sociogenesis

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在本文中,我努力解决为什么我们,有时,体验自己不自由。在这样做的过程中,我概述了一个粗略的代理理论(以及我们对自己自由的经验),这是一个在不可逆转的时间中发生的动态过程。在试图回答这个问题时,我把代理定义为追求我们欲望的能力,我声称,只要我们能做到这一点,我们就会体验到自己是自由的——需要注意的是,推理能力是一个必要的标准。我证明代理是一种社会文化发展,表现为推理能力在婴儿期和成年期通过社交互动逐渐发展。至关重要的是,我指出,理性是一把双刃剑:它使我们能够质疑我们的行为和欲望,以及它们是否值得追求,这就是把我们提升为真正的生物的原因。然而,它也让我们疏远自己的行为和欲望,从而剥夺了我们的自由体验。最后,我展示了我们的主观自由是如何在一个不断的过程中失去和获得的,由与我们自己相关的反身产生的。当我们行动时,我们不断遇到限制(身体和心理),使我们无法根据自己的欲望行事。这迫使我们反思我们的行为和愿望,所以,我们对自由的感觉消失了.然而,通过回顾性的遗忘,或者重建,我们遇到的限制,我们可能会重获自由的经验。
    In this paper, I grapple with the question of why we, at times, experience ourselves as not free. In doing so I outline a crude theory of agency (and our experience of ourselves as free) as a dynamic process happening in irreversible time. In attempting to answer this question, I define agency as the ability to pursue our desires, and I claim that we experience ourselves as free as long as we can do this - with the caveat that the ability to reason is a necessary criterion. I show that agency is a sociocultural development that manifests as the ability to reason gradually develops through social interaction during infancy and into adulthood. Crucially, I point out that reason is a double-edged sword: It allows us to question our actions and desires and whether they are worth pursuing, which is what elevates us to agentic beings. However, it also allows us to alienate ourselves from our actions and desires, and thus rob ourselves of our experience of freedom. Lastly, I show how our subjective freedom is lost and gained in a constant process, generated by a reflexive-relating-to ourselves. As we act, we continually encounter constraints (physical and psychological) that bar us from acting upon our desires. This compels us to reflect on our actions and desires, and so, our feeling of freedom evaporates. However, through a retrospective forgetting, or reconstruction, of the constraints we encounter, we may regain our experience of being free.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: English Abstract
    由于现代社会的障碍,阻碍了在成长的不同阶段最大限度地揭示他们的潜力和能力,残疾儿童的自我实现问题是现实的。尽管现代研究人员对自我实现的问题很感兴趣,这个科学类别实际上没有被调查过与残疾人相关的个人,包括孩子,这不允许以高效率确保这一类公民的包容性生活方式。这项研究的目的是确定残疾儿童在家庭条件下社会发生的各个时期自我实现的细节,作为发生包容性过程的第一个社会环境。在那,自我实现被认为是决定持续性健康障碍儿童包容性生活方式形成的社会因素之一。理论分析允许建立自我实现等类别之间的关系,残疾儿童的生活方式和社会包容。在经验层面,对摩尔曼斯克州父母(n=292)和残疾儿童(n=292)的半形式化访谈结果的分析,揭示了家庭中残疾儿童自我实现的特点。确定了在家庭环境中阻止这种自我实现和抑制包容性过程的社会障碍。研究结果可以为在不同的社会发生阶段设计和实施残疾儿童的可变包容性社会实践奠定基础,扩大他们自我实现的可能性,考虑个性社会化的需要。
    The problem of self-actualization of disabled children is actual because of barriers in modern society that prevent maximal possible disclosure of their potential and abilities at different stages of growing up. Despite high interest of modern researchers to problematic of self-actualization, this scientific category practically was not investigated in relation to individuals with disabilities, including children, that does not allow to ensure with high degree of efficiency inclusive life-style for this category of citizens. The purpose of the study is to identify specifics of self-actualization of disabled children in various periods of their sociogenesis in family conditions as first social environment of occurring inclusive processes. At that, self-actualization is considered as one of social factors determining formation of inclusive lifestyle of child with persistent health disorders. The theoretical analysis permitted to establish relationship between such categories as self-actualization, lifestyle and social inclusion of disabled children. At the empirical level, the analysis of results of semi-formalized