Models, Neurological

模型,Neurological
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对象分类已被提出作为灵长类腹侧视觉流的主要目标,并已被用作视觉系统的深度神经网络模型(DNN)的优化目标。然而,视觉大脑区域代表许多不同类型的信息,并且仅对对象身份的分类进行优化不会限制其他信息如何在视觉表示中编码。关于不同场景参数的信息可以完全丢弃(\'不变性\'),在种群活动的非干扰子空间中表示(“因式分解”)或以纠缠方式编码。在这项工作中,我们提供的证据表明,因式分解是生物视觉表征的规范原则。在猴子腹侧视觉层次中,我们发现,在更高级别的区域中,对象身份的对象姿态和背景信息的因式分解增加,并且极大地有助于提高对象身份解码性能。然后,我们对单个场景参数的分解进行了大规模分析-照明,背景,摄像机视点,和对象姿态-在视觉系统的不同DNN模型库中。最匹配神经的模型,功能磁共振成像,来自12个数据集的猴子和人类的行为数据往往是最强烈地分解场景参数的数据。值得注意的是,这些参数的不变性与神经和行为数据的匹配并不一致,这表明,在因式分解的活动子空间中维护非类信息通常比完全丢弃它更可取。因此,我们认为视觉场景信息的分解是大脑及其DNN模型中广泛使用的策略。
    看图片时,我们可以快速识别一个可识别的物体,比如苹果,对它应用一个单词标签。尽管广泛的神经科学研究集中在人类和猴子的大脑如何实现这种识别,我们对大脑和类似大脑的计算机模型如何解释视觉场景的其他复杂方面的理解-例如对象位置和环境上下文-仍然不完整。特别是,目前尚不清楚物体识别在多大程度上以牺牲其他重要场景细节为代价。例如,可以同时处理场景的各个方面。另一方面,一般物体识别可能会干扰这些细节的处理。为了调查这一点,Lindsey和Issa分析了12个猴子和人脑数据集,以及许多计算机模型,探索场景的不同方面如何在神经元中编码,以及这些方面如何由计算模型表示。分析表明,阻止有效分离和保留有关对象姿势和环境上下文的信息会恶化猴子皮层神经元中的对象识别。此外,最类似大脑的计算机模型可以独立保存其他场景细节,而不会干扰物体识别。研究结果表明,人类和猴子的高级腹侧视觉处理系统能够以比以前所理解的更复杂的方式来表示环境。在未来,研究更多的大脑活动数据可以帮助识别编码信息的丰富程度,以及它如何支持空间导航等其他功能。这些知识可以帮助建立以相同方式处理信息的计算模型,有可能提高他们对现实世界场景的理解。
    Object classification has been proposed as a principal objective of the primate ventral visual stream and has been used as an optimization target for deep neural network models (DNNs) of the visual system. However, visual brain areas represent many different types of information, and optimizing for classification of object identity alone does not constrain how other information may be encoded in visual representations. Information about different scene parameters may be discarded altogether (\'invariance\'), represented in non-interfering subspaces of population activity (\'factorization\') or encoded in an entangled fashion. In this work, we provide evidence that factorization is a normative principle of biological visual representations. In the monkey ventral visual hierarchy, we found that factorization of object pose and background information from object identity increased in higher-level regions and strongly contributed to improving object identity decoding performance. We then conducted a large-scale analysis of factorization of individual scene parameters - lighting, background, camera viewpoint, and object pose - in a diverse library of DNN models of the visual system. Models which best matched neural, fMRI, and behavioral data from both monkeys and humans across 12 datasets tended to be those which factorized scene parameters most strongly. Notably, invariance to these parameters was not as consistently associated with matches to neural and behavioral data, suggesting that maintaining non-class information in factorized activity subspaces is often preferred to dropping it altogether. Thus, we propose that factorization of visual scene information is a widely used strategy in brains and DNN models thereof.
