recollection

回忆
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    情景记忆由两个过程说明:通常识别项目时的“熟悉度”和检索与项目绑定的完整上下文详细信息时的“回忆”。矛盾的是,人们有时报告上下文信息是熟悉的,但不记得细节,这是现有理论不容易解释的。我们测试了项目识别置信度和源记忆的组合,专注于“仅项目点击,来源未知”(“项目熟悉度”),\'具有正确源内存的低置信度命中\'(\'上下文熟悉度\'),和\'具有正确源内存\'的高置信度命中(\'recollection\')。多个受试者内部(试验)和受试者之间(个体差异)水平的结果表明,这些水平在行为和生理上都不同。行为上,交叉交互在响应时间上很明显,在项目识别过程中,上下文熟悉度比每个条件都慢,但在源内存期间更快。电生理,a条件x时间x位置三重解离在事件相关电位(ERP)中是明显的,然后独立复制。上下文熟悉度在800-1200ms内表现出独立的负中心效应,在项目熟悉度(400至600ms)和回忆度(600至900ms)方面与阳性ERP区分开来。因此,这三个条件相互排斥,情节记忆的过程根本不同。语境熟悉是情景记忆的第三个独特过程。
    对过去事件的记忆被广泛认为是通过两个不同的过程来运行的:一个在获取自信时称为“回忆”,记忆的具体细节,另一个叫做“熟悉度”,当只有一种不确定但有意识的意识,即一个项目以前经历过。当人们成功地检索到诸如先前事件的源或上下文之类的详细信息时,它被认为反映了回忆。我们证明了对上下文的熟悉程度在功能上与对项目和回忆的熟悉程度不同,并提供了一个新的三变量模型。三种记忆反应类型在多种行为和生理指标上有区别,在试验水平和受试者之间的个体差异中,也是。也就是说,传统上被认为是两种记忆过程实际上是三种,当使用足够敏感的多措施时,这一点变得很明显。类似于仅使用二维镜头观看三维图片时缺少明显的元素,当我们有能力寻找记忆的三个过程时,我们可以看到它们清晰地分离,并且可以在来自不同实验室的不同队列的几项不同研究中独立复制。一起,这些数据表明,背景熟悉是人类情景记忆的第三过程。
    Episodic memory is accounted for with two processes: \'familiarity\' when generally recognizing an item and \'recollection\' when retrieving the full contextual details bound with the item. Paradoxically, people sometimes report contextual information as familiar but without recollecting details, which is not easily accounted for by existing theories. We tested a combination of item recognition confidence and source memory, focusing upon \'item-only hits with source unknown\' (\'item familiarity\'), \'low-confidence hits with correct source memory\' (\'context familiarity\'), and \'high-confidence hits with correct source memory\' (\'recollection\'). Results across multiple within-subjects (trial-wise) and between subjects (individual variability) levels indicated these were behaviorally and physiologically distinct. Behaviorally, a crossover interaction was evident in response times, with context familiarity being slower than each condition during item recognition, but faster during source memory. Electrophysiologically, a Condition x Time x Location triple dissociation was evident in event-related potentials (ERPs), which was then independently replicated. Context familiarity exhibited an independent negative central effect from 800-1200 ms, differentiated from positive ERPs for item-familiarity (400 to 600 ms) and recollection (600 to 900 ms). These three conditions thus reflect mutually exclusive, fundamentally different processes of episodic memory. Context familiarity is a third distinct process of episodic memory.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    识别记忆研究已经确定了成功检索记忆的几种电生理指标,被称为新旧效应。使用各种刺激类型在不同的感觉域中观察到这些效果,但是很少注意它们的相似性或独特性以及它们可能共享的潜在过程。这里,采用数据驱动的方法,使用来自两性健康人类参与者的公开可用的EEG数据,研究不同记忆条件之间共享信息内容的时间演变。来自六个实验。使用了涉及个人高度熟悉和不熟悉的面孔的测试数据集。结果表明,面部刺激的识别记忆神经信号从刺激开始约200ms开始就得到了高度概括。当对非人脸数据集进行训练时,在大多数感兴趣区域观察到早期(约200-300ms)至晚期(后400ms)分化。非面部刺激(音乐和对象/场景关联)的成功交叉分类在后期最为明显。此外,在熟悉的和记忆的物体之间观察到惊人的分离,共享信号仅出现在正确记住的对象的后期窗口中,而熟悉对象的交叉分类在早期也是成功的。这些发现表明,记忆检索的晚期神经信号在感觉模式和刺激类型之间具有普遍性,熟悉和记忆对象之间的分离可以提供对潜在过程的洞察。
    Recognition memory research has identified several electrophysiological indicators of successful memory retrieval, known as old-new effects. These effects have been observed in different sensory domains using various stimulus types, but little attention has been given to their similarity or distinctiveness and the underlying processes they may share. Here, a data-driven approach was taken to investigate the temporal evolution of shared information content between different memory conditions using openly available EEG data from healthy human participants of both