Evoked potentials

诱发电位
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尽管许多研究记录了命题真值在人类心理阅读行为中的作用,关于差分命题真值在处理中国反事实条件中的作用的研究相对缺乏。本研究旨在探讨差分命题值在通过事件相关电位(ERP)处理中国反事实条件中的作用。这项研究是基于理解两种类型的中国反事实条件,这是两种不同的条件连接标记在protasis和apodosis中引入的命题真值,如句子中的真反事实条件标记嘉如(if)&jiu(so)wo香yu嘉如你tuijiujiukeyizaishuixiazhify(我认为如果鱼有腿,它们可以在水下窒息),和错误的反事实条件标记如果(如果)和namo(那么)在句子中wo香沟如果你林namokeyizaishuixiahuxi(我认为如果狗有鳞片,然后他们可以在水下呼吸)。通过操纵protasis和apodosis中的真和假反事实条件标记之间的句子反事实,构造了两个反事实命题值(即真和假命题值)。24名全日制中国大学生参加了ERP研究。结果表明,使用条件标记jiaru(if)和jiu(so)处理真实的反事实命题句子会引起相对于使用条件标记ruguo(if)和namo(then)的错误命题句子的N400效应。此外,具有真实命题条件的反事实句子与原虫中N400效应的启发和自拟中N400效应的缺失不同,表明语义角色可能在命题反事实条件真值的影响下逐渐消失,和/或语义异常在累积句子处理中的作用被消除。而对于虚假的反事实条件句,显示了P300在protasis中的激发和N400在apodosis中的强大作用,表明语义在处理中的作用越来越大。有趣的是,没有P600效应来处理句法违反的句子,这表明在处理具有虚假命题条件的句子时几乎没有额外的句法成本。
    Although many studies document the role of propositional truth-value in human psychological reading behavior, there is a relative paucity of research examining the role of differential propositional truth-value in processing Chinese counterfactual conditionals. This study is to investigate the role of differential propositional value in processing Chinese counterfactual conditionals by means of ERPs (event-related potentials). The study is based on comprehending two types of Chinese counterfactual conditionals, which is propositional truth value introduced by two different markers of conditional conjunctions in the protasis and apodosis, such as true counterfactual conditional markers jiaru (if) & jiu (so) in the sentence wo xiang yu jiaru you tui jiu keyi zai shuixia zhixi (I think if fish had legs so they could stifle under water), and false counterfactual conditional markers ruguo (if) & namo (then) in the sentence wo xiang gou ruguo you lin namo keyi zai shuixia huxi (I think if dogs had scales, then they could breathe under water). Two counterfactual propositional values (i.e. true and false propositional values) are constructed through manipulating sentence counterfactuality between the true and false counterfactual conditional markers in the protasis and the apodosis. Twenty-four full-time Chinese college students participated in the ERP study. The results demonstrated that processing the true counterfactual propositional sentences with conditional markers jiaru (if) & jiu (so) elicited the N400 effect relative to false propositional sentences with conditional markers ruguo (if) & namo (then). Moreover, the counterfactual sentences with true propositional conditions varied from the elicitation of the N400 effect in the protasis and absence of the N400 effect in the apodosis, showing that semantic roles may gradually disappear under the impact of truth value of propositional counterfactual condition, and/or the roles of semantic anomaly was eliminated in the accumulated sentence processing. While for the false counterfactual conditional sentences, elicitations of P300 in the protasis and robust N400 effect in the apodosis were shown, indicating the increasing semantic role in the processing. Interestingly, there was the absence of the P600 effect for processing sentences with syntactic violation, suggesting little extra syntactic cost in processing sentences with false propositional condition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:采用神经心理学量表和事件相关电位(ERPs)研究高神经质和低神经质老年正常人的错误记忆,探讨高神经质对错误记忆的影响及其神经电生理机制。
    方法:进行了横断面研究,其中一般情况调查表,采用成人版艾森克人格问卷(EPQ)和蒙特利尔认知评估量表(MoCA)对郑州市社区60岁以上老年正常人进行多维调查,采用EPQ和一般情况问卷对206名老年人进行综合筛选,将研究对象分为高神经质组和低神经质组。人口按性别1:1匹配,年龄(±2岁),和受教育年限(±2年),最终招募了40名受试者,使用ERP检测脑电图(EEG)成分。使用E-prime2.0系统设计了错误记忆的Deese-Roediger-McDermott(DRM)范式,并提出了刺激计划。实时记录研究对象的脑电信号,并使用64通道Neuroscan脑电信号采集系统进行采集。
