Mental effort

脑力劳动
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在某些情况下,例如,当我们期望在表现良好的情况下获得奖励时,目标驱动的自上而下的注意力特别强。由于环境因素,这种自上而下的注意力增加的任务特异性知之甚少。要了解绩效-或有的奖励前景在多大程度上可以导致认知过程中特定和非特定的变化,我们在这里研究不同级别的任务规范下的奖励效应。32名参与者在两个连续的视觉刺激的提示下执行了视觉或听觉辨别任务:首先,奖励提示指示良好的表现是否得到奖励。第二,任务提示宣布两个任务中的哪一个将遵循(精确提示)或两个任务将同等可能遵循(不精确提示)。奖励和任务提示准确性均显着提高了绩效。此外,在奖励试验中,精确提示和不精确提示试验的响应时间差异明显强于未奖励试验.这些影响反映在事件相关电位(ERP)慢波振幅中:奖励和精确度都显着增强了任务刺激之前的偶然负变化(CNV)。在早期的CNV时间间隔中,这两个因素也表现出相互作用。对于奖励试验,任务提示之前的负慢波也显着增强。这种影响与响应时间的奖励差异相关。这些结果表明,奖励前景触发了自上而下的预备注意力中特定于任务的变化,这些变化可以随时间和不同的任务要求灵活地适应。这突出表明奖励诱导的认知控制增加可以在不同的特异性水平上发生。
    In some situations, for example, when we expect to gain a reward in case of good performance, goal-driven top-down attention is particularly strong. Little is known about the task specificity of such increases of top-down attention due to environmental factors. To understand to what extent performance-contingent reward prospects can result in specific and unspecific changes in cognitive processing, we here investigate reward effects under different levels of task specification. Thirty-two participants performed a visual or an auditory discrimination task cued by two consecutive visual stimuli: First, a reward cue indicated if good performance was rewarded. Second, a task cue announced either which of the two tasks would follow (precise cue) or that both tasks would follow equally likely (imprecise cue). Reward and task cue preciseness both significantly improved performance. Moreover, the response time difference between precisely and imprecisely cued trials was significantly stronger in rewarded than in unrewarded trials. These effects were reflected in event-related potential (ERP) slow wave amplitudes: Reward and preciseness both significantly enhanced the contingent negative variation (CNV) prior to the task stimulus. In an early CNV time interval, both factors also showed an interaction. A negative slow wave prior to the task cue was also significantly enhanced for rewarded trials. This effect correlated with the reward difference in response times. These results indicate that reward prospects trigger task-specific changes in preparatory top-down attention which can flexibly adapt over time and across different task requirements. This highlights that a reward-induced increase of cognitive control can occur on different specificity levels.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    手姿势生理震颤在算术计算期间增加。目前的工作旨在调查这是否可以归因于压力的增加,因为必须在有限的时间内提供正确的答案,或因不得不说话来报告答案而发出的声音振动。
    在16名参与者中,在同时执行手部姿势任务的5分钟内使用3轴加速度计记录了震颤,同时:1)从4秒内的3位数数字中减去13并进行错误纠正(干预:数学压力任务),2)与“干预任务”相同,但没有时间限制和错误纠正(干预:数学无压力任务),3)将1添加到3位数字(干预:语音振动任务),和4)只有姿势任务,同时保持安静(控制任务)。在手姿势任务中测量了radi伸肌的肌电图(EMG)活动。
    与对照相比,在两种数学干预期间,震颤都有所增加(+30.9%p=0.002,数学压力;+15.0%p=0.01,数学非压力),但不是在语音振动任务(+12.2%p=0.239)。在数学压力试验中,与语音振动试验相比,震颤更大(+21.0%p=0.021),和数学无压力试验(+13.5%p=0.01)。EMG活动未受影响。
    结果表明,在算术计算过程中,“应力分量”仅对观察到的手部姿势性震颤的增加有部分贡献,这种增加不能归因于声音振动。
    UNASSIGNED: Hand postural physiological tremor increases during arithmetic computation. The present work aims at investigating whether this could be attributed to a raise in stress for having to provide a correct answer within a constrained period of time, or to voice vibration for having to speak to report the answer.