    When looking at a picture, we can quickly identify a recognizable object, such as an apple, applying a single word label to it. Although extensive neuroscience research has focused on how human and monkey brains achieve this recognition, our understanding of how the brain and brain-like computer models interpret other complex aspects of a visual scene – such as object position and environmental context – remains incomplete. In particular, it was not clear to what extent object recognition comes at the expense of other important scene details. For example, various aspects of the scene might be processed simultaneously. On the other hand, general object recognition may interfere with processing of such details. To investigate this, Lindsey and Issa analyzed 12 monkey and human brain datasets, as well as numerous computer models, to explore how different aspects of a scene are encoded in neurons and how these aspects are represented by computational models. The analysis revealed that preventing effective separation and retention of information about object pose and environmental context worsened object identification in monkey cortex neurons. In addition, the computer models that were the most brain-like could independently preserve the other scene details without interfering with object identification. The findings suggest that human and monkey high level ventral visual processing systems are capable of representing the environment in a more complex way than previously appreciated. In the future, studying more brain activity data could help to identify how rich the encoded information is and how it might support other functions like spatial navigation. This knowledge could help to build computational models that process the information in the same way, potentially improving their understanding of real-world scenes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    神经传递的延迟是神经科学领域的重要课题。神经元树突发射或接收的尖峰信号从轴突传播到突触前细胞。尖峰信号然后触发突触处的化学反应,其中突触前细胞将神经递质转移到突触后细胞,通过离子通道的化学反应再生电信号,并将它们传递给邻近的神经元。在将复杂的生理反应过程描述为随机过程的背景下,这项研究旨在表明尖峰信号的最大时间间隔的分布遵循极端顺序统计。通过考虑泄漏积分和火焰模型时间常数的统计方差,尖峰信号的确定性时间演化模型,我们在尖峰信号的时间间隔中启用了随机性。当时间常数服从指数分布函数时,尖峰信号的时间间隔也遵循指数分布。在这种情况下,我们的理论和模拟证实,最大时间间隔的直方图遵循Gumbel分布,极值统计的三种形式之一。我们进一步证实,当尖峰信号的时间间隔遵循Pareto分布时,最大时间间隔的直方图遵循Fréchet分布。这些发现证实了神经传输延迟可以使用极值统计来描述,因此可以用作传输延迟的新指标。
    Delays in nerve transmission are an important topic in the field of neuroscience. Spike signals fired or received by the dendrites of a neuron travel from the axon to a presynaptic cell. The spike signal then triggers a chemical reaction at the synapse, wherein a presynaptic cell transfers neurotransmitters to the postsynaptic cell, regenerates electrical signals via a chemical reaction through ion channels, and transmits them to neighboring neurons. In the context of describing the complex physiological reaction process as a stochastic process, this study aimed to show that the distribution of the maximum time interval of spike signals follows extreme-order statistics. By considering the statistical variance in the time constant of the leaky Integrate-and-Fire model, a deterministic time evolution model for spike signals, we enabled randomness in the time interval of the spike signals. When the time constant follows an exponential distribution function, the time interval of the spike signal also follows an exponential distribution. In this case, our theory and simulations confirmed that the histogram of the maximum time interval follows the Gumbel distribution, one of the three forms of extreme-value statistics. We further confirmed that the histogram of the maximum time interval followed a Fréchet distribution when the time interval of the spike signal followed a Pareto distribution. These findings confirm that nerve transmission delay can be described using extreme value statistics and can therefore be used as a new indicator of transmission delay.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    同步是在涉及多种大脑活动的神经元网络中观察到的现象。诸如Wilson-Cowan(WC)和Jansen-Rit(JR)的神经质量模型显示同步状态。尽管在过去的几十年中对这些模型进行了广泛的研究,它们表现出二阶相变(SOPT)和临界性的潜力尚未得到充分承认。在这项研究中,构建了两个具有小世界拓扑的耦合WC和JR节点网络,并使用Kuramoto阶数参数(KOP)来量化同步量。此外,我们使用同步变异系数研究了SOPT的存在。两个网络都通过改变其节点之间的耦合权重来达到高同步。此外,它们在控制参数的某些值处表现出同步的突然变化,而不一定与相变有关。虽然SOPT仅在JR模型中观察到,WC和JR模型均未显示幂律行为。我们的研究进一步调查了已知存在于病理性大脑状态中的全球同步现象,比如癫痫发作。JR模型显示了全局同步,而WC模型似乎更适合产生部分同步模式。