    结果:高神经质组的平均准确度明显低于低神经质组,平均准确度差异有统计学意义(P=0.013),但反应时间差异无统计学意义。2.脑电成分N400的平均振幅:N400在脑区的主效应差异有显著性(P<0.001),N400的平均振幅在额叶区域最大,依次为中央区和顶区(均P<0.05)。神经质主效应或神经质与脑区的交互效应无统计学差别。N400的潜伏期:神经主效应没有显着差异,脑区的主要效应或神经质与脑区的交互效应。3.脑电分量LPC的平均振幅:脑区主效应差异有统计学意义(P<0.001),LPC的平均振幅在额叶区域最大,依次为中央区和顶区(均P<0.05)。神经主效应无显著差异,神经质或神经质与脑区的交互效应。至于LPC的延迟,脑区的主效应有显著性差异(P=0.025),额叶区LPC潜伏期短于中央区(P<0.05)。神经质主效应的差异,神经质与脑区的交互作用无统计学意义。
    结论:高神经质可显著增加老年正常人的错误记忆。EEG成分N400和LPC是影响错误记忆的高神经质的潜在早期指标。高神经质可能通过影响额叶皮层功能来影响错误记忆。
    OBJECTIVE: To study the false memory among senile normal people with high neuroticism and low neuroticism using neuropsychological scales and event-related potentials (ERPs), and to explore the effects of high neuroticism on false memory and its neuroelectrophysiological mechanism.
    METHODS: A cross-sectional study was conducted, in which the general situation questionnaire, adult version of Eysenck personality questionnaire (EPQ) and Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) scale were used to establish a multi-dimensional survey in senile normal people over 60 years old from communities in Zhengzhou, and the EPQ and general situation questionnaire were used to comprehensively screen and divide the study subjects into high neuroticism group and low neuroticism group from 206 senile people. The population was matched by 1:1 according to gender, age (±2 years), and years of education (±2 years), and 40 subjects were finally enrolled for detection of electroencephalograph (EEG) components using ERPs. The Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm of false memory was designed using E-prime2.0 system, and the stimulus program was presented. The EEG signals of the study subjects were recorded in real time and acquired using 64-channel Neuroscan EEG signals acquisition system.
    RESULTS: High neuroticism group was evidently lower in the mean accuracy than low neuroticism group, and the difference in the mean accuracy was statistically significant (P = 0.013), but the difference in reaction time was not statistically significant. 2. The mean amplitude of EEG component N400: The difference in the main effect of N400 in the brain region was significantly different (P<0.001), and the mean amplitude of N400 was the largest in frontal region, followed by central region and parietal region successively (all P<0.05). There was no statistically significant difference in the neurotic main effect or the interaction effect of neuroticism and brain region. The latency of N400: There was no significant difference in the neurotic main effect, main effect of the brain region or the interaction effect of neuroticism and brain region. 3. The mean amplitude of EEG component LPC: The difference in the main effect of the brain region was significantly different (P<0.001), and the mean amplitude of LPC was the largest in frontal region, followed by central region and parietal region successively (all P<0.05). There was no significant difference in the neurotic main effect, neuroticism or the interaction effect of neuroticism and brain region. As to the latency of LPC, there was significant difference in the main effect of the brain region (P = 0.025), and the latency of LPC was shorter in frontal region than that in central region (P<0.05). The differences in the neurotic main effect, interaction effect of neuroticism and brain region were not statistically significant.