sexes, taken from six experiments. A test dataset involving personally highly familiar and unfamiliar faces was used. The results show that neural signals of recognition memory for face stimuli were highly generalized starting from around 200 ms following stimulus onset. When training was performed on non-face datasets, an early (around 200-300 ms) to late (post-400 ms) differentiation was observed over most regions of interest. Successful cross-classification for non-face stimuli (music and object/scene associations) was most pronounced in late period. Additionally, a striking dissociation was observed between familiar and remembered objects, with shared signals present only in the late window for correctly remembered objects, while cross-classification for familiar objects was successful in the early period as well. These findings suggest that late neural signals of memory retrieval generalize across sensory modalities and stimulus types, and the dissociation between familiar and remembered objects may provide insight into the underlying processes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这篇评论批判性地审查了瞳孔测量对记忆研究的贡献,主要集中在增强我们对记忆编码和检索机制的理解上,主要研究识别记忆范式。证据支持瞳孔反应和记忆形成之间的密切联系,特别受检测到的新奇类型的影响。该建议调和了文献中有关瞳孔反应模式的不一致之处,这些模式可以预测成功的记忆形成。并强调了编码机制的重要意义。该评论还讨论了瞳孔的新旧效应及其在回忆和反映与熟悉或新颖性检测相关的大脑信号中的意义。此外,评估瞳孔反应作为真实记忆信号和区分真实记忆和错误记忆的能力。这些证据提供了对错误记忆性质的见解,并提供了对记忆扭曲所涉及的认知机制的新理解。当与严格的实验设计相结合时,瞳孔测量可以显着完善记忆编码和检索的理论模型。此外,将瞳孔测量与神经影像学和药物干预相结合被认为是未来研究的有希望的方向。
    This review critically examines the contributions of pupillometry to memory research, primarily focusing on its enhancement of our understanding of memory encoding and retrieval mechanisms mainly investigated with the recognition memory paradigm. The evidence supports a close link between pupil response and memory formation, notably influenced by the type of novelty detected. This proposal reconciles inconsistencies in the literature regarding pupil response patterns that may predict successful memory formation, and highlights important implications for encoding mechanisms. The review also discusses the pupil old/new effect and its significance in the context of recollection and in reflecting brain signals related to familiarity or novelty detection. Additionally, the capacity of pupil response to serve as a true memory signal and to distinguish between true and false memories is evaluated. The evidence provides insights into the nature of false memories and offers a novel understanding of the cognitive mechanisms involved in memory distortions. When integrated with rigorous experimental design, pupillometry can significantly refine theoretical models of memory encoding and retrieval. Furthermore, combining pupillometry with neuroimaging and pharmacological interventions is identified as a promising direction for future research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    先前研究了检索成功和精度的神经机制,但得出的结果不一致。这里,使用评估空间位置精度的记忆任务检查了它们的神经相关性。在单个研究测试周期中,健康的年轻人样本进行了fMRI扫描。在研究中,参与者观看了一系列物体图像,每个放置在一个假想圆上随机选择的位置。在测试中,研究的图像与新图像混合并呈现给参与者。要求是将光标移动到所研究图像的位置,猜测,如果有必要。然后,参与者发出信号是否已对所呈现的图像进行了研究。记忆精度被量化为研究位置和参与者选择的位置之间的角度。左角回有明显的精确效应,其中BOLD活动在试验中具有定位精度。多体素模式分析还揭示了高精度试验的显着项目级恢复效果。没有证据表明角回具有检索成功的效果。海马体的BOLD活动对成功和精度都不敏感。这些发现部分与先前的证据一致,即成功和精确是记忆检索的可分离特征。