    Synchronization is a phenomenon observed in neuronal networks involved in diverse brain activities. Neural mass models such as Wilson-Cowan (WC) and Jansen-Rit (JR) manifest synchronized states. Despite extensive research on these models over the past several decades, their potential of manifesting second-order phase transitions (SOPT) and criticality has not been sufficiently acknowledged. In this study, two networks of coupled WC and JR nodes with small-world topologies were constructed and Kuramoto order parameter (KOP) was used to quantify the amount of synchronization. In addition, we investigated the presence of SOPT using the synchronization coefficient of variation. Both networks reached high synchrony by changing the coupling weight between their nodes. Moreover, they exhibited abrupt changes in the synchronization at certain values of the control parameter not necessarily related to a phase transition. While SOPT was observed only in JR model, neither WC nor JR model showed power-law behavior. Our study further investigated the global synchronization phenomenon that is known to exist in pathological brain states, such as seizure. JR model showed global synchronization, while WC model seemed to be more suitable in producing partially synchronized patterns.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    了解感觉皮层内神经元种群活动的共享试验间变异性的起源对于揭示大脑中信息处理的生物学基础至关重要。共享的变异性通常是皮质连通性结构的反映,因为它可能会出现,在某种程度上,从本地电路输入。来自小鼠初级视觉皮层中(兴奋性)锥体神经元的隔离网络的一系列实验挑战了这种观点。具体来说,在已知弱的跨网络连接性的情况下,发现跨网络相关性大于预期.我们的目标是通过生物学动机的皮层电路模型来揭示导致这些增强相关性的电路机制。我们的主要发现是,将每个兴奋性亚群与特定的抑制性亚群耦合在塑造这些增强的相关性方面提供了最强大的网络内在解决方案。该结果证明了在早期感觉区域中存在兴奋性-抑制性功能组件,这些组件不仅反映了反应特性,而且反映了锥体细胞之间的连通性。此外,我们的发现为最近的实验观察提供了理论支持,这些实验表明皮质抑制与兴奋性细胞形成结构和功能子网络,与经典观点相反,抑制是对局部激发的非特异性全面抑制。
    Understanding the genesis of shared trial-to-trial variability in neuronal population activity within the sensory cortex is critical to uncovering the biological basis of information processing in the brain. Shared variability is often a reflection of the structure of cortical connectivity since it likely arises, in part, from local circuit inputs. A series of experiments from segregated networks of (excitatory) pyramidal neurons in the mouse primary visual cortex challenge this view. Specifically, the across-network correlations were found to be larger than predicted given the known weak cross-network connectivity. We aim to uncover the circuit mechanisms responsible for these enhanced correlations through biologically motivated cortical circuit models. Our central finding is that coupling each excitatory subpopulation with a specific inhibitory subpopulation provides the most robust network-intrinsic solution in shaping these enhanced correlations. This result argues for the existence of excitatory-inhibitory functional assemblies in early sensory areas which mirror not just response properties but also connectivity between pyramidal cells. Furthermore, our findings provide theoretical support for recent experimental observations showing that cortical inhibition forms structural and functional subnetworks with excitatory cells, in contrast to the classical view that inhibition is a nonspecific blanket suppression of local excitation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    注意通过选择与决策相关的特征来支持决策。已提出选择性增强相关特征和抑制干扰物作为驱动此选择过程的潜在神经机制。然而,当相关性无法直接确定时,注意力是如何运作的,需要在内部构建的注意力信号很少被理解。在这里,我们记录了注意力转移任务中小鼠前扣带皮质(ACC)中的神经元群体,其中刺激方式的相关性在试验块之间发生了变化。与V1录音相比,在初始瞬态后,ACC中无关模态的解码逐渐下降。我们的分析证明和任务的递归神经网络模型揭示了相互抑制的连接,这些连接产生了在小鼠中观察到的上下文门控抑制。使用此RNN模型,我们预测了单个神经元的上下文调制与其刺激驱动之间的相关性,我们在ACC中证实了这一点,但在V1中没有证实。