    UNASSIGNED: In 16 participants tremor was recorded by using a 3-axial accelerometer during 5 min of a hand postural task performed simultaneously while: 1) subtracting 13 from a 3-digit number within 4 s and with mistakes correction (intervention: math stress task), 2) same as for the \"intervention task\" but without time limit and mistakes correction (intervention: math nonstress task), 3) adding 1 to a 3-digit number (intervention: voice vibration task), and 4) only postural task while keeping quiet (control task). Electromyographic (EMG) activity from the extensor carpi radialis was measured during the hand postural task.
    UNASSIGNED: Compared to control, tremor increased during both math interventions (+30.9 % p = 0.002, math stress; +15.0 % p = 0.01, math nonstress), but not during the voice vibration task (+12.2 % p = 0.239). During the math stress trial tremor was greater compared to both the voice vibration trial (+21.0 % p = 0.021), and the math nonstress trial (+13.5 % p = 0.01). EMG activity was not affected.
    UNASSIGNED: The results suggest that during arithmetic computation the \"stress component\" contributes only partially to the observed increase in hand postural tremor, and that this increase cannot be attributed to voice vibrations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    脑力在调节认知中起着至关重要的作用。然而,注意缺陷/多动障碍(ADHD)患者的精神努力体验可能有所不同,持续的精神努力“回避”或“不喜欢”是DSM的标准。我们进行了范围审查,以描述有关ADHD努力经验的文献。
    本系统范围审查遵循了系统审查和荟萃分析(PRISMA)扩展的首选报告项目,用于范围审查和JoannaBriggs方法。PsycINFO(OVID),搜索了PsycINFO(ProQuest)和PubMed在2023年2月14日之前以英文发表的研究。研究必须包括ADHD人群或ADHD症状学的测量,除了自我报告测量的努力经验或使用努力偏好范式。两名研究人员回顾了所有摘要,一位研究人员回顾了全文文章。
    只有12项研究符合纳入标准。在方法方面,确定了研究中的几个差距和不一致之处,努力的定义,多动症的测量,和样本特征。此外,努力经验的结果模式喜忧参半。
    尽管具有诊断和概念意义,注意力缺陷多动症的心理努力经验没有得到很好的研究。在现有文献中发现了严重的差距。特别提出了努力的三个方面的概念化,任务引发的努力,自愿付出努力,以及与参与努力相关的影响-指导未来探索多动症的努力经验。
    UNASSIGNED: Mental effort plays a critical role in regulating cognition. However, the experience of mental effort may differ for individuals with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), a disorder for which sustained mental effort \'avoidance\' or \'dislike\' is a criterion in the DSM. We conducted a scoping review to characterize the literature on the experiences of effort in ADHD.
    UNASSIGNED: This systematic scoping review adhered to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) Extension for Scoping Reviews and Joanna Briggs Methodology. PsycINFO (OVID), PsycINFO (ProQuest) and PubMed were searched for studies published in English before February 14, 2023. Studies must have included an ADHD population or a measure of ADHD symptomatology, in addition to a self-report measure of the experience of effort or the use of an effort preference paradigm. Two researchers reviewed all abstracts, and one researcher reviewed full-text articles.
    UNASSIGNED: Only 12 studies met the inclusion criteria. Several gaps and inconsistencies in the research were identified in terms of method, definitions of effort, measurements of ADHD, and sample characteristics. Moreover, the pattern of results on the experience of effort was mixed.
