group size

组大小
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    警惕是一种常见的行为适应,可以在逃脱为时已晚之前增加发现捕食者的机会。从长远来看,行为特征在个体之间通常是可重复的,暗示个性上的差异。早期的研究已经记录了分配给警惕的时间的个体一致性。然而,关于个体一致性从一个时刻到另一个时刻实现警惕的方式,以及个体之间不同的警惕模式是否与生存相关,人们知之甚少。我们的目的是确定合作饲养员的哨兵在警惕性上是否表现出个体一致性,以及个体变异是否与年生存率有关。在有利位置的哨兵比赛中,佛罗里达磨砂鸟(Aphelocomacorenulescens)将头从一侧转到另一侧以监视周围环境。在三个野外季节,我们发现,转头频率在育种者中可重复,但在少年或非育种助手中不可重复。育种者的中等可重复性与生存率无关。我们的结果表明,佛罗里达磨砂jay哨兵的转头频率在育种者中是可重复的,但在经验较少的少年或助手中却不是,因此,随着个体年龄的增长,可能变得更加可重复。在我们的研究中,个体警惕性变异与生存有关的假设没有得到支持,需要进一步研究。
    Vigilance is a common behavioural adaptation to increase the chances of detecting predators before it is too late to escape. Behavioural traits are often repeatable among individuals over the long term, suggesting differences in personality. Earlier studies have documented individual consistency in the time allocated to vigilance. However, little is known about individual consistency in the ways vigilance is achieved from one moment to another and whether different patterns of vigilance among individuals are associated with survival. We aimed to determine whether sentinels of a cooperative breeder showed individual consistency in their vigilance and if individual variation was related to annual survival. During sentinel bouts from vantage points, Florida scrub-jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) turn their heads from side to side to monitor their surroundings. Over three field seasons, we found that the head-turning frequency was repeatable in breeders but not in juveniles or non-breeding helpers. The moderate repeatability in breeders was not related to survival. Our results suggest that the head-turning frequency in sentinels of the Florida scrub-jay is repeatable in breeders but not in less experienced juveniles or helpers and, therefore, likely becomes more repeatable as individuals age. The assumption that individual variation in vigilance is related to survival was unsupported in our study and requires further study.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    秃鹰觅食组大小的变化模式,以及它们的生态原因和后果,尽管与这个关键功能组提供的腐肉回收服务有着密切的联系,但仍然很少探索。我们记录了安第斯秃鹰Vulturgryphus的群体大小-频率模式,他们聚集在玻利维亚实验放置的42头马身上,2012年至2019年,海拔范围为1300-4500米。根据觅食组的位置(海拔和纬度),我们研究了它们的大小与物种种群参数(大小和趋势)之间的关系,栖息地条件,以及牲畜尸体的可用性和可预测性。秃鹰利用高频率(93%)的屠体形成组,范围为1至80个个体(平均值=25,中位数=18),并形成了“懒惰-J曲线”典型的大小频率分布模式,其中少数群体(5,12%)大(>55个个体),大多数(21,50%)相对较小(<19个个体)。与海拔高度相关的群体规模,大多数较大的群体形成于较低的地点(低于c。3000masl),可能遵循海拔梯度,从而较大的群体更有可能在较大的屠体周围形成(即,牛),更有可能发生在较低的海拔。不管人口大小,群体规模可能是秃鹰通过局部增强的适应性反应,以提高个体清除效率。关于这一主题的许多信息差距仍然存在,因此,我们提供了一系列问题来解决它们,尤其是在人类活动的不受限制的影响下,秃鹰在全球范围内生存。
    Patterns of variation in the size of vulture foraging groups, and their ecological causes and consequences, remain little explored despite strong links with the carrion recycling service that this key functional group provides. We documented the group size-frequency pattern of Andean condors Vultur gryphus gathered to feed on 42 equine carcasses experimentally placed in Bolivia, between 2012 and 2019, along an elevation range of 1,300-4,500 m asl. Based on the location (altitude and latitude) of the foraging groups, we examined the relationship between their size and species\' population parameters (size and trend), habitat conditions, and livestock carcass availability and predictability. Condors utilized a high frequency (93%) of carcasses forming groups that ranged from 1 to 80 individuals (mean = 25, median = 18) and shaped a \"lazy-J curve\" typical pattern of size-frequency distribution whereby few groups (5, 12%) were large (> 55 individuals) and most (21, 50%) were relatively small (<19 individuals). Group size related to