    Attention supports decision making by selecting the features that are relevant for decisions. Selective enhancement of the relevant features and inhibition of distractors has been proposed as potential neural mechanisms driving this selection process. Yet, how attention operates when relevance cannot be directly determined, and the attention signal needs to be internally constructed is less understood. Here we recorded from populations of neurons in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) of mice in an attention-shifting task where relevance of stimulus modalities changed across blocks of trials. In contrast with V1 recordings, decoding of the irrelevant modality gradually declined in ACC after an initial transient. Our analytical proof and a recurrent neural network model of the task revealed mutually inhibiting connections that produced context-gated suppression as observed in mice. Using this RNN model we predicted a correlation between contextual modulation of individual neurons and their stimulus drive, which we confirmed in ACC but not in V1.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    处理语言时,大脑被认为部署专门的计算来从复杂的语言结构中构建意义。最近,基于Transformer架构的人工神经网络彻底改变了自然语言处理领域。变形金刚通过结构化电路计算跨单词集成上下文信息。先前的工作集中在这些电路生成的内部表示(“嵌入”)上。在本文中,相反,我们直接分析电路计算:我们将这些计算解构为功能专用的“转换”,将上下文信息集成到单词中。使用参与者听自然主义故事时获得的功能MRI数据,我们首先验证了在整个皮层语言网络中大脑活动的变化。然后,我们证明了由个人执行的紧急计算,功能专门的“注意头”差异预测特定皮质区域的大脑活动。这些头部沿着对应于低维皮层空间中的不同层和上下文长度的梯度下降。
    When processing language, the brain is thought to deploy specialized computations to construct meaning from complex linguistic structures. Recently, artificial neural networks based on the Transformer architecture have revolutionized the field of natural language processing. Transformers integrate contextual information across words via structured circuit computations. Prior work has focused on the internal representations (\"embeddings\") generated by these circuits. In this paper, we instead analyze the circuit computations directly: we deconstruct these computations into the functionally-specialized \"transformations\" that integrate contextual information across words. Using functional MRI data acquired while participants listened to naturalistic stories, we first verify that the transformations account for considerable variance in brain activity across the cortical language network. We then demonstrate that the emergent computations performed by individual, functionally-specialized \"attention heads\" differentially predict brain activity in specific cortical regions. These heads fall along gradients corresponding to different layers and context lengths in a low-dimensional cortical space.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在海马中观察到嵌套在θ节律中的伽马振荡,假设在顺序情景记忆中发挥作用,即,记忆和检索及时展开的事件。在这项工作中,我们提出了一个基于神经质量的原始神经计算模型,它通过利用theta-gamma代码来模拟海马中事件序列的编码以及随后的检索。该模型基于三层结构,其中各个单元以伽玛节奏振荡,并编码情节的各个特征。第一层(前额叶皮层中的工作记忆)在记忆中保持提示,直到出现新信号。第二层(CA3单元)实现自动关联存储器,利用兴奋性和抑制性塑料突触从单个特征恢复整个发作。该层中的单位被来自外部来源(隔膜或Papez回路)的theta节律抑制。第三层(CA1单元)与上一层实现异质关联网,能够从第一个事件中恢复一系列事件。在编码阶段,模拟高乙酰胆碱水平,网络使用Hebbian(同步)和反Hebbian(去同步)规则进行训练。在检索过程中(低乙酰胆碱),网络可以使用嵌套在theta节奏内的伽马振荡从初始线索中正确恢复序列。此外,在高噪音中,与环境隔离的网络模拟了一种精神错乱的状态,随机复制以前的序列。有趣的是,在模拟睡眠的状态下,随着噪音的增加和突触的减少,网络可以通过创造性地组合序列来“梦想”,利用不同情节共有的特征。最后,非理性行为(错误叠加各种情节中的特征,像“妄想”)发生在快速抑制性突触的病理性减少之后。该模型可以代表一种简单而创新的工具,以帮助机械地理解不同精神状态下的theta-gamma代码。