interviews of parents (n=292) and disabled children (n=292) in the Murmansk Oblast, revealed characteristics of self-actualization of disabled children in the family. The social barriers preventing this self-actualization in family environment and inhibiting inclusive processes were identified. The results of the study can be laid in the foundation of designing and implementing variable inclusive social practices of disabled children with disabilities at different stages of sociogenesis, and expanding possibilities for their self-realization, considering needs of socializing personality.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对于许多需要分布式多代理系统的现实应用程序来说,在无限空间中保持随机移动代理之间的内聚力是一项必不可少的功能。我们在一维无限空间中开发了生物启发的集体运动模型,以确保这种功能。使用内部代理信念来估计系统的中观状态,代理运动耦合到动态自生成的社会排名变量。社会信息和个体运动之间的这种耦合被用来诱导空间自我分类,并产生自适应,在无限空间中稳定随机运动的群相对坐标系。我们根据模型的关键控制参数研究了模型的状态空间,并为系统找到了两个独立的状态,以达到动态内聚状态,包括部分感测机制,其中系统自选择最近邻距离以便确保感测到的邻居的接近恒定的平均数量。总的来说,我们的方法构成了集体运动模型的新理论发展,因为它考虑了基于其社会环境的内部表示做出决策的代理人,这些内部表示明确考虑了动态内部变量的空间变化。
    Maintaining cohesion between randomly moving agents in unbounded space is an essential functionality for many real-world applications requiring distributed multi-agent systems. We develop a bio-inspired collective movement model in 1D unbounded space to ensure such functionality. Using an internal agent belief to estimate the mesoscopic state of the system, agent motion is coupled to a dynamically self-generated social ranking variable. This coupling between social information and individual movement is exploited to induce spatial self-sorting and produces an adaptive, group-relative coordinate system that stabilises random motion in unbounded space. We investigate the state-space of the model in terms of its key control parameters and find two separate regimes for the system to attain dynamical cohesive states, including a Partial Sensing regime in which the system self-selects nearest-neighbour distances so as to ensure a near-constant mean number of sensed neighbours. Overall, our approach constitutes a novel theoretical development in models of collective movement, as it considers agents who make decisions based on internal representations of their social environment that explicitly take into account spatial variation in a dynamic internal variable.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    随着生物体的发展,他们首先将资源投资于生存和增长,但是在达到一定条件后,他们也开始投资繁殖。同样,超级有机体,比如蜜蜂殖民地,首先投资于生存和增长,后来,一旦殖民地中的工人数量超过生殖阈值,就投入资源进行生殖。蜜蜂殖民地的第一种生殖投资形式是建造由特殊大细胞制成的蜂蜡梳子,用于饲养雄性(无人机)。工人们如何感觉到他们的殖民地足够大,可以开始建造这个“无人机梳子”?为了解决这个问题,我们通过实验增加了菌落大小-工人密度的三种可能线索,挥发性信息素浓度和巢穴温度-并寻找对蜜蜂梳子结构的影响。只有工人密度增加的殖民地才被刺激建造更高比例的无人机梳子。然后,我们监测并量化了小菌落和大菌落的潜在线索,以确定哪些线索随菌落大小而变化。我们发现大殖民地的工人,相对于小的,接触率增加,花更多的时间活跃,体验更少的可变工人密度。尽管单细胞和多细胞生物主要使用化学线索来感知它们的大小,我们的结果表明,至少有一个超有机体,蜜蜂蜂群,使用物理线索来感知它的大小,从而感知它的发展状态。
    As organisms develop, they first invest resources in survival and growth, but after reaching a certain condition they start to also invest in reproduction. Likewise, superorganisms, such as honey bee colonies, first invest in survival and growth, and later commit resources to reproduction once the number of workers in the colony surpasses a reproductive threshold. The first form of reproductive investment for a honey bee colony is the building of beeswax comb made of special large cells used for rearing males (drones). How do the workers sense that their colony is large enough to start building this \'drone comb\'? To address this question, we experimentally increased three possible cues of colony size - worker density, volatile pheromone concentration and nest temperature - and looked for effects on the bees\' comb construction. Only the