Event-related potentials (ERPs)

事件相关电位 (ERPs)
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    写作是人类最深刻的发明之一,促进大量信息的有效共享和传输。类似于图像和面部表情,视觉(书面)单词具有唤起情感内涵的能力。理解大脑如何感知这些用高度象征性的视觉词汇编码的情感细微差别是“情感神经语言学”新兴领域的一个重点。“这项调查的核心在于对早期后验负(EPN)的检查,事件相关电位(ERPs)成分在枕颞叶头皮区域刺激开始后约300ms达到峰值。EPN一直在回应情绪刺激,包括图片,面孔,视觉词。然而,先前的研究明显缺乏对EPN对汉语情感词的反应的观察,提出了关于汉语和其他语言之间情感处理的潜在差异的问题。鉴于汉语书写系统的语标性质和复合词在汉语词典中的普遍性,本研究旨在探讨汉语单态和复合词的情绪处理是否会引发EPN反应。进行了两个实验:实验1使用单字符单词(单形单词),而实验2使用了两个字符的单词(复合词)。参与者被分配了一个去/不去的任务,指示对未知单词(单词识别任务)或蓝色刺激(颜色决定任务)做出反应。使用数据驱动的质量单变量方法进行的数据分析显示,情感词和中性词之间的ERP差异显着。值得注意的是,时间进程,头皮地形图,在两个实验中,差异ERP的皮层发生器都表现出特征性的EPN响应。这些发现强烈支持了这样一种观点,即EPN反映了汉语单态和复合词中情感内涵的加工,为将来使用EPN作为与情感相关的ERP组件来调查汉语单词的情感处理奠定基础。
    Writing stands as one of humanity\'s most profound inventions, facilitating the efficient sharing and transmission of vast amounts of information. Similar to images and facial expressions, visual (written) words possess the ability to evoke emotional connotations. Understanding how the brain perceives these emotional nuances encoded in highly symbolic visual words is a key focus of the emerging field of \"affective neurolinguistics.\" At the core of this inquiry lies the examination of the early posterior negativity (EPN), an event-related potentials (ERPs) component peaking around 300 ms after stimulus onset in the occipitotemporal scalp region. EPN has consistently emerged in response to emotional stimuli, encompassing pictures, faces, and visual words. However, prior research has notably lacked observation of EPN in response to Chinese emotional words, raising questions about potential differences in emotional processing between Chinese and other languages. Given the logographic nature of the Chinese writing system and the prevalence of compound words in the Chinese lexicon, this study aims to explore whether the emotional processing of Chinese monomorphic and compound words elicits an EPN response. Two experiments were conducted: Experiment 1 utilized one-character words (monomorphic words), while Experiment 2 employed two-character words (compound words). Participants were assigned a go/no-go task, instructed to respond to unknown words (word recognition task) or blue stimuli (color decision task). Data analysis using a data-driven mass univariate approach revealed significant ERP differences between emotional and neutral words. Notably, the time course, scalp topography, and cortical generators of the difference ERP presented a characteristic EPN response in both experiments. These findings strongly support the notion that the processing of emotional connotations in both Chinese monomorphic and compound words is reflected by the EPN, paving the way for future research using EPN as an emotion-related ERP component for investigating emotional processing of Chinese words.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    语音中的声学信息不断变化,然而,听众形成离散的感知类别,以缓解感知的需求。与更离散/分类的收听者相反,更连续/梯度对于通过增加感知灵活性和解决模糊性来理解噪声中的语音可以是进一步有利的。在语音标记任务期间,可以使用视觉模拟缩放(VAS)对收听者对连续语音的响应进行分类与连续的程度进行量化。这里,我们沿声-音连续体(/u/to/a/)记录了与事件相关的脑电位(ERP)与元音的关系,而听者在清洁和噪音条件下对音素进行了分类.使用标准的两种替代强制选择(2AFC)和VAS范例评估行为,以评估任务结构下的分类,从而促进离散与离散的连续听力,分别。行为上,在2AFC下,识别曲线更陡峭。VAS分类,但相对不受噪声影响,建议对抽象的强大访问,即使在信号退化的情况下,语音类别也是如此。行为斜率与听众QuickSIN分数相关;较浅的斜率对应于较好的噪声表现,暗示了更梯度的听力策略赋予的噪声降低的语音理解的感知优势。在神经层面,ERP的P2幅度和延迟由任务和噪声调制;与2AFC响应相比,VAS响应更大,并且显示出与噪声相关的延迟延迟更大。更多的梯度响应者在有噪声的ERP延迟中有较小的变化,这表明他们的语音神经编码对噪声退化更有弹性。有趣的是,源解析的ERP显示,更多的梯度聆听也与左颞上回的更强的神经反应相关。我们的结果表明,听力策略调节语音和行为成功的分类组织,更连续/梯度的聆听有利于噪声感知中的句子语音。