    Prior studies examining the neural mechanisms underlying retrieval success and precision have yielded inconsistent results. Here, their neural correlates were examined using a memory task that assessed precision for spatial location. A sample of healthy young adults underwent fMRI scanning during a single study-test cycle. At study, participants viewed a series of object images, each placed at a randomly selected location on an imaginary circle. At test, studied images were intermixed with new images and presented to the participants. The requirement was to move a cursor to the location of the studied image, guessing if necessary. Participants then signaled whether the presented image as having been studied. Memory precision was quantified as the angle between the studied location and the location selected by the participant. A precision effect was evident in the left angular gyrus, where BOLD activity covaried across trials with location accuracy. Multi-voxel pattern analysis also revealed a significant item-level reinstatement effect for high-precision trials. There was no evidence of a retrieval success effect in the angular gyrus. BOLD activity in the hippocampus was insensitive to both success and precision. These findings are partially consistent with prior evidence that success and precision are dissociable features of memory retrieval.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    注意力增强效应(ABE)和动作诱发的记忆增强(AIME)表明,当参与者同时执行目标检测任务和记忆编码任务时,目标配对项目的记忆性能优于干扰者配对项目。虽然记忆增强已经很好地建立了,目标检测任务如何影响记忆编码的时间动态尚不清楚。为了调查这一点,我们操纵的刺激开始异步(SOA)之间的检测刺激和单词之间的记忆使用记忆/知道研究测试范式,我们主要关注检测响应后出现的单词的记忆性能。结果表明,从SOA=0s到SOA=0.75s,目标配对记忆增强具有鲁棒性,但在实验1A中单独检查或在实验2和SOA=1s时的联合分析中减弱时并不重要,仅在R反应中观察到。当单词和目标之间没有时间重叠时,反应后记忆增强仍然存在,与时间重叠时观察到的记忆增强幅度相似。这些结果支持这样的观点,即目标配对记忆增强(回忆而不是熟悉)发生,而不管这些项目是否与目标同时出现或在反应后的短时间内出现,单词和目标的时间重叠对于反应后记忆增强是不必要的。
    The attentional boost effect (ABE) and action-induced memory enhancement (AIME) suggest that memory performance for target-paired items is superior to that for distractor-paired items when participants performed a target detection task and a memory encoding task simultaneously. Though the memory enhancement has been well established, the temporal dynamics of how the target detection task influenced memory encoding remains unclear. To investigate this, we manipulated the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between detection stimuli and the words to be memorized using a remember/know study-test paradigm, and we focused primarily on memory performance for the words that appeared after the detection response. The results showed that target-paired memory enhancement was robust from SOA = 0 s to SOA = 0.75 s, but was not significant when examined by itself in Experiment 1A or weakened in Experiment 2 and the conjoint analysis when SOA = 1 s, which were only observed in R responses. The post-response memory enhancement still existed when there was no temporal overlap between the word and target, similar to the magnitude of memory enhancement observed with temporal overlap. These results supported the view that target-paired memory enhancement (recollection rather than familiarity) occurred irrespective of whether the items appeared simultaneously with the targets or within a short period after the response, and the temporal overlap of the word and target was not necessary for post-response memory enhancement.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    测试比重新研究相同内容更有利于记忆保留。然而,初始编码方法对测试效果的影响尚不清楚。在这项研究中,采用了经典的测试效果范式,以及事件相关电位(ERP),研究颁布编码对测试效果影响的电生理过程。参与者被随机分配到自我执行测试(SPT)或言语测试(VT)组。两组都经历了三个阶段:初始编码阶段,初始测试阶段(包括源内存任务和重新研究任务),最后的测试阶段。在初始编码阶段,SPT小组通过制定对动作短语进行编码,而VT组通过读取对信息进行编码。在初始测试阶段,与室性心动过速组相比,SPT组在项目记忆方面表现出较好的识别性能.两组在源记忆任务中都表现出显著的顶叶旧/新效应,在重新研究任务中,只有SPT组显示顶叶阳性。在最后的测试阶段,仅在VT组中观察到行为测试效应.此外,与重新研究条件相比,VT组在测试条件下显示出更明显的顶叶阳性,而SPT组两种情况之间的顶叶阳性具有可比性。总之,SPT组缺乏最终行为测试效果可能归因于制定和测试主要通过基于回忆的检索来增强记忆性能,如顶叶阳性所示。因此,最初的制定编码方法为通过后续测试进一步改进留下了有限的余地。这些发现表明,最初的成文法编码,和随后的测试可能是多余的,在提高情节记忆性能。
