Dehumanization

非人化
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    非正式的照顾者,为残疾人提供无偿护理工作的人,尽管他们对社会做出了重要贡献,却被贬值了。确定导致其贬值的因素对于认识和评价其工作至关重要。在两项实验研究中,我们研究了(a)非正式照顾者是否非人性化;(b)公正世界(BJW)信念对这一过程的调节作用;(c)BJW和非正式照顾者的非人性化对非正式照顾者痛苦感知的预测影响。在研究1中(N=180),a2(非正式护理人员与非护理人员)X2(女性vs.男性)采用参与者间设计;在研究2(N=205)中,有两个实验条件:女性非正式照顾者与男性非正式照顾者。参与者被随机分配到一个目标的描述,并被要求完成评估目标去人性化的措施(研究1和2),对目标痛苦的感知(研究2),以及衡量BJW的指标(研究2)。结果显示了预期的去人源化效果,因此,与非照顾者相比,参与者将更少的独特人类情感归因于非正式照顾者,无论他们的性别(研究1和2)。然而,这种效应仅在BJW较高的参与者中观察到(研究2).此外,BJW和非正式照顾者的非人性化预测了非正式照顾者痛苦的感知最小化(研究2)。这些结果建立了这些研究领域之间的理论关系,并为实际意义和未来研究提供了见解。
    Informal caregivers, who provide unpaid care work to individuals with disabilities, are devalued despite their important contributions to society. Identifying the factors contributing to their devaluation is crucial for recognizing and valuing their work. In two experimental studies, we examined (a) whether informal caregivers are dehumanized; (b) the moderating impact of belief in a just world (BJW) on this process; and (c) the predictive impact of BJW and the dehumanization of informal caregivers on the perception of informal caregivers\' suffering. In Study 1 (N = 180), a 2 (informal caregiver vs. non-caregiver) X 2 (female vs. male) between-participants design was used; in Study 2 (N = 205), there were two experimental conditions: female informal caregiver vs. male informal caregiver. Participants were randomly assigned to one description of a target and were asked to complete measures assessing the dehumanization of the target (Studies 1 and 2), the perception of the suffering of the target (Study 2), and a measure of BJW referring to themselves (Study 2). Results showed the expected dehumanization effect, such that participants attributed fewer uniquely human emotions to informal caregivers compared to non-caregivers, regardless of their gender (Studies 1 and 2). However, this effect was observed only among participants with higher BJW (Study 2). Furthermore, BJW and the dehumanization of informal caregivers predicted the minimization of the perception of informal caregivers\' suffering (Study 2). These results establish a theoretical relationship between these research areas and offer insights for practical implications and future research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    以前的工作报告说,参与者通过否认罪犯独特的人类性格特征,例如精致,理性和道德预测了对他们认可的惩罚的严重性。我们在六项强大且预先注册的研究中重新审视了这一有影响力的发现。首先,我们在概念上复制了以前工作中报告的效果,证明我们的方法对检测基于特征的去人性化和惩罚之间的关系很敏感。然后,我们研究了基于特征的去人性化和惩罚之间的明显关系是否由整合到刺激集中的特征的可取性驱动,他们感知到的人性,或者两者兼而有之。要做到这一点,我们要求参与者对罪犯拥有独特的人类特征的程度进行评分,这些特征在社会上是可取的(例如文化和文明)或在社会上是不可取的(例如傲慢和痛苦)。相关证据和实验证据都集中在这样的结论上,即基于特征的去人性化和惩罚之间关系的明显证据可以更好地解释为参与者将社会期望的属性归因于罪犯的程度,而不是他们将独特的人类属性归因于罪犯的程度。这些研究对基于特征的去人性化与伤害之间的假设因果关系提出了质疑,至少在这种情况下。
    Previous work has reported that the extent to which participants dehumanized criminals by denying them uniquely human character traits such as refinement, rationality and morality predicted the severity of the punishment endorsed for them. We revisit this influential finding across six highly powered and pre-registered studies. First, we conceptually replicate the effect reported in previous work, demonstrating that our method is sensitive to detecting relationships between trait-based dehumanization and punishment should they occur. We then investigate whether the apparent relationship between trait-based dehumanization and punishment is driven by the desirability of the traits incorporated into the stimulus set, their perceived humanness, or both. To do this, we asked participants to rate the extent to which criminals possessed uniquely human traits that were either socially desirable (e.g. cultured and civilized) or socially undesirable (e.g. arrogant and bitter). Correlational and experimental evidence converge on the conclusion that apparent evidence for the relationship between trait-based dehumanization and punishment is better explained by the extent to which participants attribute socially desirable attributes to criminals rather than the extent to which they attribute uniquely human attributes. These studies cast doubt on the hypothesized causal relationship between trait-based dehumanization and harm, at least in this context.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    基于性别的骚扰是一项紧迫的社会挑战,迫切需要根除。虽然社会运动强调支持受害者,社会反应有时倾向于为肇事者开脱。这项研究考察了影响这种免责的两个因素:与肇事者的亲密关系和同理心。