    UNASSIGNED: Despite its diagnostic and conceptual significance, the experience of mental effort in ADHD is not well studied. Critical gaps were identified in the existing literature. A three-facet conceptualization of effort is proposed-specifically, task-elicited effort, volitionally exerted effort, and the affect associated with engaging in effort - to guide future explorations of the experience of effort in ADHD.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Letter
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    急性精神疲劳,其特征是在长时间的认知任务期间或之后的认知效率短暂下降,可以通过自适应努力部署进行管理。为了应对精神疲劳,个人可以采用两种主要的行为模式:进行补偿性努力以限制绩效下降,或脱离努力,导致性能下降。这项研究调查了参与者在精神疲劳状况下使用的行为模式。50名参与者接受了顺序任务协议,并在两个单独的会议中进行了平衡会议:30分钟不一致的Stroop任务(疲劳会议)或30分钟的纪录片观看任务(控制会议),其次是最大自愿收缩的13%的时间到耗尽(TTE)手握任务。心理生理措施包括射血前期,心率变异性,血压,和呼吸。行为结果显示,与纪录片观看任务后相比,Stroop任务后的TTE握力性能下降。在Stroop任务期间,参与者更加保守,并且优先考虑准确性而不是速度。在Stroop任务后,自我报告的疲劳更大。心理生理数据显示,随着时间的推移,两种任务的交感神经活动都逐渐减少,Stroop任务显示出更明显的下降。一起来看,这些研究结果表明,大部分参与者(49%)的努力脱离,部分原因可能是对Stroop任务要求的习惯.这项研究说明了在精神疲劳的背景下,努力投资的行为模式之间的相互作用,并强调了脱离接触作为健康参与者对这一现象的主要反应的作用。
    Acute mental fatigue, characterized by a transient decline in cognitive efficiency during or following prolonged cognitive tasks, can be managed through adaptive effort deployment. In response to mental fatigue, individuals can employ two main behavioral patterns: engaging a compensatory effort to limit performance decrements, or disengaging effort, leading to performance deterioration. This study investigated the behavioral pattern used by participants in mental fatigue conditions. Fifty participants underwent a sequential-task protocol with counterbalanced sessions who took place in two separate sessions: a 30-min incongruent Stroop task (fatiguing session) or a 30-min documentary viewing task (control session), followed by a time-to-exhaustion (TTE) handgrip task at 13 % of maximal voluntary contraction. Psychophysiological measures included the preejection period, heart rate variability, blood pressure, and respiration. Behavioral results showed deteriorated TTE handgrip performance after the Stroop task compared to after the documentary viewing task. During the Stroop task participants were more conservative and prioritized accuracy over speed. Self-reported fatigue was greater after the Stroop task. Psychophysiological data revealed a gradual decrease in sympathetic activity over time in both tasks, with the Stroop task showing a more pronounced decrease. Taken together, these findings suggest a disengagement of effort for a large proportion of participants (49 %) that could be partly attributed to a habituation to the demands of the Stroop task. This study illustrates the interplay of behavioral patterns of effort investment in the context of mental fatigue and underscores the role of disengagement as a dominant response to this phenomenon among healthy participants.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在噪音中听语音可能需要大量的脑力,即使是年轻的听力正常的成年人。任务诱发的瞳孔反应(TEPR)已被证明可以跟踪在不断增加的噪声中识别单词或句子所付出的努力。然而,很少有研究研究了倾听努力跨越更长时间的轨迹,更自然,延伸的演讲,或对即将到来的听力困难的预期在多大程度上调节了TEPR。17名听力正常的年轻成年人听了60段长的有声读物,连续重复三次,在两种不同的信噪比(SNR)下,同时记录瞳孔大小。信噪比之间存在显著的相互作用,重复,和持续倾听努力的基线瞳孔大小。在较低基线瞳孔尺寸下,可能反映了较低的注意力动员,TEPR在更硬的信噪比条件下更持久,特别是在第三次演讲之前注意力动员仍然很低的时候。在中等基线瞳孔大小,条件之间的差异基本上不存在,这表明这些听众已经最佳地调动了他们对两个SNR的注意力。最后,在较高的基线瞳孔大小,可能反映出注意力过度调动,SNR的影响最初在第二次和第三次演示中被逆转:参与者最初似乎在更硬的SNR条件下脱离,导致在故事的后半部分恢复的TEPR减少。一起,这些发现表明,随着时间的推移,听力努力的展开主要取决于个人在预期困难的听力条件下成功调动注意力的程度。