    Acoustic information in speech changes continuously, yet listeners form discrete perceptual categories to ease the demands of perception. Being a more continuous/gradient as opposed to a more discrete/categorical listener may be further advantageous for understanding speech in noise by increasing perceptual flexibility and resolving ambiguity. The degree to which a listener\'s responses to a continuum of speech sounds are categorical versus continuous can be quantified using visual analog scaling (VAS) during speech labeling tasks. Here, we recorded event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to vowels along an acoustic-phonetic continuum (/u/ to /a/) while listeners categorized phonemes in both clean and noise conditions. Behavior was assessed using standard two alternative forced choice (2AFC) and VAS paradigms to evaluate categorization under task structures that promote discrete vs. continuous hearing, respectively. Behaviorally, identification curves were steeper under 2AFC vs. VAS categorization but were relatively immune to noise, suggesting robust access to abstract, phonetic categories even under signal degradation. Behavioral slopes were correlated with listeners\' QuickSIN scores; shallower slopes corresponded with better speech in noise performance, suggesting a perceptual advantage to noise degraded speech comprehension conferred by a more gradient listening strategy. At the neural level, P2 amplitudes and latencies of the ERPs were modulated by task and noise; VAS responses were larger and showed greater noise-related latency delays than 2AFC responses. More gradient responders had smaller shifts in ERP latency with noise, suggesting their neural encoding of speech was more resilient to noise degradation. Interestingly, source-resolved ERPs showed that more gradient listening was also correlated with stronger neural responses in left superior temporal gyrus. Our results demonstrate that listening strategy modulates the categorical organization of speech and behavioral success, with more continuous/gradient listening being advantageous to sentential speech in noise perception.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对新刺激的定向反应(OR)受制于习惯,即,随着刺激的重复逐渐衰减。已知皮肤电导响应(SCR)表示在外周水平的OR的可靠测量。然而,P3子组件中的哪一个最有可能代表OR的中央对应物仍然是一个争论的问题。当前工作的目的是研究习惯,recovery,以及双刺激听觉怪球范例所固有的令人沮丧的现象,研究和临床中最常用的范例之一,通过同时记录20名健康志愿者的SCR和P3。我们的研究结果表明,目标刺激能够触发更明显的OR,根据SCR和P3的索引,与标准刺激相比,这可能是由于其对完成任务的情感显著性和相关性;将时间主成分分析(PCA)应用于P3复合体使我们能够识别几个子成分,包括早期和晚期P3a(eP3a;lP3a),P3b,新奇P3(nP3),以及正和负慢波(+SW;-SW)。特别是,lP3a和P3b子组件显示出与SCR相似的行为,建议他们作为OR的中心对应物。最后,目标后的第一个标准刺激诱发的P3显示出明显的泄气现象,这可能代表了局部刺激变化的迹象。然而,它没有达到足够的水平来触发SCR/OR,因为它不代表任务上下文中的突出事件。
    The orienting reaction (OR) towards a new stimulus is subject to habituation, i.e., progressively attenuates with stimulus repetition. The skin conductance responses (SCRs) are known to represent a reliable measure of OR at the peripheral level. Yet, it is still a matter of debate which of the P3 subcomponents is the most likely to represent the central counterpart of the OR. The aim of the present work was to study habituation, recovery, and dishabituation phenomena intrinsic to a two-stimulus auditory oddball paradigm, one of the most-used paradigms both in research and clinic, by simultaneously recording SCRs and P3 in twenty healthy volunteers. Our findings show that the target stimulus was capable of triggering a more marked OR, as indexed by both SCRs and P3, compared to the standard stimulus, that could be due to its affective saliency and relevance for task completion; the application of temporal