colonies that experienced increased worker density were stimulated to build a higher proportion of drone comb. We then monitored and quantified potential cues in small and large colonies, to determine which cues change with colony size. We found that workers in large colonies, relative to small ones, have increased contact rates, spend more time active and experience less variable worker density. Whereas unicellular and multicellular organisms use mainly chemical cues to sense their sizes, our results suggest that at least one superorganism, a honey bee colony, uses physical cues to sense its size and thus its developmental state.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究调查了两种社交剑术蜘蛛的紧凑丝巢的建议保护功能(S.dumicola和S.mimosarum),作为他们社会发生的可能的最终因素。作为非洲干旱荆棘国家的居民,这些蜘蛛因过热和干燥而受到威胁。在实验室里,发现这两个物种都避免了温度>40°C。7:00至21:00h之间的巢穴温度往往高于室外的空气温度,并且在13:00至15:00小时之间甚至倾向于超过40°C。因此,巢在温度调节方面没有价值。它也不能有效地保护蜘蛛免受干燥:两种物种的体内含水量均为65.2±3.8%,并且极耐干燥;它们在带有P2O5的兴奋性炉中在37°C下存活了9天,日平均体重减少了4.94(±1.1)。在田野里,在11:00至19:00h之间,巢内的相对湿度往往低于室外空气的相对湿度。对烧毁区域的检查表明,巢穴不能保护蜘蛛免受丛林大火的侵害。巢确实可以抵御风,冰雹,和直接的太阳辐射;但可用的自然撤退,或者一个简单的丝盾,或者是孤立的剑术所居住的漏斗状的丝管,有同样的效果。
    This study investigates the suggested protective function of the compact silken nest of two species of social Stegodyphus spiders (S. dumicola and S. mimosarum), as a possible ultimate factor for their sociogenesis. Being inhabitants of African dry thornbush country, these spiders are endangered by overheating and desiccation. In the laboratory, both species were found to avoid temperatures >40° C. In the field, temperature in the nest between 7:00 and 21:00 h tends to be higher than air-temperature outside, and between 13:00 and 15:00 h even tends to exceed 40° C. The nest thus is of no value in temperature regulation. Nor is it effective in protecting the spiders from desiccation: Both species have a body water content of 65.2±3.8% and are extremely desiccation resistant; they survived 9 days at 37°C in an exciccator with P2O5, with a daily average % liveweight reduction of 4.94(±1.1). In the field, relative humidity inside the nest between 11:00 and 19:00 h tends to be lower than that of the air outside. Inspection of burnt areas showed that the nest does not protect the spiders from bushfire. The nest does protect from wind, hail, and direct sun-radiation; but available natural retreats, or a simple silken shield, or the funnel-shaped silk tube inhabited by solitary Stegodyphus, have the same effect.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    陈述文化特征(知识,身份,实践)是“社会建构的”。\"然而,批评家指出,绝大多数社会被理解为一种背景——一种微不足道的社会意义——而人类实践的真正社会性质往往不会被展示出来。俄罗斯社会心理学家L.S.Vygotsky在他的后期著作中认为社会的首要地位,因此所有更高的心理功能在功能之前都是人与人之间的社会关系。因此,人类发展发生在社会中,并作为社会发生。基于对社会的种族方法论,本文的目的是阐明和发展这种未被认可和未被重视的情况,晚期维果茨基理论的激进方面,从而超越了书面和可能的意图。
    It has become a truism to state that cultural characteristics (knowledge, identity, practices) are \"socially constructed.\" However, critics point out that the social overwhelmingly is understood as a context-a trivial sense of the social-and that the real social nature of human practices tends not to be shown. The Russian social psychologist L. S. Vygotsky assumes in his late work a primacy of the social such that all higher psychological functions are social relations between people before they are functions. As a result, human development occurs in and as sociogenesis. Grounded in an ethnomethodological take on the social, the purpose of this article is to articulate and develop this unrecognized and unheeded, radical aspect of the late Vygotskian theory, thereby going beyond wrote and may have intended.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

公众号