altitude in that most larger groups formed at lower sites (below c. 3,000 m asl), likely following an altitudinal gradient whereby larger groups are more likely to form around larger carcasses (i.e., cattle), which are more likely to occur at lower elevations. Regardless of population size, group size could be an adaptive response of condors via local enhancement for improving individual scavenging efficiency. Many information gaps on this topic still exist, thus we provide a set of questions to address them, especially amidst the unrestricted impacts of human activities that condition vulture survival globally.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们从我们的环境中收集各种类型的信息,并对其进行组织以创建连贯的表示。一些研究人员建议,时间结合窗口(TBW)内的多个信号可以整合到单个相干体验中。如闪光,蜂鸣声,还有McGurk效应.然而,没有证据表明TBW失真也发生在群体相互作用中。本研究使用计算机生成的拍手声实验研究了小组大小(即小组大小效应)和任务参与程度对时间感知的影响。参与者聆听随机产生的拍手声,并评估它们是否同步。我们根据不同的任务参与水平建立了三个条件:低(L),中间(M),和高(H)条件。不同的任务参与反映了参与者如何与拍手的声音互动,从L条件下的被动收听到M条件下的按键主动产生声音,或尝试将按键声音与H状态下的声音同步。我们的实验表明,群体大小对TBW有很强的影响,不管不同的条件。换句话说,随着组大小的增加,参与者认为集体鼓掌是一个单一的事件。此外,我们发现不确定的因果关系条件(H条件)显示最高的TBW。有趣的是,刚性因果关系(M条件)中的TBW与无牵连条件(L条件)中的TBW相同。我们的结果表明,集体行为中扩展的TBW可能会促进凝聚力行动,使个人能够在相对不确定的环境中适应群体。
    We collect various types of information from our environment and organise it to create a coherent representation. Several researchers have suggested that multiple signals within the temporal binding window (TBW) can be integrated into a single coherent experience, such as flashes, beeps, and the McGurk effect. However, there is no evidence that TBW distortion also occurs in group interactions. This study investigates the influence of group size (i.e. the group size effect) and the degree of task involvement in temporal perception using computer-generated clap sound experiments. Participants listened to the randomly generated clap sounds and evaluated whether they were synchronised. We established three conditions based on different levels of task involvement: low (L), middle (M), and high (H) conditions. The varying task involvements reflect how participants interact with the clap sounds, ranging from passive listening in the L condition to actively generating sounds by pressing a key in the M condition, or attempting to synchronise key pressing sounds with the sounds in the H condition. Our experiments show a robust group size effect on TBW, regardless of the different conditions. In other words, as the group size increases, participants perceive the group clap as a single event. Furthermore, we found that the uncertain cause-effect relationship condition (H condition) shows the highest TBW. Interestingly, the TBW in the rigid cause-effect relationship (M condition) is the same as that in the no involvement condition (L condition). Our results suggest that a widened TBW in collective behaviour may facilitate cohesive action, enabling individuals to adapt to the group in relatively uncertain contexts.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    一些认知能力被认为是复杂的社会生活的结果,允许个人通过先进的策略实现更高的健身。然而,大多数证据都是相关的。这里,我们提供了一项实验研究,以了解小组的大小和组成如何影响孔雀鱼(Poeciliareticulata)的大脑和认知发育。6个月,我们在3种社会治疗方法中的一种中饲养性成熟的雌性:一个由3只孔雀鱼组成的小物种组,一个由3只孔雀鱼和3只飞溅的四强(Copellaarnoldi)组成的大型异类组-与野外孔雀鱼共同出现的物种,和一大群6只孔雀鱼。然后我们测试了孔雀鱼在自我控制(抑制控制)中的表现,操作条件(联想学习),和认知灵活性(反转学习)任务。用X射线成像,我们测量了他们的大脑大小和主要的大脑区域。6个人的较大群体,同种和异种群体,与较小组相比,显示出更好的认知灵活性,但在自我控制和操作条件测试中没有差异。有趣的是,虽然社会操纵对大脑形态没有显著影响,相对较大的端脑与较好的认知灵活性相关.这表明超出大脑区域大小的替代机制使来自更大群体的个体具有更大的认知灵活性。尽管没有明确的证据表明对大脑形态的影响,我们的研究表明,生活在更大的社会群体中可以增强认知灵活性。这表明社会环境在孔雀鱼的认知发展中起着作用。