principal components analysis (PCA) to the P3 complex allowed us to identify several subcomponents including both early and late P3a (eP3a; lP3a), P3b, novelty P3 (nP3), and both a positive and a negative Slow Wave (+SW; -SW). Particularly, lP3a and P3b subcomponents showed a similar behavior to that observed for SCRs , suggesting them as central counterparts of OR. Finally, the P3 evoked by the first standard stimulus after the target showed a significant dishabituation phenomenon which could represent a sign of the local stimulus change. However, it did not reach a sufficient level to trigger an SCR/OR since it did not represent a salient event in the context of the task.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在不同的强制选择响应任务中,性能监控已被广泛研究。参与者通常表现出更长的响应时间(RT)和错误后更高的准确性,但是错误相关的事件相关的脑电位(ERP)和行为之间的联系存在不一致,如RT和精度。任何给定研究中的特定任务都可能导致这些不一致,因为不同的任务可能需要不同的认知过程,影响ERP-行为关系。本研究旨在确定任务是否缓和了ERP-行为关系,以及当任务和刺激被视为随机效应时,这些关系是否得到了强有力的观察。ERP和行为指数(RTs,准确性)在侧翼期间记录,Stroop,来自180人的Go/Nogo任务展示了对ERP-行为关系的特定任务影响,这样,更大的先前试验错误相关的负性(ERN)预测了更长的RTs,并且在侧翼和Stroop任务期间而不是在Go/Nogo任务期间对后续试验做出正确反应的可能性更大。此外,在Stroop和Go/Nogo任务的后续试验中,较大的先前试验错误阳性(Pe)预测了较快的RT和较小的RT方差,但对于侧翼任务则没有。当任务和刺激被视为随机效应时,未观察到ERP与行为的关系。这些发现支持在解释研究结果时需要考虑用于记录性能监控措施的任务。
    Performance monitoring has been widely studied during different forced-choice response tasks. Participants typically show longer response times (RTs) and increased accuracy following errors, but there are inconsistencies regarding the connection between error-related event-related brain potentials (ERPs) and behavior, such as RT and accuracy. The specific task in any given study could contribute to these inconsistencies, as different tasks may require distinct cognitive processes that impact ERP-behavior relationships. The present study sought to determine whether task moderates ERP-behavior relationships and whether these relationships are robustly observed when tasks and stimuli are treated as random effects. ERPs and behavioral indices (RTs and accuracy) recorded during flanker, Stroop, and Go/Nogo tasks from 180 people demonstrated a task-specific effect on ERP-behavior relationships, such that larger previous-trial error-related negativity (ERN) predicted longer RTs and greater likelihood of a correct response on subsequent trials during flanker and Stroop tasks but not during Go/Nogo task. Additionally, larger previous-trial error positivity (Pe) predicted faster RTs and smaller variances of RTs on subsequent trials for Stroop and Go/Nogo tasks but not for flanker task. When tasks and stimuli were treated as random effects, ERP-behavior relationships were not observed. These findings support the need to consider the task used for recording performance monitoring measures when interpreting results across studies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    可以快速准确地从多个面部表情中提取平均情绪。然而,人们仍在争论平均情绪处理是否是自动的,这可以在没有注意的情况下发生。为了解决这个问题,我们使用被动怪球模式,并记录事件相关的脑电位,当参与者辨别中心固定的变化,而一组四张面孔出现在外周时.面部包括一个快乐和三个愤怒的表情(平均否定)或一个愤怒和三个快乐的表情(平均肯定),并且在序列中分别以20%(偏差)和80%(标准)的概率显示平均负和平均正人脸集,反之亦然。基于聚类的排列分析表明,视觉失配的负性在大约92ms的早期开始,并且在稍后的时间窗口中也观察到,当平均情绪为负面时,而当平均情绪为阳性时,在约168-266ms观察到错配阳性。结果表明,平均负面和平均正面情绪的处理可能存在不同的机制。更重要的是,大脑可以自动检测平均情绪的变化,和集成编码的多个面部表情可以发生在一个自动的方式没有注意。