    CONCLUSIONS: High neuroticism can significantly increase the false memory of senile normal people. The EEG components N400 and LPC are potential early indicators of high neuroticism affecting false memory. High neuroticism may influence false memory by affecting the frontal cortex function.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    θ脉冲刺激(TBS),一种模式化的脑刺激技术,模拟3-8Hz内源性脑节律的节律性爆发,已经成为治疗各种脑部疾病的一种有前途的治疗方法,尽管对TBS作用的神经机制仍知之甚少。我们使用颅内脑电图(iEEG)在10名接受颅内监测的术前癫痫参与者中研究了TBS的神经效应。在这里,我们表明,在29个额叶和颞叶部位的直接电TBS的个体爆发引起了跨越广泛皮质区域的强烈神经反应。这些响应在刺激呈现过程中表现出动态的局部场电位电压变化。包括增加或减少反应,暗示短期可塑性。更强的刺激增加了平均TBS反应幅度,并在更多的记录部位传播,证明了短期可塑性。TBS反应是刺激位点特异性的,在具有强基线刺激有效(皮质-皮质诱发电位)和功能(低频锁相)连通性的区域中观察到更强的TBS反应。Further,我们可以使用这些措施来预测稳定和变化(例如短期可塑性)的TBS响应位置。未来的工作可能会将治疗前的连通性与其他生物物理因素相结合,以个性化刺激参数,从而优化疾病相关脑网络中神经可塑性的诱导。
    Theta-burst stimulation (TBS), a patterned brain stimulation technique that mimics rhythmic bursts of 3-8 Hz endogenous brain rhythms, has emerged as a promising therapeutic approach for treating a wide range of brain disorders, though the neural mechanism of TBS action remains poorly understood. We investigated the neural effects of TBS using intracranial EEG (iEEG) in 10 pre-surgical epilepsy participants undergoing intracranial monitoring. Here we show that individual bursts of direct electrical TBS at 29 frontal and temporal sites evoked strong neural responses spanning broad cortical regions. These responses exhibited dynamic local field potential voltage changes over the course of stimulation presentations, including either increasing or decreasing responses, suggestive of short-term plasticity. Stronger stimulation augmented the mean TBS response amplitude and spread with more recording sites demonstrating short-term plasticity. TBS responses were stimulation site-specific with stronger TBS responses observed in regions with strong baseline stimulation effective (cortico-cortical evoked potentials) and functional (low frequency phase locking) connectivity. Further, we could use these measures to predict stable and varying (e.g. short-term plasticity) TBS response locations. Future work may integrate pre-treatment connectivity alongside other biophysical factors to personalize stimulation parameters, thereby optimizing induction of neuroplasticity within disease-relevant brain networks.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    脑电图(EEG)是一种非侵入性方法,用于跟踪人脑随时间的活动。外部事件的时间锁定EEG被称为事件相关电位(ERP)。ERP可以是人类感知和其他认知过程的生物标志物。ERP研究的成功取决于测试对象的实验室条件和注意力。具体来说,无法控制实验变量减少了现实世界中的ERP研究。这项研究在听觉怪球范例实验中收集了各种实验情况下的EEG数据,以在正常实验室条件下将ERP用作活性生物标志物。然后,对ERP时代进行了分析,以识别不集中的时代,受典型伪影和外部失真的影响。对于初始比较,评估了四种无监督机器学习算法(MLA)识别未聚焦时代的能力。然后,将其准确性与人体检查和当前的EEG分析工具(EEGLab)进行了比较.所有四个MLA通常为95-100%准确。总之,我们的分析发现,人类可能会错过常规ERP模式的细微差异,但是MLA可以有效地识别这些。因此,我们的分析表明,与其他两种标准方法相比,无监督MLA在检测未集中的ERP时期方面表现更好.
    Electroencephalography (EEG) is a non-invasive method used to track human brain activity over time. The time-locked EEG to an external event is known as event-related potential (ERP). ERP can be a biomarker of human perception and other cognitive processes. The success of ERP research depends on the laboratory conditions and attentiveness of the test subjects. Specifically, the inability to control experimental variables has reduced ERP research in the real world. This study collected EEG data under various experimental circumstances within an auditory oddball paradigm experiment to enable the use of ERP as an active biomarker in normal laboratory conditions. Then, ERP epochs were analyzed to identify unfocused epochs, affected by typical artifacts and external distortion. For the initial comparison, the ability of four unsupervised machine learning algorithms (MLAs) was evaluated to identify unfocused epochs. Then, their accuracy was compared with the human inspection and a current EEG analysis tool (EEGLab). All four MLAs were typically 95-100% accurate. In summary, our analysis finds that humans might miss subtle differences in the regular ERP patterns, but MLAs could efficiently identify those. Thus, our analysis suggests that unsupervised MLAs perform better for detecting unfocused ERP epochs compared with the other two standard methods.