    Some cognitive abilities are suggested to be the result of a complex social life, allowing individuals to achieve higher fitness through advanced strategies. However, most evidence is correlative. Here, we provide an experimental investigation of how group size and composition affect brain and cognitive development in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). For 6 months, we reared sexually mature females in one of 3 social treatments: a small conspecific group of 3 guppies, a large heterospecific group of 3 guppies and 3 splash tetras (Copella arnoldi)-a species that co-occurs with the guppy in the wild, and a large conspecific group of 6 guppies. We then tested the guppies\' performance in self-control (inhibitory control), operant conditioning (associative learning), and cognitive flexibility (reversal learning) tasks. Using X-ray imaging, we measured their brain size and major brain regions. Larger groups of 6 individuals, both conspecific and heterospecific groups, showed better cognitive flexibility than smaller groups but no difference in self-control and operant conditioning tests. Interestingly, while social manipulation had no significant effect on brain morphology, relatively larger telencephalons were associated with better cognitive flexibility. This suggests alternative mechanisms beyond brain region size enabled greater cognitive flexibility in individuals from larger groups. Although there is no clear evidence for the impact on brain morphology, our research shows that living in larger social groups can enhance cognitive flexibility. This indicates that the social environment plays a role in the cognitive development of guppies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    捕食风险会影响行为,生殖投资,and,最终,个人健身。在高风险环境中,雌性经常减少生殖分配,会影响后代表型和繁殖成功。在合作育种者中,帮助者有助于喂养后代,和群体经常一起生活和觅食。助手可以,因此,提高生殖成功率,但也会影响育种者的状况,压力水平和捕食风险。然而,辅助存在是否可以缓冲捕食风险对母体生殖分配的影响仍未研究。这里,我们使用合作繁殖的社交编织者Philetairussocius来测试捕食风险和繁殖群体规模对母体分配到离合器大小的交互影响,鸡蛋质量,蛋黄质量,和蛋黄皮质酮.我们使用成虫主要捕食者的回放增加了产卵前的感知捕食风险,gabargoshawk(Micronisusgabar)。我们还测试了群体规模和产前捕食者回放对后代孵化和雏鸟概率的交互影响。暴露于捕食者的雌性产卵,蛋黄轻4%,但是捕食者的暴露并没有明显影响离合器的大小,鸡蛋质量,或鸡蛋皮质酮水平。回放处理对蛋黄质量的影响与组大小无关,这表明助手的存在并不能减轻捕食风险对产妇分配的影响。尽管捕食者诱导的蛋黄质量减少可能会降低后代的养分利用率,可能会影响他们的生存,回放处理对孵化和成卵成功的影响不明显。助手的存在与捕食者对母体生殖投资的影响之间的相互作用仍然是生活史和生理进化权衡中被忽视的领域,需要进一步研究。
    Predation risk can influence behavior, reproductive investment, and, ultimately, individuals\' fitness. In high-risk environments, females often reduce allocation to reproduction, which can affect offspring phenotype and breeding success. In cooperative breeders, helpers contribute to feed the offspring, and groups often live and forage together. Helpers can, therefore, improve reproductive success, but also influence breeders\' condition, stress levels and predation risk. Yet, whether helper presence can buffer the effects of predation risk on maternal reproductive allocation remains unstudied. Here, we used the cooperatively breeding sociable weaver Philetairus socius to test the interactive effects of predation risk and breeding group size on maternal allocation to clutch size, egg mass, yolk mass, and yolk corticosterone. We increased perceived predation risk before egg laying using playbacks of the adults\' main predator, gabar goshawk (Micronisus gabar). We also tested the interactive effects of group size and prenatal predator playbacks on offspring hatching and fledging probability. Predator-exposed females laid eggs with 4% lighter yolks, but predator-calls\' exposure did not clearly affect clutch size, egg mass, or egg corticosterone levels. Playback-treatment effects on yolk mass were independent of group size, suggesting that helpers\' presence did not mitigate predation risk effects on maternal allocation. Although predator-induced reductions in yolk mass may decrease nutrient availability to offspring, potentially affecting their survival, playback-treatment effects on hatching and fledging success were not evident. The interplay between helper presence and predator effects on maternal reproductive investment is still an overlooked area of life history and physiological evolutionary trade-offs that requires further studies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    群体规模是许多生态和进化过程的重要特征。