    共有345名参与者参加了一项实验设计,以评估与肇事者的接近程度(接近与遥远)和同理心焦点(对肇事者和受害者与控制)影响骚扰的道德理由和双方的非人性化。
    结果表明,对肇事者的亲密和同情会导致更大的宽大处理-更多的道德辩护和对肇事者的非人化。对亲密肇事者的道德辩护是通过对他们的同理心增加和对受害者的同理心减少来调节的,减少对亲密肇事者的非人化对应于对他们的高度同情。这项研究强调了亲密和同情,两个最初的积极因素,可以促进对性别骚扰的宽容。
    UNASSIGNED: Gender-based harassment is a pressing social challenge urgently demanding eradication. While social movements emphasize supporting victims, societal responses sometimes lean toward exculpating perpetrators. This study examines two factors influencing this exoneration: closeness to perpetrators and empathy focus.
    UNASSIGNED: A total of 345 participants took part in an experimental design to assess how closeness to perpetrators (close vs. distant) and empathy focus (on the perpetrator vs. victim vs. control) impact the moral justification of harassment and the dehumanization of both parties.
    UNASSIGNED: Results indicate that closeness and empathizing with perpetrators lead to greater leniency-more moral justification and less dehumanization of the perpetrator. Heightened moral justification for close perpetrators is mediated by increased empathy toward them and decreased empathy for victims, and reduced dehumanization of close perpetrators corresponds to heightened empathy toward them. This research highlights how closeness and empathy, two initially positive factors, can foster tolerance toward gender harassment.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我捍卫一种新颖的说法,即对人们进行非自愿绝育(NCS)的错误,特别是在国家赞助的优生学计划曾经在美国盛行的背景下。是什么使NCS的优生实践明显错误,我声称,是它的非人性化核心:它等于把人当作非人类动物对待,从而表达了他们具有动物价值的社会意义。NCS的做法在一定程度上是表面上严重错误的,但至关重要的是,在这些理由上。我考虑并拒绝NCS错误的说法,这些说法没有提及其动画化特征,比如它侵犯了受害者的(生育)自主权,相当于把它们仅仅当作一种手段,对他们造成心理伤害,或者构成对他们人类尊严的侮辱。我的讨论表明,生命伦理学的重要词汇应该扩展到侵犯权利的讨论之外,好处和危害,和平等对待-以及非人性化的语言对生物伦理学家来说是不可或缺的。
    I defend a novel account of the wrong of subjecting people to non-consensual sterilization (NCS), particularly in the context of the state-sponsored eugenics programmes once prevalent in the United States. What makes the eugenicist practice of NCS distinctively wrong, I claim, is its dehumanizing core: the fact that it is tantamount to treating people as nonhuman animals, thereby expressing the degrading social meaning that they have the value of animals. The practice of NCS is prima facie seriously wrong partly, but crucially, on these grounds. I consider and reject accounts of the wrong of NCS that make no reference to its animalizing character, such as that it violates victims\' (procreative) autonomy, amounts to treating them merely as a means, inflicts psychological harm on them, or constitutes an affront to their human dignity. My discussion suggests that the critical vocabulary of bioethics should be expanded beyond talk of rights violations, benefits and harms, and equal treatment-and that the language of dehumanization is indispensable to bioethicists.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    一眼足以将心理属性赋予他人。吸引力与积极的属性(\'美丽是好\'刻板印象)相关。这里,我们提出了一个类似但负面偏见的问题。面部异常的人是否与消极的个人特征有关?我们假设,由于负面的刻板印象(温暖和能力不足)和非人性化的形式(动物性和机械性),会产生对异常面孔的偏见。我们招募了1493名mTurk参与者(排除后N=1306),以使用整形手术前后的60对同一人的照片评估被拍照者的31个特征。一半的异常面孔有疤痕,另一半有麻痹。计算温暖和能力,我们对31个属性进行了主成分分析.通过平均与道德敏感性和理性/逻辑相对应的反向评分来评估动物的非人性化,通过平均对应于情绪反应和人际温暖的反向评分评分,实现机械性的非人性化。我们发现两种异常的面孔都被认为不那么温暖,称职,被非人化。我们的发现表明,无论异常的病因如何,“异常坏”的刻板印象都是普遍的。这种效应可能与反向晕轮效应有关,也就是说,号角效应。