    Testing is more beneficial for memory retention than restudying the same content. However, the effect of the initial encoding method on the testing effect remains unclear. In this study, a classical testing effect paradigm was employed, along with event-related potentials (ERP), to investigate the electrophysiological processes underlying the effect of enactment encoding on the testing effect. Participants were randomly assigned to the Self-Performed Test (SPT) or Verbalized Test (VT) groups. Both groups underwent three stages: an initial encoding phase, an initial test phase (comprising a source memory task and a restudy task), and a final test phase. During the initial encoding phase, the SPT group encoded action phrases through enactment, while the VT group encoded information through reading. During the initial test phase, the SPT group exhibited superior recognition performance in item memory compared with the VT group. Both groups exhibited significant parietal old/new effects in the source memory task, with only the SPT group displaying parietal positivity during the restudy task. During the final test phase, the behavioral testing effect was exclusively observed in the VT group. Furthermore, the VT group displayed a more pronounced parietal positivity in the test condition compared to the restudy condition, while the parietal positivity between the two conditions was comparable in the SPT group. In summary, the absence of a final behavioral testing effect in the SPT group may be attributed to both enactment and testing primarily enhancing memory performance through recollection-based retrieval, as indicated by the parietal positivity. Consequently, the initial enactment encoding method leaves limited scope for further improvements through subsequent testing. These findings suggest that initial enactment encoding, and subsequent testing may be redundant in improving episodic memory performance.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当前的研究检查了人们对回忆和发生信念的元记忆判断如何随着时间的推移而变化。此外,我们研究了这些判断在多大程度上受到记忆不信任-对一个人的记忆功能的主观评价-通过记忆不信任量表(MDS)和Squire主观记忆量表(SSMQ)测量。参与者(N=234)研究了图形刺激,并在同一时期晚些时候对其中一些刺激进行了测试,但在1、2、4、8和17天后对其他刺激进行了测试。回忆和信念等级高度相关,并且随着时间的推移遵循类似的下降模式。然而,信念下降的速度相对慢于回忆,这样回忆和信念之间的差异随着时间的推移而增加。记忆不信任缓和了回忆和信念之间的联系,在报告记忆不信任较大(与较低)的人群中,这种关联较弱。记忆不信任还与保留期相互作用,以预测记忆判断。记忆不信任的两种衡量标准在预测能力上存在分歧。特别是,只有MDS预测了不相信记忆的自发报告。我们的结果为理论观点提供了支持,即发生信念是一种总结性判断,不仅取决于回忆现象学,而且还取决于元思维信念。
    The current study examined how people\'s metamemory judgments of recollection and belief-in-occurrence change over time. Furthermore, we examined to what extent these judgments are affected by memory distrust - the subjective appraisal of one\'s memory functioning - as measured by the Memory Distrust Scale (MDS) and the Squire Subjective Memory Scale (SSMQ). Participants (N = 234) studied pictorial stimuli and were tested on some of these stimuli later in the same session, but were tested on other stimuli 1, 2, 4, 8, and 17 days later. Recollection and belief ratings were correlated highly and followed similar declining patterns over time. However, belief decreased relatively more slowly than recollection, such that the discrepancy between recollection and belief increased over time. Memory distrust moderated the association between recollection and belief, with this association being weaker among people who reported greater (versus lower) memory distrust. Memory distrust also interacted with retention period to predict memory judgments. Two measures of memory distrust diverged in their predictive power. In particular, only the MDS predicted the spontaneous reporting of nonbelieved memories. Our results provide support to the theoretical perspective that belief-in-occurrence is a summative judgment informed not only by recollective phenomenology but also by metamemorial beliefs.