    Gamma oscillations nested in a theta rhythm are observed in the hippocampus, where are assumed to play a role in sequential episodic memory, i.e., memorization and retrieval of events that unfold in time. In this work, we present an original neurocomputational model based on neural masses, which simulates the encoding of sequences of events in the hippocampus and subsequent retrieval by exploiting the theta-gamma code. The model is based on a three-layer structure in which individual Units oscillate with a gamma rhythm and code for individual features of an episode. The first layer (working memory in the prefrontal cortex) maintains a cue in memory until a new signal is presented. The second layer (CA3 cells) implements an auto-associative memory, exploiting excitatory and inhibitory plastic synapses to recover an entire episode from a single feature. Units in this layer are disinhibited by a theta rhythm from an external source (septum or Papez circuit). The third layer (CA1 cells) implements a hetero-associative net with the previous layer, able to recover a sequence of episodes from the first one. During an encoding phase, simulating high-acetylcholine levels, the network is trained with Hebbian (synchronizing) and anti-Hebbian (desynchronizing) rules. During retrieval (low-acetylcholine), the network can correctly recover sequences from an initial cue using gamma oscillations nested inside the theta rhythm. Moreover, in high noise, the network isolated from the environment simulates a mind-wandering condition, randomly replicating previous sequences. Interestingly, in a state simulating sleep, with increased noise and reduced synapses, the network can \"dream\" by creatively combining sequences, exploiting features shared by different episodes. Finally, an irrational behavior (erroneous superimposition of features in various episodes, like \"delusion\") occurs after pathological-like reduction in fast inhibitory synapses. The model can represent a straightforward and innovative tool to help mechanistically understand the theta-gamma code in different mental states.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在这篇文章中,我们开发了一种评估大脑连接网络的分析方法,该方法考虑了受试者的异质性。更具体地说,我们考虑了多主体贝叶斯向量自回归模型的新扩展,该模型估计特定群体的有向脑连接网络,并考虑协变量对网络边缘的影响.我们采取灵活的方法,通过采用加权混合高斯过程的新型贝叶斯非参数先验,允许协变量对边缘强度的(可能)非线性影响。对于后验推断,我们通过实现变分贝叶斯方案来实现计算可扩展性。我们的方法可以同时估计特定于组的网络并选择相关的协变量效应。我们在模拟数据上显示了与竞争的两阶段方法相比的改进性能。我们将我们的方法应用于具有创伤性脑损伤(TBI)病史和健康对照的儿童的静息状态功能磁共振成像数据,以估计年龄和性别对群体水平连通性的影响。我们的结果强调了父节点分布的差异。他们还建议改变年龄关系,在患有TBI的儿童中具有峰值边缘强度,以及男性和女性之间有效连接强度的差异。
    In this article, we develop an analytical approach for estimating brain connectivity networks that accounts for subject heterogeneity. More specifically, we consider a novel extension of a multi-subject Bayesian vector autoregressive model that estimates group-specific directed brain connectivity networks and accounts for the effects of covariates on the network edges. We adopt a flexible approach, allowing for (possibly) nonlinear effects of the covariates on edge strength via a novel Bayesian nonparametric prior that employs a weighted mixture of Gaussian processes. For posterior inference, we achieve computational scalability by implementing a variational Bayes scheme. Our approach enables simultaneous estimation of group-specific networks and selection of relevant covariate effects. We show improved performance over competing two-stage approaches on simulated data. We apply our method on resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data from children with a history of traumatic brain injury (TBI) and healthy controls to estimate the effects of age and sex on the group-level connectivities. Our results highlight differences in the distribution of parent nodes. They also suggest alteration in the relation of age, with peak edge strength in children with TBI, and differences in effective connectivity strength between males and females.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目标导向行为的协调取决于大脑恢复世界上相关物体位置的能力。在人类中,视觉系统编码感官输入的空间组织,但是早期视觉区域的神经元根据物体的视网膜位置来映射物体,而不是他们在世界上的什么地方。大脑如何计算跨眼球运动的世界参考空间信息已被广泛研究和辩论。这里,我们测试了隐秘注意力的转移是否在空间和时间上足够精确,可以在眼球运动中跟踪物体的真实世界位置。我们发现,观察者的注意力选择性非常精确,并且几乎不会因扫视的执行而受到干扰。受最近神经生理学发现的启发,我们开发了一个观察者模型,可以快速估计物体的真实世界位置,并在这个参考框架内分配注意力。该模型概括了人类数据,并为先前报道的现象提供了简约的解释,在这些现象中,观察者将注意力分配到与任务无关的位置。我们的研究结果表明,视觉注意力在现实世界坐标中运作,可以在皮层处理的最早阶段快速计算。