然而,它不是个人拥有的特质,而是社会群体拥有的特质,许多基因组有助于群体大小理解其遗传基础,因此预测其进化是一个概念上的挑战。在这里,我建议如何将群体大小建模为多个个体的联合表型,因此,解释间接遗传效应的进化模型对于理解群体大小的遗传变异至关重要。这种方法清楚地表明,1)群体规模的遗传变异应该比最初预期的更大,因为间接遗传效应的贡献总是与直接遗传效应的贡献完全相同;2)对群体规模选择的响应应该比基于直接遗传变异的预期更快,因为直接和间接效应之间的相关性始终处于最大正极限1。因此,群体规模应该表现出相对快速的增长和减少,我讨论的后果和证据。
    Group size is an important trait for many ecological and evolutionary processes. However, it is not a trait possessed by individuals but by social groups, and as many genomes contribute to group size understanding its genetic underpinnings and so predicting its evolution is a conceptual challenge. Here I suggest how group size can be modelled as a joint phenotype of multiple individuals, and so how models for evolution accounting for indirect genetic effects are essential for understanding the genetic variance of group size. This approach makes it clear that (a) group size should have a larger genetic variance than initially expected as indirect genetic effects always contribute exactly as much as direct genetic effects and (b) the response to selection of group size should be faster than expected based on direct genetic variance alone as the correlation between direct and indirect effects is always at the maximum positive limit of 1. Group size should therefore show relatively rapid evolved increases and decreases, the consequences of which and evidence for I discuss.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    社会食肉动物经常生活在裂变聚变社会中,可以单独找到共享共同领土或家庭范围的个人,在子组中,或者完全。绝对组大小和子组大小预计将根据资源分配而变化,但是对于易受人为压力影响的物种,其他因素可能是重要的驱动因素。非洲狮子(Pantheraleo)是唯一真正的社会动物,狮子骄傲的特征是裂变聚变动力学,社会群体经常分裂和改革,子组成员身份可以连续且频繁地变化。一个群体中的个体数量可以反映社会,生态,和人为条件。这种动态行为使了解狮子的分组模式对于定制保护措施至关重要。生活在狮子中的群体的进化一直是众多研究的主题,我们利用这些来制定与群体规模和子群规模变化有关的假设。根据从肯尼亚八个地点的199个狮子群体收集的数据,我们发现当狮子更接近人类居住区时,群体的规模会更小,这表明边缘效应正在全国范围内影响狮子。当他们远离水时,较小的群体也更有可能,并且与非常低和非常高的非树木植被水平有关。我们发现研究地点之间存在显著差异,MaasaiMara组最大(平均值±SD=7.7±4.7,范围=1-19),安博塞利保护区最小(4.3±3.5,范围=1-14)。虽然单个站点内的长期研究非常适合彻底区分绝对组大小和亚组大小,我们的研究在全国范围内提供了对脆弱物种中分组模式相关性的独特见解。
    Social carnivores frequently live in fission-fusion societies, where individuals that share a common territory or home range may be found alone, in subgroups, or altogether. Absolute group size and subgroup size is expected to vary according to resource distribution, but for species that are susceptible to anthropogenic pressures, other factors may be important drivers. African lions (Panthera leo) are the only truly social felid and lion prides are characterized by fission-fusion dynamics with social groups frequently splitting and reforming, and subgroup membership can change continuously and frequently. The number of individuals in a group can be reflective of social, ecological, and anthropogenic conditions. This dynamic behavior makes understanding lion grouping patterns crucial for tailoring conservation measures. The evolution of group living in lions has been the topic of numerous studies, and we drew on these to formulate hypotheses relating to group size and subgroup size variation. Based on data collected from 199 lion groups across eight sites in Kenya, we found that group sizes were smaller when lions were closer to human