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    独特的编码通常会增加正确的识别,同时也会减少错误的识别。在Deese-Roediger-McDermott(DRM)的错觉中,称为镜像效果,当参与者专注于研究列表中每个单词的独特特征时,就会发生。在以往的研究中,愉快评级任务,用来培养独特的编码,产生了不同的结果模式。我们研究的主要目的是研究在什么情况下这项任务可以在DRM范式中产生镜像效果,基于识别准确性和主观检索经验的证据。在实验1中,使用标准版本(5点Likert型量表上的单词愉悦度等级)与其他两个编码条件进行比较:浅处理(元音识别)和只读控件。标准任务,与其他条件相比,增加正确的识别,但并没有减少错误识别,这个结果可能会受到提供给研究的列表数量的影响。因此,在实验2中,为了最小化所谓的保留尺寸的可能影响,研究名单的数量减少了.此外,将标准版本与据称更特定于项目的版本进行了比较(参与者在考虑唯一原因时对单词的愉悦进行了评分),还包括只读控制条件。在愉快评级任务的两个版本中,与控制条件相比,实现了更正确的识别,两个版本之间没有区别。在这里观察到的错误识别中,只有特定的愉快评级任务相对于控制条件实现了减少。另一方面,在各种学习条件下,主观检索经验伴随着正确和错误的识别。尽管标准的愉快评级任务已被考虑执行特定于项目的处理,我们的结果挑战了这一说法。此外,我们提出了标准任务的可能边界条件,以减少DRM范式中的错误识别。
    Distinctive encoding usually increases correct recognition while also producing a reduction in false recognition. In the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) illusion this phenomenon, called the mirror effect, occurs when participants focus on unique features of each of the words in the study list. In previous studies, the pleasantness rating task, used to foster distinctive encoding, generated different patterns of results. The main aim of our research is to examine under what circumstances this task can produce the mirror effect in the DRM paradigm, based on evidence from recognition accuracy and subjective retrieval experience. In Experiment 1, a standard version (word pleasantness rating on a 5-point Likert-type scale) was used for comparison with two other encoding conditions: shallow processing (vowel identification) and a read-only control. The standard task, compared to the other conditions, increased correct recognition, but did not reduce false recognition, and this result may be affected by the number of lists presented for study. Therefore, in experiment 2, to minimize the possible effect of the so-called retention size, the number of studied lists was reduced. In addition, the standard version was compared with a supposedly more item-specific version (participants rated the pleasantness of words while thinking of a single reason for this), also including the read-only control condition. In both versions of the pleasantness rating task, more correct recognition is achieved compared to the control condition, with no differences between the two versions. In the false recognition observed here, only the specific pleasantness rating task achieved a reduction relative to the control condition. On the other hand, the subjective retrieval experience accompanied correct and false recognition in the various study conditions. Although the standard pleasantness rating task has been considered to perform item-specific processing, our results challenge that claim. Furthermore, we propose a possible boundary condition of the standard task for the reduction of false recognition in the DRM paradigm.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    改善老年人情景记忆的工作记忆(WM)训练结果不一致。这种不一致可能是由于使用的情景记忆测试与经过训练的WM任务不共享相同的认知资源。这项研究的目的是评估情景记忆测试的性能是否只有在该测试需要从二级记忆或回忆中检索信息的受控过程时才会提高。类似于WM培训期间执行的过程。
    55名60岁以上的人参加了这项研究:27人被随机分配到实验组(EG),其余的人被分配到对照组(CG)。EG接受了复杂跨度任务的培训。训练前后,两组都进行了情景记忆测试(口头和视觉空间识别测试)和WM跨度任务(阅读,数字和空间位置)。