    A glance is enough to assign psychological attributes to others. Attractiveness is associated with positive attributes (\'beauty-is-good\' stereotype). Here, we raise the question of a similar but negative bias. Are people with facial anomalies associated with negative personal characteristics? We hypothesized that biases against faces with anomalies arise because of negative stereotypes (less warmth and competence) and forms of dehumanization (animalistic and mechanistic). We enrolled 1493 mTurk participants (N = 1306 after exclusion) to assess 31 traits of photographed people using 60 pairs of photographs of the same person before and after plastic surgery. Half anomalous faces had a scar and the other half had a palsy. To calculate warmth and competence, we conducted a principal components analysis of the 31 attributes. Animalistic dehumanization was assessed by averaging reverse-scored ratings corresponding to moral sensibility and rationality/logic, and mechanistic dehumanization by averaging across reverse-scored ratings corresponding to emotional responsiveness and interpersonal warmth. We found that both kinds of anomalous faces were seen as less warm, competent and were dehumanized. Our findings suggest that an \'anomalous-is-bad\' stereotype generalizes regardless of the aetiology of the anomaly. This effect may be related to a reverse halo effect, that is, the horn effect.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Editorial
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    社会心理学的研究长期以来一直认为,暴露于对女性的客观描述会导致越来越多的厌恶女性的态度和行为。我们认为,这样的形象也会影响性别化的政策态度。我们建议,客观化的图像主要性别歧视的态度,并减少对女性机构的看法,温暖,和能力。我们认为,这可能会导致对生殖权利和其他性别突出政策的支持减少。此外,这些影响可能因暴露于这些图像的性别而异。在两次短暂暴露于客观化图像的调查实验中,我们发现这些预测的支持不一。尽管我们发现了一些预期的负面影响,我们还在样本中的女性中发现了客观化的积极影响,这些影响暗示了强烈反对效应。我们讨论了这种异质性的潜在解释。总的来说,我们的结果为进一步探讨客观化对政治结果的影响提供了有趣的途径.
    Research in social psychology has long argued that exposure to objectifying portrayals of women can lead to increasingly misogynist attitudes and behavior. We argue that such images can also impact on gendered policy attitudes. We suggest that objectifying images prime sexist attitudes and reduce perceptions of women\'s agency, warmth, and competence. We argue that this may translate into decreased support for reproductive rights and other gender-salient policies. Furthermore, these effects may vary by the gender of those exposed to these images. In two survey experiments with brief exposures to objectifying images, we find mixed support for these predictions. Although we find some negative effects as predicted, we also find positive effects of objectification among women in the sample that are suggestive of a backlash effect. We discuss potential explanations for this heterogeneity. Overall, our results suggest interesting avenues to further explore the effects of objectification on political outcomes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人性感知,将基本和独特的人类特征归因于其他物体或人,对人们的互动有重大影响。值得注意的是,未能感知老年人的人性会导致偏见。这项研究从两个维度调查了目标年龄对人类感知的影响:代理(行为和行为的能力)和经验(感觉和感觉的能力)。我们还使用磁共振成像(MRI)扫描仪检查了大脑活动,以了解潜在的神经机制。健康的大学生查看老年人和年轻人的面部图像,并在MRI扫描仪内部根据机构和经验判断每个人的人性。结果表明,老年人的经验评分更高,并且在年轻和老年面部图像之间的代理商评级中没有发现差异。对大脑成像数据的分析表明,在判断年轻面孔的人性时,内侧前额叶皮层(mPFC)的腹侧和背侧区域之间的正功能连接要比年长面孔更大。我们还发现,与年轻面孔相比,在判断老面孔的人性时,左额下回和中央后回之间的负功能连接更大。尽管目前的研究没有显示出与人类感知相关的独特的大脑活动,这表明不同的大脑连通性可能与对属于不同年龄段的目标的人类感知有关。
    Humanness perception, which attributes fundamental and unique human characteristics to other objects or people, has significant consequences for people\'s interactions. Notably, the failure to perceive humanness in older adults can lead to prejudice. This study investigates the effect of a target\'s age on humanness perception in terms of two dimensions: agency (the ability to act and do) and experience (the ability to feel and sense). We also examined brain activity using a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner in order to understand the underlying neural mechanisms. Healthy university students viewed the facial images of older and younger individuals and judged the humanness of each individual in terms of agency and experience while inside the MRI scanner. The results indicated that older adults were rated higher on experience, and no difference was found in ratings for agency between younger and older face