    The mean emotion from multiple facial expressions can be extracted rapidly and precisely. However, it remains debated whether mean emotion processing is automatic which can occur under no attention. To address this question, we used a passive oddball paradigm and recorded event-related brain potentials when participants discriminated the changes in the central fixation while a set of four faces was presented in the periphery. The face set consisted of one happy and three angry expressions (mean negative) or one angry and three happy expressions (mean positive), and the mean negative and mean positive face sets were shown with a probability of 20% (deviant) and 80% (standard) respectively in the sequence, or the vice versa. The cluster-based permutation analyses showed that the visual mismatch negativity started early at around 92 ms and was also observed in later time windows when the mean emotion was negative, while a mismatch positivity was observed at around 168-266 ms when the mean emotion was positive. The results suggest that there might be different mechanisms underlying the processing of mean negative and mean positive emotions. More importantly, the brain can detect the changes in the mean emotion automatically, and ensemble coding for multiple facial expressions can occur in an automatic fashion without attention.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    老年人担心自己的认知能力下降,但是在神经心理学测试中没有显示实际认知能力下降的证据,在以后被诊断为痴呆症的风险增加。由于神经标记物可能对认知衰退的早期阶段更敏感,在本研究中,我们检查了事件相关的反馈处理的潜在反应,在概率学习任务中引发的,从社区招募的健康老年人不同,有(主观认知能力下降/SCD组)或未报告(无SCD组)的人担心自己的认知能力下降超过正常的年龄相关发展。在概率学习任务中,在从情感反馈中学习时没有小组差异的情况下,在两组中,反馈相关负性(FRN)的幅度随反馈效价的变化不同:在No-SCD组中,正反馈比负反馈更大,在SCD组中,FRN振幅在正反馈和负反馈之间没有差异。两组的负反馈均增强了P3b,而组间P3b波幅差异均无统计学意义。负反馈与正反馈的神经处理中灵敏度的改变可能是SCD的标志。
    Older adults who worry about their own cognitive capabilities declining, but who do not show evidence of actual cognitive decline in neuropsychological tests, are at an increased risk of being diagnosed with dementia at a later time. Since neural markers may be more sensitive to early stages of cognitive decline, in the present study we examined whether event-related potential responses of feedback processing, elicited in a probabilistic learning task, differ between healthy older adults recruited from the community, who either did (subjective cognitive decline/SCD-group) or did not report (No-SCD group) worry about their own cognition declining beyond the normal age-related development. In the absence of group differences in learning from emotionally charged feedback in the probabilistic learning task, the amplitude of the feedback-related negativity (FRN) varied with feedback valence differently in the two groups: In the No-SCD group, the FRN was larger for positive than negative feedback, while in the SCD group, FRN amplitude did not differ between positive and negative feedback. The P3b was enhanced for negative feedback in both groups, and group differences in P3b amplitude were not significant. Altered sensitivity in neural processing of negative versus positive feedback may be a marker of SCD.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在情感的处理中,大脑准备和反应的独特的方式取决于负面或积极的细微差别的情绪激发。以前的调查表明,阴性诱导子通常比阳性和中性诱导子引起更强烈的神经活动,如事件相关电位(ERP)晚期后验阳性(LPP)复合体的所有子分量的振幅增加所反映的那样,而对厌恶情绪的了解较少。本研究旨在检查在厌恶刺激的处理过程中,LPP复合物是否比其他具有负或正效价的情绪激发者显示出更大的振幅,从而证实了它在早期或后期是厌恶相关的负偏性的神经标记。因此,在本研究中,我们在执行情感自我管理的视觉刺激任务期间利用ERP技术来解开与正面图像相关的神经贡献,负,厌恶,或中性情绪。至关重要的是,我们表明,在自我给药过程中,用厌恶激发子处理引起了最大的神经活动和最大的延迟。总的来说,与所有其他情绪相比,我们证明了与情绪激发者的不愉快和厌恶的特殊处理相关的进行性神经活动。