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    拟人化机器人正日益融入人类社会生活,在各个领域发挥重要作用。这项研究旨在阐明用户对具有不同拟人化水平的机器人的感知和情绪反应的潜在神经动力学。我们调查了参与者观看时引起的事件相关电位(ERPs)和事件相关频谱扰动(ERSP),感知,并对机器人的影响进行了低评价(L-AR),中等(M-AR),和高(H-AR)水平的拟人化。记录42名参与者的脑电图数据。结果表明,H-AR引起更负的N1和增加的额叶θ功率,但在早期时间窗口中P2降低。相反,与H-AR相比,M-AR和L-AR引起更大的P2。在以后的时间窗口中,与H-AR和L-AR相比,M-AR产生更大的晚期正电位(LPP)和增强的顶叶-枕骨theta振荡。这些发现表明了不同的神经处理阶段:早期特征检测和选择性注意力分配,其次是后来的情感评估。早期发现面部形态和敌意,P2反映高阶视觉处理,似乎与拟人化水平相关。这项研究促进了对拟人化机器人设计中情感处理的理解,并为机器人设计师和制造商提供了有关情感和功能设计的宝贵见解,评估,和推广拟人化机器人。
    Anthropomorphized robots are increasingly integrated into human social life, playing vital roles across various fields. This study aimed to elucidate the neural dynamics underlying users\' perceptual and emotional responses to robots with varying levels of anthropomorphism. We investigated event-related potentials (ERPs) and event-related spectral perturbations (ERSPs) elicited while participants viewed, perceived, and rated the affection of robots with low (L-AR), medium (M-AR), and high (H-AR) levels of anthropomorphism. EEG data were recorded from 42 participants. Results revealed that H-AR induced a more negative N1 and increased frontal theta power, but decreased P2 in early time windows. Conversely, M-AR and L-AR elicited larger P2 compared to H-AR. In later time windows, M-AR generated greater late positive potential (LPP) and enhanced parietal-occipital theta oscillations than H-AR and L-AR. These findings suggest distinct neural processing phases: early feature detection and selective attention allocation, followed by later affective appraisal. Early detection of facial form and animacy, with P2 reflecting higher-order visual processing, appeared to correlate with anthropomorphism levels. This research advances the understanding of emotional processing in anthropomorphic robot design and provides valuable insights for robot designers and manufacturers regarding emotional and feature design, evaluation, and promotion of anthropomorphic robots.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    视觉单词识别的所有主要模型都采用分层过程,该过程将视觉输入逐步转换为抽象字母和单词表示。然而,最近的行为研究结果表明,具有高度一致的视觉格式的单词的心理表征,如标识,可能不仅包括纯粹的抽象信息,还包括感知信息。这个假设可以解释为什么参与者经常在标识中更大程度地误解了原始基词的转置字母拼写错误(例如,萨斯蒙格,但不是SARVUNG,被认为是SAMSUNG)而不是常用语。本实验在ERPgo/no-go语义分类实验中检查了正确拼写和拼写错误的标识(通过字母转位或替换)背后的电生理特征。结果表明,转置字母拼写错误的逻辑型(SASMUNG)和完整的逻辑型(SAMSUNG)的N400振幅在各个时间窗口(直到600ms)中没有显着差异,而替换字母拼写错误的逻辑型(SARVUNG)产生的N400振幅始终较大。这些发现表明,逻辑型的心理表征对微小的正字法变化特别有抵抗力,具有重要的理论和应用价值(例如,营销)含义。
    All leading models of visual word recognition assume a hierarchical process that progressively converts the visual input into abstract letter and word representations. However, the results from recent behavioral studies suggest that the mental representations of words with a highly consistent visual format, such as logotypes, may comprise not only purely abstract information but also perceptual information. This hypothesis would explain why participants often misperceive transposed-letter misspellings with the original base words to a larger degree in logotypes (e.g., SASMUNG, but not SARVUNG, is perceived as SAMSUNG) than in common words. The present experiment examined the electrophysiological signature behind the identification of correctly spelled and misspelled logotypes (via letter transposition or replacement) in an ERP go/no-go semantic categorization experiment. Results showed that N400 amplitudes for transposed-letter misspelled logotypes (SASMUNG) and intact logotypes (SAMSUNG) did not differ significantly across various time windows (until 600 ms), whereas replacement-letter misspelled logotypes (SARVUNG) yielded consistently larger N400 amplitudes. These findings reveal that the mental representations of logotypes are particularly resistant to minor orthographic changes, which has important theoretical and applied (e.g., marketing) implications.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    选择的自由增强了我们的代理意识。在目标导向行为期间,在不同的反应选项之间进行选择的自由增加了正反馈和负反馈的神经处理,表明在高机构经验的条件下加强了结果监测。然而,目前尚不清楚这种增强是否主要是由自我相对于外部确定的行动结果的显著性增加所驱动,或者在目标导向任务中,感知的结果工具价值的差异是否有助于结果监测.为了测试这个,我们记录了脑电图,而参与者执行了涉及自由选择的强化学习任务,与行动相关的强迫选择,和行动无关的强迫选择。与行动相关和行动无关的强迫选择相比,我们观察到自由选择后反馈的中额θ功率和N100幅度更大。此外,奖励积极性仅是免费的,而不是强迫选择的结果。至关重要的是,我们的结果表明,增强的结局处理不是由结局与未来行动的相关性驱动的,而是源于结局与近期自主选择的关联.我们的发现强调了自决在跟踪我们行为的后果中的关键作用,并有助于理解结果监测中选择引起的促进作用的认知过程。