    Listening to speech in noise can require substantial mental effort, even among younger normal-hearing adults. The task-evoked pupil response (TEPR) has been shown to track the increased effort exerted to recognize words or sentences in increasing noise. However, few studies have examined the trajectory of listening effort across longer, more natural, stretches of speech, or the extent to which expectations about upcoming listening difficulty modulate the TEPR. Seventeen younger normal-hearing adults listened to 60-s-long audiobook passages, repeated three times in a row, at two different signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs) while pupil size was recorded. There was a significant interaction between SNR, repetition, and baseline pupil size on sustained listening effort. At lower baseline pupil sizes, potentially reflecting lower attention mobilization, TEPRs were more sustained in the harder SNR condition, particularly when attention mobilization remained low by the third presentation. At intermediate baseline pupil sizes, differences between conditions were largely absent, suggesting these listeners had optimally mobilized their attention for both SNRs. Lastly, at higher baseline pupil sizes, potentially reflecting overmobilization of attention, the effect of SNR was initially reversed for the second and third presentations: participants initially appeared to disengage in the harder SNR condition, resulting in reduced TEPRs that recovered in the second half of the story. Together, these findings suggest that the unfolding of listening effort over time depends critically on the extent to which individuals have successfully mobilized their attention in anticipation of difficult listening conditions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:慢性阻力训练和急性阻力练习可改善身体表现,并可增强认知表现。然而,抗阻训练和运动后认知改善的机制仍不确定.最近的研究结果表明,抗阻运动具有代谢和认知需求,这可能会激活与高阶认知功能任务相关的类似神经回路。在不稳定的设备上锻炼会增加协调和代谢需求,因此可能会进一步增加抵抗运动期间的认知激活。瞳孔直径的测量可以提供抵抗运动过程中认知激活和唤醒的指示。瞳孔扩张与多个神经调节系统中的活动有关(例如,蓝斑的激活和神经递质去甲肾上腺素的释放(LC-NE系统)),参与支持执行控制的过程。因此,这项研究的目的是比较在稳定和不稳定的表面上进行急性阻力运动时,通过瞳孔直径测量的认知激活。
    方法:18名参与者(23.5±1.5岁;10名女性)在稳定和不稳定(BOSU®BalanceTrainer)的地面条件下以首选的重复速度进行了十次壶铃深蹲。在站立(基线)和下蹲期间,用眼动眼镜(SMIETG)记录瞳孔直径。清除原始瞳孔数据中的伪影(对缺失值进行线性插值),并进行减法基线校正。学生t检验用于比较地面条件之间的平均瞳孔直径。
    结果:不稳定状态下的下蹲过程中的平均瞳孔直径明显大于稳定状态下的平均瞳孔直径,t(17)=-2.63,p=.018,Cohen'sdZ=-0.62;稳定:0.49±0.32mm;不稳定:0.61±0.25mm)。
    结论:如瞳孔扩张所示,使用不稳定的设备可以增加急性抵抗运动中的认知激活和努力。因为瞳孔扩张只是一种间接方法,进一步的研究是必要的,以描述原因和影响的神经调节系统的活动,在抗阻运动。可以向所有年龄段的人推荐具有和不具有表面不稳定性的阻力训练,作为一种具有身体和认知挑战性的训练计划,有助于保持身体和认知功能。
    BACKGROUND: Chronic resistance training and acute resistance exercises improve physical performance and can enhance cognitive performance. However, there is still uncertainty about the mechanism(s) responsible for cognitive improvement following resistance training and exercise. Recent findings suggest that resistance exercise has metabolic as well as cognitive demands, which potentially activate similar neural circuitry associated with higher-order cognitive function tasks. Exercising on unstable devices increases the coordinative and metabolic demands and thus may further increase cognitive activation during resistance exercise. The measurement of pupil diameter could provide indications of cognitive activation and arousal during resistance exercise. Pupil dilation is linked to the activity in multiple neuromodulatory systems (e.g., activation of the locus coeruleus and the release of the neurotransmitter norepinephrine (LC-NE system)), which are involved in supporting processes for executive control. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to compare the cognitive activation measured by pupil diameter during an acute bout of resistance exercise on stable and unstable surfaces.