    In the processing of emotions, the brain prepares and reacts in distinctive manners depending upon the negative or positive nuance of the emotion elicitors. Previous investigations showed that negative elicitors generally evoke more intense neural activities than positive and neutral ones, as reflected in the augmented amplitude of all sub-components of the event-related potentials (ERP) late posterior positivity (LPP) complex, while less is known about the emotion of disgust. The present study aimed to examine whether the LPP complex during the processing of disgust stimuli showed greater amplitude than other emotion elicitors with negative or positive valences, thus confirming it as a neural marker of disgust-related negativity bias at earlier or later stages. Thus, in the present study, we leveraged the ERP technique during the execution of an affective self-administered visual stimuli task to disentangle the neural contributions associated with images of positive, negative, disgust, or neutral emotions. Crucially, we showed that handling with disgust elicitors prompted the greatest neural activity and the highest delay during self-administration. Overall, we demonstrated progressive neural activities associated with the unpleasantness of the emotion elicitors and peculiar processing for disgust compared with all other emotions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    社会回避是指主动不参与社会活动,这不利于新兴成年人健康的人际交往。社会排斥是一种来自他人的负面社会评价,使人们感到社会痛苦。然而,社会回避的新兴成年人在经历社会拒绝后如何处理社会反馈信息,受到的关注较少。本研究旨在探讨在人与人之间的互动背景下,社交回避组(n=16)和比较组(n=16)之间在社交拒绝后的社交互动反馈处理的差异。在两种情况下(社会拒绝和控制条件)接收社交互动反馈时,使用具有两种互动(合作和竞争)的计算机游戏任务来记录与事件相关的电位。结果表明:(1)在接受社会反馈时,社会回避组的奖励积极性幅度低于对照组;(2)社会回避组在社会排斥条件下的P300幅度大于对照组。但对照组没有;(3)社会排斥在社会回避组和对照组中引起更多的负N1振幅。研究结果表明,在人际交往过程中,社交回避的新兴成年人可能在奖励敏感性上存在缺陷,在社会拒绝后,他们也可能会对社会反馈施加更多的注意力和情感资源。
    Social avoidance refers to active non-participation in social activities, which is detrimental to healthy interpersonal interaction for emerging adults. Social rejection is a kind of negative social evaluation from others making people feel social pain. However, how socially avoidant emerging adults process social feedback information after experiencing social rejection has received less attention. The current study aimed to explore the differences in social interaction feedback processing after social rejection between a socially avoidant group (n = 16) and a comparison group (n = 16) in a human-to-human interaction context. Computer game tasks with two types of interaction (cooperation and competition) were used to record the event-related potentials when receiving social interaction feedback in two conditions (social rejection and control condition). The results showed that (1) the socially avoidant group had lower reward positivity amplitudes than the comparison group when receiving social feedback; (2) the socially avoidant group presented larger P300 amplitudes in the social rejection condition than in the control condition, but the comparison group did not; and (3) social rejection evoked more negative N1 amplitudes in the socially avoidant and comparison groups. The findings suggest that socially avoidant emerging adults may have flaws in reward sensitivity during interpersonal interaction, and they might also exert more attentional and emotional resources to social feedback after social rejection.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本章的目的是概述如何在人类成年人中研究诸如音乐中的常规节拍之类的有节奏的时间规律性的感知,人类新生儿,和使用事件相关脑电位(ERP)的非人灵长类动物。首先,我们一般讨论时间结构的不同方面,特别是音乐节奏,我们讨论了规律性感知的潜在机制(例如,节拍)有节奏。此外,我们强调了将节拍感知与节奏中其他类型结构的感知分离的重要性,例如可预测的时间间隔序列,序数结构,和有节奏的分组。在本章的第二节,我们首先讨论由不频繁和频繁的声音引起的听觉ERP:ERP对规律性违规的反应,例如失配消极性(MMN),N2b,和P3,以及对声音的早期感官反应,如P1和N1,已被证明有助于探测节拍感知。随后,我们讨论了如何通过比较ERP对规则和不规则序列中声音的反应来探测节拍感知,通过比较ERP对不同节奏位置的声音的反应,例如在节拍上和节拍下或在强和弱的节拍上。最后,我们将讨论使用上述ERP和范式来研究人类成年人的节拍感知的先前研究,人类新生儿,和非人灵长类动物。在这样做的时候,我们考虑该技术可能存在的缺陷和前景,以及未来的前景。