images. Analysis of brain imaging data indicated that positive functional connectivity between the ventral and dorsal regions of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) was greater when judging the humanness of younger faces than older faces. We also found that the negative functional connectivity between the left inferior frontal gyrus and postcentral gyrus was greater when judging the humanness of older faces as compared to that of younger faces. Although the current study did not show distinct brain activities related to humanness perception, it suggests the possibility that different brain connectivities are related to humanness perception regarding targets belonging to different age groups.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    医学面临着许多棘手的现代挑战,这些挑战可以通过超智能化来理解;同情危机,倦怠,非人化,失去了意义。这些挑战源于医学哲学,甚至是通过历史上将人类情感从人类理性中排除出来的一般西方哲学。解决这些医学挑战首先需要一种新颖的人类知识和理性哲学模式,其中包含人类智力和人类情感的平衡互动。这种必然性模式要求将双重过程理论新颖地扩展到认识论中,无论是智力还是情感,都会产生一种独立于其他知识的独特自然知识,以及这两种形式的心理过程如何共同构建人类理性。这里提出了这样一种新颖的哲学图式。然后将该方案应用于医学实践,并提供实际应用的示例,目的是以更知识的方式重新制定医学实践,平衡,健康的方式。这种模式的扩展认识论成为更充分地将人文科学纳入医学的哲学基础。
    Medicine is faced with a number of intractable modern challenges that can be understood in terms of hyper-intellectualization; a compassion crisis, burnout, dehumanization, and lost meaning. These challenges have roots in medical philosophy and indeed general Western philosophy by way of the historic exclusion of human emotion from human reason. The resolution of these medical challenges first requires a novel philosophic schema of human knowledge and reason that incorporates the balanced interaction of human intellect and human emotion. This schema of necessity requires a novel extension of dual-process theory into epistemology in terms of both intellect and emotion each generating a distinct natural kind of knowledge independent of the other as well as how these two forms of mental process together construct human reason. Such a novel philosophic schema is here proposed. This scheme is then applied to the practice of medicine with examples of practical applications with the goal of reformulating medical practice in a more knowledgable, balanced, and healthy way. This schema\'s expanded epistemology becomes the philosophic foundation for more fully incorporating the humanities in medicine.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对象化是一种具有各种负面后果的日常体验。在四项研究中(N=877),我们测试了客观化经验是否以及如何导致亲社会性下降。使用相关设计(研究1和2),我们发现,参与者的客观化经验对他们的亲社会意图有负面影响,而自我去人性化可以解释客观化和亲社会意图之间的负相关。接下来,通过操纵参与者的客观化经验,我们发现客观化对亲社会意图的负面影响,以及自我去人性化的中介作用(研究3和4)。此外,我们测试了自我去人性化与相对剥夺(另一个潜在的中介)相比的中介作用,并一致发现,自我去人性化在解释客观化对亲社会意图的影响方面是一个更强的中介(研究1、2和4)。一起,我们的研究结果支持客观化后的自我非人化过程,并为客观化和亲社会之间的关系提供了新的见解。讨论了研究的意义和局限性。
    Objectification is a daily experience with various negative consequences. In four studies (N = 877), we tested whether and how objectification experience contributes to decreased prosociality. Using correlational designs (Studies 1 and 2), we found that participants\' objectification experience negatively predicted their prosocial intention and that self-dehumanization could account for the negative association between objectification and prosocial intention. Next, by manipulating participants\' objectification experience, we found the negative effect of objectification on prosocial intention, as well as the mediating role of self-dehumanization (Studies 3 and 4). Additionally, we tested the mediating role of self-dehumanization in comparison with relative deprivation (another potential mediator), and consistently found that self-dehumanization was a stronger mediator in accounting for the effect of objectification on prosocial intention (Studies 1, 2, and 4). Together, our findings support the process of self-dehumanization following objectification and offer new insights into the relationship between objectification and prosociality. The implications and limitations of the research were discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号