    METHODS: 18 participants (23.5 ± 1.5 years; 10 females) performed ten kettlebell squats in a preferred repetition velocity in stable and unstable (BOSU® Balance Trainer) ground conditions. Pupil diameter was recorded with eye tracking glasses (SMI ETG) during standing (baseline) and during squatting. Raw pupil data were cleaned of artifacts (missing values were linearly interpolated) and subjected to a subtractive baseline correction. A student t-test was used to compare mean pupil diameter between ground conditions.
    RESULTS: The mean pupil diameter was significantly greater during squats in the unstable condition than in the stable condition, t (17) = -2.63, p =.018, Cohen\'s dZ = -0.62; stable: 0.49 ± 0.32 mm; unstable: 0.61 ± 0.25 mm).
    CONCLUSIONS: As indicated by pupil dilation, the use of unstable devices can increase the cognitive activation and effort during acute bouts of resistance exercise. Since pupil dilation is only an indirect method, further investigations are necessary to describe causes and effects of neuromodulatory system activity during resistance exercise. Resistance training with and without surface instability can be recommended to people of all ages as a physically and cognitively challenging training program contributing to the preservation of both physical and cognitive functioning.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    虽然心力是一个经常使用的术语,它定义和理解不清。因此,它的用法经常是松散的和潜在的误导。在神经科学研究中,该术语用于表示在满足任务需求时完成的认知“工作”,以及执行这项工作的主观经验。我们认为,将这两种含义混为一谈会阻碍对神经精神疾病中认知障碍的理解,因为认知工作和它的主观体验有不同的潜在机制。我们建议,关于心理努力的最连贯和临床上最有用的观点是它是一种主观体验。这明确区分了由于认知装置的变化而引起的认知障碍,就像痴呆症和脑损伤一样,以及那些在进行认知工作时由于主观困难而引起的,就像注意力缺陷多动障碍一样,抑郁症,和其他动机障碍。我们回顾了神经科学研究的最新进展,表明已经出现了控制任务切换的努力经验,以最大程度地减少“成本”相对于“收益”。我们考虑这些进展如何有助于我们理解临床人群中增加努力感知的经验。这种更具体的精神努力框架将提供对不同临床群体认知障碍机制的更深入的理解,并将最终促进更好的治疗干预。
    Although mental effort is a frequently used term, it is poorly defined and understood. Consequently, its usage is frequently loose and potentially misleading. In neuroscience research, the term is used to mean both the cognitive work that is done to meet task demands and the subjective experience of performing that work. We argue that conflating these two meanings hampers progress in understanding cognitive impairments in neuropsychiatric conditions because cognitive work and the subjective experience of it have distinct underlying mechanisms. We suggest that the most coherent and clinically useful perspective on mental effort is that it is a subjective experience. This makes a clear distinction between cognitive impairments that arise from changes in the cognitive apparatus, as in dementia and brain injury, and those that arise from subjective difficulties in carrying out the cognitive work, as in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, depression, and other motivational disorders. We review recent advances in neuroscience research that suggests that the experience of effort has emerged to control task switches so as to minimize costs relative to benefits. We consider how these advances can contribute to our understanding of the experience of increased effort perception in clinical populations. This more specific framing of mental effort will offer a deeper understanding of the mechanisms of cognitive impairments in differing clinical groups and will ultimately facilitate better therapeutic interventions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Case Reports
    背景:全人群调查和大规模调查强调了严重COVID-19急性期和急性期存在认知缺陷;一些研究记录了在没有先前或COVID-19相关脑损伤的病例中发生认知缺陷。后者人群中认知缺陷的演变及其与COVID-19后疲劳综合征的关系知之甚少。