settlements, suggesting that edge effects are impacting lions at a national scale. Smaller groups were also more likely when they were far from water, and were associated with very low and very high levels of non-tree vegetation. We found significant differences between the study sites, with the Maasai Mara having the largest groups (mean ± SD = 7.7 ± 4.7, range = 1-19), and Amboseli conservation area the smallest (4.3 ± 3.5, range = 1-14). While long-term studies within a single site are well suited to thoroughly differentiate between absolute group size and subgroup size, our study provides unique insight into the correlates of grouping patterns in a vulnerable species at a national scale.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在目前的研究中,我们探讨了环境如何影响3至6岁儿童的群体间感知(N=242;Mage=55.5个月,SD=9.94)在法国。我们检查了参与者组成员的影响(属于高与低社会地位群体)及其群体规模对种族分类的发展和文化距离的感知。儿童使用描绘法国三个最具代表性种族群体的儿童的照片完成了两项任务:高加索人,非洲黑人,和北非人。在第一个任务中,免费分类任务,他们被要求给他们认为属于一起的孩子们拍照。结果显示,随着孩子年龄的增长,他们越来越多地根据种族对孩子进行分组。此外,与低社会地位儿童相比,高社会地位(非边缘化)儿童更多地基于种族进行分类。在第二个任务中,孩子们被要求在5点李克特量表上对相同的照片进行评分,以了解三个标准的文化距离:音乐,饮食习惯,和语言。结果显示,无论他们自己的组成员身份如何,在所有标准上,儿童都认为代表有色人种儿童(北非和黑人)的照片在文化上比白人儿童更遥远。然而,这种偏见在群体具有同等数量地位的学校中没有观察到,表明群体在数量上相等的环境会产生积极影响。
    In the current study, we explored how context influences intergroup perception in 3- to 6-year-old children (N = 242; Mage = 55.5 months, SD = 9.94) in France. We examined the impact of participants\' group membership (belonging to a high- vs. low-social-status group) and their group size on the development of racial categorization and the perception of cultural distance. Children completed two tasks using photographs depicting children from the three most represented racial groups in France: Caucasians, Black Africans, and North Africans. In the first task, the free categorization task, they were asked to group photographs of children they thought belonged together. Results revealed that as children grew older, they increasingly grouped children based on their race. In addition, high-social-status (nonmarginalized) children categorized more based on race than low-social-status children. In a second task, children were requested to rate the same photographs on a 5-point Likert scale for perceived cultural distance in three criteria: music, eating habits, and language. Results showed that regardless of their own group membership, children perceived photos representing children of color (North and Black Africans) as culturally more distant than White children on all criteria. However, this bias was not observed in schools where groups have equal numerical status, suggesting a positive impact of environments where groups are numerically equal.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尼安德特人猎杀直牙大象,更新世最大的陆生哺乳动物,在北欧平原的湖泊景观中,125,000年前,正如最近对Neumark-Nord(德国)的最后一次冰川间大象组合的研究所示。有证据表明,人们非常关注成年男性及其广泛的利用,到目前为止,这个位置的数据在考古记录中没有平行。鉴于它们与我们对尼安德特人生态位的了解有关,我们调查了诺马克-诺德的生存习俗是否不仅仅是一种当地现象,可能是由当地特点决定的。分析来自北欧平原上另外两个最后冰川间考古遗址的大象遗骸,格伦和陶巴赫,我们在两个组合中发现了与Neumark-Nord相似的屠宰模式,证明延长大象的开发是尼安德特人在(早期)最后间冰期期间的普遍做法。处理这些动物需要大量的努力,重达13公吨,和大量的食物产生表明,尼安德特人要么有办法储存大量的肉和脂肪和/或暂时聚集在更大的群体比一般公认的。数据不允许我们排除两种解释之一,此外,这两个因素,短期较大的群体规模以及某种形式的食物保存,可能发挥了作用。数据确实表明,在北欧平原的最后一个间冰期尼安德特人中,对大型直齿大象的剥削是一种普遍且反复出现的现象。