    Freedom of choice enhances our sense of agency. During goal-directed behavior, the freedom to choose between different response options increases the neural processing of positive and negative feedback, indicating enhanced outcome monitoring under conditions of high agency experience. However, it is unclear whether this enhancement is predominantly driven by an increased salience of self- compared to externally determined action outcomes or whether differences in the perceived instrumental value of outcomes contribute to outcome monitoring in goal-directed tasks. To test this, we recorded electroencephalography while participants performed a reinforcement learning task involving free choices, action-relevant forced choices, and action-irrelevant forced choices. We observed larger midfrontal theta power and N100 amplitudes for feedback following free choices compared with action-relevant and action-irrelevant forced choices. In addition, a Reward Positivity was only present for free but not forced choice outcomes. Crucially, our results indicate that enhanced outcome processing is not driven by the relevance of outcomes for future actions but rather stems from the association of outcomes with recent self-determined choice. Our findings highlight the pivotal role of self-determination in tracking the consequences of our actions and contribute to an understanding of the cognitive processes underlying the choice-induced facilitation in outcome monitoring.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    即使在轻度认知障碍(MCI)的早期阶段,阿尔茨海默病的认知下降也与脑电图(EEG)生物特征有关。这项工作的目的是通过汇总来自多种EEG模态的生物特征来提供认知下降的统一度量,并在个体水平上评估复合度量的可重复性。这些模式包括静息状态EEG(闭眼)和两个与视觉记忆和注意力有关的事件相关电位(ERP)任务。我们比较了MCI个体(n=38)和年龄匹配的健康对照HC(n=44)。在静息状态下脑电图,MCI组在Theta(3-7Hz)中表现出更高的功率,在Beta(13-20Hz)频带中表现出更低的功率。在这两个ERP任务中,MCI组表现出降低的ERP晚期正电位(LPP),延迟的ERP早期组件延迟,较慢的反应时间,降低了响应精度。基于簇的排列分析显示,MCI和HC组在频率通道和时间通道空间中存在明显的差异。选择基于聚类的测量和性能测量(总共12个生物特征)作为MCI的预测因子。我们使用所有数据在交叉验证中训练了支持向量机(SVM)分类器,实现AUC=0.89,准确度=77%。分割数据验证导致基线和随访时的测试数据(AUC=0.87,准确性=76%)和(AUC=0.75,准确性=70%),分别。基线和随访时的分类评分是相关的(r=0.72,p<0.001,ICC=0.84),支持EEG生物特征的重测可靠性。这些结果支持EEG/ERP用于预后测试的实用性,反复评估,并在有限的临床试验期间跟踪潜在的治疗结果。
    Cognitive decline in Alzheimer\'s disease is associated with electroencephalographic (EEG) biosignatures even at early stages of mild cognitive impairment (MCI). The aim of this work is to provide a unified measure of cognitive decline by aggregating biosignatures from multiple EEG modalities and to evaluate repeatability of the composite measure at an individual level. These modalities included resting state EEG (eyes-closed) and two event-related potential (ERP) tasks on visual memory and attention. We compared individuals with MCI (n = 38) to age-matched healthy controls HC (n = 44). In resting state EEG, the MCI group exhibited higher power in Theta (3-7Hz) and lower power in Beta (13-20Hz) frequency bands. In both ERP tasks, the MCI group exhibited reduced ERP late positive potential (LPP), delayed ERP early component latency, slower reaction time, and decreased response accuracy. Cluster-based permutation analysis revealed significant clusters of difference between the MCI and HC groups in the frequency-channel and time-channel spaces. Cluster-based measures and performance measures (12 biosignatures in total) were selected as predictors of MCI. We trained a support vector machine (SVM) classifier achieving AUC = 0.89, accuracy = 77% in cross-validation using all data. Split-data validation resulted in (AUC = 0.87, accuracy = 76%) and (AUC = 0.75, accuracy = 70%) on testing data at baseline and follow-up visits, respectively. Classification scores at baseline and follow-up visits were correlated (r = 0.72, p<0.001, ICC = 0.84), supporting test-retest reliability of EEG biosignature. These results support the utility of EEG/ERP for prognostic testing, repeated assessments, and tracking potential treatment outcomes in the limited duration of clinical trials.