    方法:我们报告了6名(5名白种人和1名亚洲人)患者(年龄范围:53-71岁,严重COVID-19包括重症监护病房和机械通气的12个月后的结果,平均年龄61.7±6.5岁),在COVID-19期间或之后没有脑功能障碍病史,也没有中风和/或心脏骤停。所有患者都报告了普遍的精神和身体疲劳以及许多多领域的投诉,影响了日常生活。个别患者描述了精神疲劳,冷漠,和/或焦虑。标准化的神经心理学测试显示,在注意力正常的下限,认知功能障碍或表现的孤立症状,Executive,和/或工作记忆领域的六名患者。体细胞量表记录了呼吸困难,肌肉无力,嗅觉障碍,和/或一些轻微的睡眠问题,但不是全部,患者。
    结论:疲劳,可疲劳性,多领域投诉,认知困难,或功能障碍,以及孤立的神经行为和/或精神和/或躯体症状,往往发生在严重的COVID-19之后,并在12个月时持续存在,即使没有先前和/或COVID-19相关的脑损伤。这种临床情况,影响日常生活,呼吁对患者的投诉进行详细调查,它的神经支撑,并制定了具体的康复计划。
    BACKGROUND: Population-wide surveys and large-scale investigations highlighted the presence of cognitive deficits in the acute and postacute stages of severe COVID-19; a few studies documented their occurrence in cases without prior or COVID-19-related brain damage. The evolution of cognitive deficits in the latter population and their relationship to the post-COVID-19 fatigue syndrome are poorly understood.
    METHODS: We report the outcome at 12 months after severe COVID-19 involving an intensive care unit stay and mechanical ventilation in six (five Caucasian and one Asian) patients (age range: 53-71 years, mean age 61.7 ± 6.5 years) without history of prior brain dysfunction and without stroke and/or cardiac arrest during or after COVID-19. All patients reported pervading mental and physical fatigue as well as numerous multidomain complaints, which impacted everyday life. Individual patients described mental fatigability, apathy, and/or anxiety. Standardized neuropsychological tests revealed isolated symptoms of cognitive dysfunction or performance at the lower limit of the norm in the attentional, executive, and/or working memory domains in four of the six patients. Somatic scales documented dyspnoea, muscle weakness, olfactory disorder, and/or minor sleep problems in some, but not all, patients.
    CONCLUSIONS: Fatigue, fatigability, multidomain complaints, cognitive difficulties, or dysfunction, as well as isolated neurobehavioral and/or psychiatric and/or somatic symptoms, tend to occur in the aftermath of severe COVID-19 and persist at 12 months, even in the absence of prior and/or COVID-19-related brain damage. This clinical situation, which impacts everyday life, calls for a detailed investigation of patients\' complaints, its neural underpinning, and an elaboration of specific rehabilitation programs.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在经验文献中,阅读熟悉和不熟悉的单词之间在速度和准确性方面的行为差异是公认的。然而,这些技能熟练程度的标准衡量标准在捕捉视觉单词识别中涉及的即时处理的能力方面受到限制。在本研究中,单词熟悉度的影响最初是使用成人和儿童的眨眼率进行调查的。熟悉(真实)单词的眨眼概率高于不熟悉(伪)单词的眨眼概率。这种违反直觉的结果模式表明,与熟悉的(真实的)单词的处理相比,对不熟悉的(伪)单词的处理要求更高,而且收益可能更低,如先前在行为和瞳孔测量数据中观察到的那样。我们的发现表明,眨眼的测量可能会为视觉单词识别和人类认知的其他领域所涉及的认知过程提供新的思路。
    Behavioral differences in speed and accuracy between reading familiar and unfamiliar words are well-established in the empirical literature. However, these standard measures of skill proficiency are limited in their ability to capture the moment-to-moment processing involved in visual word recognition. In the present study, the effect of word familiarity was initially investigated using an eye blink rate among adults and children. The probability of eye blinking was higher for familiar (real) words than for unfamiliar (pseudo)words. This counterintuitive pattern of results suggests that the processing of unfamiliar (pseudo)words is more demanding and perhaps less rewarding than the processing of familiar (real) words, as previously observed in both behavioral and pupillometry data. Our findings suggest that the measurement of eye blinks might shed new light on the cognitive processes involved in visual word recognition and other domains of human cognition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号