    The aim of this chapter is to give an overview of how the perception of rhythmic temporal regularity such as a regular beat in music can be studied in human adults, human newborns, and nonhuman primates using event-related brain potentials (ERPs). First, we discuss different aspects of temporal structure in general, and musical rhythm in particular, and we discuss the possible mechanisms underlying the perception of regularity (e.g., a beat) in rhythm. Additionally, we highlight the importance of dissociating beat perception from the perception of other types of structure in rhythm, such as predictable sequences of temporal intervals, ordinal structure, and rhythmic grouping. In the second section of the chapter, we start with a discussion of auditory ERPs elicited by infrequent and frequent sounds: ERP responses to regularity violations, such as mismatch negativity (MMN), N2b, and P3, as well as early sensory responses to sounds, such as P1 and N1, have been shown to be instrumental in probing beat perception. Subsequently, we discuss how beat perception can be probed by comparing ERP responses to sounds in regular and irregular sequences, and by comparing ERP responses to sounds in different metrical positions in a rhythm, such as on and off the beat or on strong and weak beats. Finally, we will discuss previous research that has used the aforementioned ERPs and paradigms to study beat perception in human adults, human newborns, and nonhuman primates. In doing so, we consider the possible pitfalls and prospects of the technique, as well as future perspectives.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    注意力是知觉意识的先决条件还是独立和可分离的过程仍然是一个争论的问题。重要的是,如果不考虑到两者都是异质和多方面机制的事实,理解注意力和意识之间的关系可能是不可能的。因此,本研究测试了波斯纳模型提出的两种注意机制对视觉意识的影响:时间警报和时空定向。具体来说,我们评估了注意力对感知水平的影响,通过测量阈值水平刺激的客观和主观意识;在神经水平上,通过调查注意力如何影响两个假定的与事件相关的潜在意识相关性。我们发现,警报和定向机制可以增加知觉意识,与后者的激活导致最生动的意识。此外,我们发现,晚期阳性不太可能构成意识的神经相关,因为它的振幅是由两种注意机制调制的,但是早期的视觉意识消极独立于警报和定向机制。总之,我们的研究揭示了注意力和意识之间的微妙关系;此外,通过研究警报机制的效果,这项研究提供了对时间注意力在知觉意识中的作用的见解。
    Whether attention is a prerequisite of perceptual awareness or an independent and dissociable process remains a matter of debate. Importantly, understanding the relation between attention and awareness is probably not possible without taking into account the fact that both are heterogeneous and multifaceted mechanisms. Therefore, the present study tested the impact on visual awareness of two attentional mechanisms proposed by the Posner model: temporal alerting and spatio-temporal orienting. Specifically, we evaluated the effects of attention on the perceptual level, by measuring objective and subjective awareness of a threshold-level stimulus; and on the neural level, by investigating how attention affects two postulated event-related potential correlates of awareness. We found that alerting and orienting mechanisms additively facilitate perceptual consciousness, with activation of the latter resulting in the most vivid awareness. Furthermore, we found that late positivity is unlikely to constitute a neural correlate of consciousness as its amplitude was modulated by both attentional mechanisms, but early visual awareness negativity was independent of the alerting and orienting mechanisms. In conclusion, our study reveals a nuanced relationship between attention and awareness; moreover, by investigating the effect of the alerting mechanism, this study provides insights into the role of temporal attention in perceptual consciousness.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号