    Neanderthals hunted and butchered straight-tusked elephants, the largest terrestrial mammals of the Pleistocene, in a lake landscape on the North European plain, 125,000 years ago, as recently shown by a study of the Last Interglacial elephant assemblage from Neumark-Nord (Germany). With evidence for a remarkable focus on adult males and on their extended utilization, the data from this location are thus far without parallel in the archaeological record. Given their relevance for our knowledge of the Neanderthal niche, we investigated whether the Neumark-Nord subsistence practices were more than a local phenomenon, possibly determined by local characteristics. Analyzing elephant remains from two other Last Interglacial archaeological sites on the North European plain, Gröbern and Taubach, we identified in both assemblages similar butchering patterns as at Neumark-Nord, demonstrating that extended elephant exploitation was a widespread Neanderthal practice during the (early part of the) Last Interglacial. The substantial efforts needed to process these animals, weighing up to 13 metric tons, and the large amounts of food generated suggest that Neanderthals either had ways of storing vast amounts of meat and fat and/or temporarily aggregated in larger groups than commonly acknowledged. The data do not allow us to rule out one of the two explanations, and furthermore both factors, short-term larger group sizes as well as some form of food preservation, may have played a role. What the data do show is that exploitation of large straight-tusked elephants was a widespread and recurring phenomenon amongst Last Interglacial Neanderthals on the North European plain.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景临床病例小插曲是医学教育中广泛采用的教学方法。为了客观起见,可以将案例提供给具有封闭响应选项的学生。虽然解决临床病例已经证明了其在提高医学生临床推理方面的有效性,关于最有效的方法:个人解决问题或基于团队解决问题。目的观察和比较个人临床问题解决方法与基于团队的临床问题解决方法获得的分数。方法征得同意后,共随机抽取了100名学生进行研究.参与者分为两组:个人方法组(IAG)(n=25)和基于团队的方法组(TAG),每组25名学生。两组都有一组10个临床问题,每个都需要“是”的封闭回答,\"no\",或“不知道”。记录并分析参与者的反应,以评估他们解决问题的功效。结果从IAG组的25名学生和TAG组的25名学生中获得了25个回答。IAG评分(7.44±1.12)和TAG评分(7.52±1.66)p值=0.58无差异。IAG组和TAG组之间的10个问题的单个得分之间没有差异。结论本研究发现,个体和基于团队的临床病例解决组之间的得分差异无统计学意义。因此,对于本研究中使用的客观类型的案例解决模式,基于团队的方法可能是不必要的。在未来的研究中,需要进一步的研究来探索这些发现的因素。
    Background Clinical case vignettes are a widely adopted pedagogical approach in medical education. The cases may be presented to students with a closed response option for objectivity. While solving clinical cases has demonstrated its effectiveness in enhancing medical students\' clinical reasoning, there is an ongoing debate regarding the most effective approach: individual problem-solving or team-based problem-solving. Objective To observe and compare the score obtained from individual clinical problem-solving approaches versus team-based clinical problem-solving approaches. Methods After obtaining consent, a total of 100 students were randomly selected for the study. The participants were divided into two groups: an individual approach group (IAG) (n=25) and a team-based approach group (TAG) comprising 25 groups of three students each. Both groups were presented with a set of 10 clinical problems, each requiring a closed-answer response of \"yes\", \"no\", or \"don\'t know\". The participants\' responses were recorded and analyzed to evaluate their problem-solving efficacy. Results A total of 25 responses were obtained from 25 students from the IAG group and 25 responses from 25 groups from the TAG group. There was no difference between the score in IAG (7.44±1.12) and TAG (7.52 1.66) p-value=0.58. There was no difference between individual scores in 10 questions between IAG and TAG groups. Conclusion The study found no significant score differences between individual and team-based clinical case-solving groups. Hence, for the objective type of case-solving pattern used in this study, a team-based approach may not be necessary. Further research is needed to explore factors for such findings in future studies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号