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    选择历史是指以前的注意力或抑制分配有可能引起与目标驱动或刺激驱动的注意力隔离的挥之不去的持久选择偏见。然而,在单例检测模式任务中,操纵干扰物的选择历史不能引起纯粹的主动抑制。因此,我们采用了工作记忆任务和特征搜索模式任务的组合,同时使用脑电图记录皮质活动,研究选择史指导下的抑制机制。事件相关电位和反应时间的结果显示,当干扰物出现在高概率位置时,抑制性能增强,以及目标出现在干扰物的高概率位置的实例。这些发现表明,广义的主动抑制偏差是独立于认知资源而学习和处理的,它由选择历史记录支持。相比之下,在不同的认知资源条件下,通过Pd成分对低概率位置的反应性拒绝很明显。一起来看,我们的研究结果表明,当分心者处于高概率位置时,参与者学会了主动抑制,而反应性排斥涉及低概率位置。
    Selection history refers to the notion that previous allocations of attention or suppression have the potential to elicit lingering and enduring selection biases that are isolated from goal-driven or stimulus-driven attention. However, in the singleton detection mode task, manipulating the selection history of distractors cannot give rise to pure proactive inhibition. Therefore, we employed a combination of a working memory task and a feature search mode task, simultaneously recording cortical activity using EEG, to investigate the mechanisms of suppression guided by selection history. The results from event-related potential and reaction times showed an enhanced inhibitory performance when the distractor was presented at the high-probability location, along with instances where the target appeared at the high-probability location of distractors. These findings demonstrate that a generalized proactive inhibition bias is learned and processed independent of cognitive resources, which is supported by selection history. In contrast, reactive rejection toward the low-probability location was evident through the Pd component under varying cognitive resource conditions. Taken together, our findings indicated that participants learned proactive inhibition when the distractor was at the high-probability location, whereas reactive rejection was involved at low-probability location.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究的目的是确定认知改变,如事件相关电位(ERP)所示,每天暴露于theta双耳搏动(BBs)10分钟一个月后。将招募的健康受试者(n=60)平均分为实验组和对照组。一个月来,实验组要求每天练习BB听力,而对照组没有。在为期一个月的三次独立访问中评估了ERP,每次访问之间有两周的间隔。每次访问,在听之前和之后测量ERPs。每次就诊时,听觉和视觉ERP均显着增加了听觉和视觉P300的振幅。BBs在所有访问中一致地增强了听觉N200振幅,但是视觉N200振幅仅在第二次和第三次访问时增加。与健康对照相比,每日暴露于BBs两周导致听觉P300振幅增加。此外,四周的BBs暴露不仅增加了听觉P300的振幅,而且减少了P300的潜伏期。这些初步发现表明,每天以6Hz的频率听BB10分钟可能会增强认知功能的某些方面。然而,需要进一步的研究来确认这些影响并了解潜在的机制.确定听6HzBB的最佳持续时间和练习可能有助于健康个体的认知增强策略。
    The aim of the present study was to identify cognitive alterations, as indicated by event-related potentials (ERPs), after one month of daily exposure to theta binaural beats (BBs) for 10 minutes. The recruited healthy subjects (n = 60) were equally divided into experimental and control groups. For a month, the experimental group was required to practice BBs listening daily, while the control group did not. ERPs were assessed at three separate visits over a span of one month, with a two-week interval between each visit. At each visit, ERPs were measured before and after listening. The auditory and visual ERPs significantly increased the auditory and visual P300 amplitudes consistently at each visit. BBs enhanced the auditory N200 amplitude consistently across all visits, but the visual N200 amplitude increased only at the second and third visits. Compared to the healthy controls, daily exposure to BBs for two weeks resulted in increased auditory P300 amplitude. Additionally, four weeks of BBs exposure not only increased auditory P300 amplitude but also reduced P300 latency. These preliminary findings suggest that listening to BBs at 6 Hz for 10 minutes daily may enhance certain aspects of cognitive function. However, further research is needed to confirm these effects and to understand the underlying mechanisms. Identifying the optimal duration and practice of listening to 6 Hz BBs could potentially contribute to cognitive enhancement strategies in healthy individuals.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号