hands in early language learning was revealed-though it was a behavior that was often uncoded, undiscussed, or not even allowed in decades of previous research. When we study infants in their natural environment, they can show us how they learn about and explore their world. Word learning is hands-on.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:越来越多的证据表明,COVID-19带来了立即影响早期语言发展的变化。鲜为人知的是,这些与COVID-19相关的变化影响了发展的时间。当前的研究系统和纵向地解决了这个问题,在最初的2.5年中跟踪儿童的语言发展。
    方法:本研究对Frota等人的样品进行了跟进。(2022),这表明,在大流行期间出生和抚养的7-9个月大的婴儿不会像大流行前的同龄人那样划分单词。进行了四项研究:(1)12个月大的分词任务(Npregiume=15);(2)20个月大的单词学习任务(Npregiume=20);(3)30个月大的语言和交流发展,通过CDI和CSBS父母报告(Npredima范围为25-74);(4)使用Griffiths发育量表(Npredima=16)在30月龄时的总体发育。
    结果:与大流行前的数据相比,在所有四项研究中,大流行样本的表现始终不佳。没有证据表明在12个月大时有发达的分词能力,在20个月大的时候没有成功的单词学习。12至24月龄之间的词汇发育低于大流行前样本,而社会交往似乎没有受到影响。在30个月大的时候,大流行样本在语言和交流格里菲斯子量表上显示较低的分数和较低的心理年龄,与大流行前的数据相比。
    结论:大流行期间出生和抚养的婴儿语言发育较差,这种情况至少持续到30个月大。
    BACKGROUND: There is growing evidence that COVID-19 brought changes that immediately affected early language development. Little is known for how long these COVID-19 related changes shaped development. The current study systematically and longitudinally addressed this issue, following up children\'s language development throughout the first 2.5 years.
    METHODS: The present study follows up on the sample from Frota et al. (2022), which demonstrated that 7-9-month-old infants born and raised during the pandemic do not segment words unlike pre-pandemic peers. Four studies were conducted: (1) word segmentation task at 12 months of age (Npandemic = 15); (2) word learning task at 20 months of age (Npandemic = 20); (3) language and communication development up to 30 months of age, via CDI and CSBS parental reports (Npandemic ranged 25-74); (4) overall development at 30 months of age using the Griffiths Developmental Scales (Npandemic = 16).
    RESULTS: The pandemic sample consistently underperformed in all four studies in comparison to pre-pandemic data. There was no evidence of developed word segmentation abilities at 12 months of age, and no successful word learning at 20 months of age. Lexical development between 12 and 24 months of age was lower than in the pre-pandemic sample, while social communication did not seem to be affected. At 30 months of age, the pandemic sample showed lower scores and lower mental age on the Language and Communication Griffiths\' subscale, in comparison to the pre-pandemic data.
    CONCLUSIONS: Infants born and raised during the pandemic have a poorer language development, that persists at least until 30 months of age.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号