    Coordination of goal-directed behavior depends on the brain\'s ability to recover the locations of relevant objects in the world. In humans, the visual system encodes the spatial organization of sensory inputs, but neurons in early visual areas map objects according to their retinal positions, rather than where they are in the world. How the brain computes world-referenced spatial information across eye movements has been widely researched and debated. Here, we tested whether shifts of covert attention are sufficiently precise in space and time to track an object\'s real-world location across eye movements. We found that observers\' attentional selectivity is remarkably precise and is barely perturbed by the execution of saccades. Inspired by recent neurophysiological discoveries, we developed an observer model that rapidly estimates the real-world locations of objects and allocates attention within this reference frame. The model recapitulates the human data and provides a parsimonious explanation for previously reported phenomena in which observers allocate attention to task-irrelevant locations across eye movements. Our findings reveal that visual attention operates in real-world coordinates, which can be computed rapidly at the earliest stages of cortical processing.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这次回顾始于18世纪末Galvani对青蛙的实验以及他对“动物电”的发现。它继续说明了19世纪下半叶对物理化学领域的众多贡献(能斯特的平衡势,根据威廉·奥斯特瓦尔德的工作,马克斯·普朗克的离子电扩散,爱因斯坦对布朗运动的研究)导致伯恩斯坦在1900年代初提出了他的膜理论,以解释伽伐尼的发现和细胞兴奋性。Hodgkin和Huxley在1952年充分阐明了这些过程,他们详细介绍了静息和动作电位的离子基础,但没有解决这些离子从哪里通过的问题。离子通道存在的新问题,在接下来的二十年里广泛争论,最终被接受,十年后,他们中的许多人开始被克隆。这导致了对大脑中单个神经元的活动以及简单电路的活动进行建模的可能性。利用新千年计算机科学的显著进步,以及对大脑结构的更深入的理解,人们梦想有更雄心勃勃的科学目标来了解大脑及其工作原理。回顾总结了这方面的主要努力,即数字大脑的构建,大脑的计算机模拟副本,可以在超级计算机上运行,表现得像一个真正的大脑。
    This retrospective begins with Galvani\'s experiments on frogs at the end of the 18th century and his discovery of \'animal electricity\'. It goes on to illustrate the numerous contributions to the field of physical chemistry in the second half of the 19th century (Nernst\'s equilibrium potential, based on the work of Wilhelm Ostwald, Max Planck\'s ion electrodiffusion, Einstein\'s studies of Brownian motion) which led Bernstein to propose his membrane theory in the early 1900s as an explanation of Galvani\'s findings and cell excitability. These processes were fully elucidated by Hodgkin and Huxley in 1952 who detailed the ionic basis of resting and action potentials, but without addressing the question of where these ions passed. The emerging question of the existence of ion channels, widely debated over the next two decades, was finally accepted and, a decade later, many of them began to be cloned. This led to the possibility of modelling the activity of individual neurons in the brain and then that of simple circuits. Taking advantage of the remarkable advances in computer science in the new millennium, together with a much deeper understanding of brain architecture, more ambitious scientific goals were dreamed of to understand the brain and how it works. The retrospective concludes by reviewing the main efforts in this direction, namely the construction of a digital brain, an in silico copy of the brain that would run on supercomputers and behave just like a real brain.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号