    ANOVA显示,对于言语识别(p=0.023)和视觉空间识别(p=0.014),EG的回忆估计比CG的提高更大。
    我们的结果为一种认知机制提供了支持,该机制的共享存在有利于从WM任务的训练转移到对情景记忆的测试。与我们的预测一致,只有当召回需要在辅助内存中进行受控搜索过程时,对复杂跨度任务的培训才能提高识别测试的性能,或回忆。因此,我们强调识别容易从训练任务转移到其他未经训练任务的其他认知资源的重要性。更好地了解转移现象对于为老年人设计越来越有效的干预计划至关重要。
    UNASSIGNED: The results of working memory (WM) training to improve episodic memory in older people are inconsistent. This inconsistency could be due to the fact that the episodic memory tests used do not share the same cognitive resources as the trained WM task. The aim of this study was to assess whether performance on an episodic memory test will improve only when this test requires controlled processes of retrieval of information from secondary memory or recollection, similar to the processes exercised during WM training.
    UNASSIGNED: Fifty-five people over 60 years of age participated in the study: 27 were randomly assigned to the experimental group (EG) and the rest to the control group (CG). The EG was trained in complex span tasks. Before and after training, both groups were tested on episodic memory tests (a verbal and a visuospatial recognition test) and WM span tasks (reading, digit and spatial location).
    UNASSIGNED: ANOVAs revealed a greater improvement of recollection estimates in the EG than in the CG for both verbal recognition (p = 0.023) and visuospatial recognition (p = 0.014).
    UNASSIGNED: Our results provide support for a cognitive mechanism whose shared presence favored transfer from training on a WM task to a test of episodic memory. Consistent with our predictions, training on complex span tasks improved performance on recognition tests only when recall required a controlled search process in secondary memory, or recollection. We therefore stress the importance of identifying other cognitive resources that are susceptible to transfer from a training task to other untrained tasks. A better understanding of the phenomenon of transfer is crucial for the design of increasingly effective intervention programs for older people.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    社会记忆由两个过程组成:与新颖的物种相比,对熟悉的发现以及对过去社会事件的详细回忆。我们使用背侧CA2海马锥体神经元的钙成像研究了这些过程的神经基础,已知对社会记忆很重要,在与新物种和熟悉的同窝动物的社会/空间相遇期间。尽管新颖的个体以低维几何形式表示,该几何形状允许跨不同空间位置的社会身份和跨不同身份的位置的概括,同窝以支持高容量内存存储的高维几何形状表示。此外,熟悉程度以抽象格式表示,独立于个人身份。熟悉程度增加了单个小鼠的CA2表示的维度,预测了它们在社交新颖性识别记忆测试中的表现。因此,通过调整结构化神经活动的几何形状,CA2能够满足不同社会记忆过程的需求。
    Social memory consists of two processes: the detection of familiar compared with novel conspecifics and the detailed recollection of past social episodes. We investigated the neural bases for these processes using calcium imaging of dorsal CA2 hippocampal pyramidal neurons, known to be important for social memory, during social/spatial encounters with novel conspecifics and familiar littermates. Whereas novel individuals were represented in a low-dimensional geometry that allows for generalization of social identity across different spatial locations and of location across different identities, littermates were represented in a higher-dimensional geometry that supports high-capacity memory storage. Moreover, familiarity was represented in an abstract format, independent of individual identity. The degree to which familiarity increased the dimensionality of CA2 representations for individual mice predicted their performance in a social novelty recognition memory test. Thus, by tuning the geometry of structured neural activity, CA2 is able to meet the demands of distinct social memory processes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号