Color Perception

颜色感知
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人类表现出一致的颜色偏好,通常被描述为跨色调的曲线图案。最近的文献认为颜色偏好与对象或与这些颜色相关联的其他实体的偏好相关联。然而,许多研究使用隔离光颜色来检验这种偏好,它不能反映通常受不同光强度影响的自然观看体验。在彩色刺激中包括随机亮度水平(亮度噪声)可以提供评估在现实世界中呈现的颜色偏好的初始步骤。采用马赛克刺激,本研究旨在评估亮度噪声对人类颜色偏好的影响。30个正常的三色视者参与了两种选择的强迫选择范式,指示所呈现的对之间的颜色偏好。色彩刺激包括8种标准色调的饱和版本,在不同亮度噪声条件下以不同直径的马赛克形式呈现。结果表明,包含亮度噪声增加了所有色调的颜色偏好,特别是在高亮度噪声范围下,而曲线模式保持不变。最后,女性对亮度噪声的存在比男性更敏感,可能是由于男女在审美评价策略上的差异。
    Humans exhibit consistent color preferences that are often described as a curvilinear pattern across hues. The recent literature posits that color preference is linked to the preference for objects or other entities associated with those colors. However, many studies examine this preference using isoluminant colors, which don\'t reflect the natural viewing experience typically influenced by different light intensities. The inclusion of random luminance levels (luminance noise) in chromatic stimuli may provide an initial step towards assessing color preference as it is presented in the real world. Employing mosaic stimuli, this study aimed to evaluate the influence of luminance noise on human color preference. Thirty normal trichromats engaged in a two-alternative forced-choice paradigm, indicating their color preferences between presented pairs. The chromatic stimuli included saturated versions of 8 standard hues, presented in mosaics with varying diameters under different luminance noise conditions. Results indicated that the inclusion of luminance noise increased color preference across all hues, specifically under the high luminance noise range, while the curvilinear pattern remained unchanged. Finally, women exhibit a greater sensitivity to the presence of luminance noise than men, potentially due to differences between men and women in aesthetic evaluation strategies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的是比较用于直接修复的单色调和多色调树脂基复合材料(RBC)的颜色匹配和颜色稳定性。本综述是根据系统评价和荟萃分析指南的首选报告项目进行的。包括评估单阴影红细胞在直接修复体中的阴影性能的随机临床试验。在五个数据库中搜索科学文献(2024年4月)。使用RevMan5.4进行荟萃分析,使用随机效应模型计算二分结果的风险差异(RD)和95%置信区间(CI)。使用RoB2.0工具评估偏差,并使用GRADepro工具评估证据的确定性。选择了四项研究,分析了263个修复体。结果显示在12个月内,单色调RBC和多色调RBC之间在颜色匹配和颜色稳定性方面具有相当的性能。三项研究的偏倚风险较低,所有预期结果,一项研究有一些担忧。由于事件数量和样本量少,所有随访期的颜色稳定性证据的确定性都被认为是低的。根据美国公共卫生服务评估(USPHS)和世界牙科联合会(FDI),在12个月内,单色调和多色调红细胞的临床颜色表现相当.
    The objective was to compare the color match and color stability behavior of single- and multi-shade resin-based composites (RBCs) used for direct restorations. This review was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines. Randomized clinical trials evaluating the shade performance of single-shade RBCs in direct restorations were included. A search of the scientific literature was performed in five databases (April 2024). The meta-analysis was performed using RevMan 5.4, calculating the risk difference (RD) and 95% confidence interval (CI) of the dichotomous outcome using a random effects model. Bias was assessed using the RoB 2.0 tool, and certainty of evidence was assessed using the GRADEpro tool. Four studies were selected, with 263 restorations analyzed. The results showed comparable performance between single-shade RBCs and multi-shade RBCs in terms of color match and color stability over 12 months. Three studies had a low risk of bias with all expected results, and one study had some concerns. The certainty of evidence for color stability was considered low for all follow-up periods due to the small number of events and sample size. According to the United States Public Health Service Evaluation (USPHS) and the World Dental Federation (FDI), there is comparable clinical color performance between single-shade and multi-shade RBCs over 12 months.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    窗户提供日光和户外景观,影响建筑设计。各种玻璃和窗户遮阳材料用于减轻眩光,过热和隐私问题,它们会影响视野的清晰度。其中,我们评估了窗膜的效果,电致变色(EC)玻璃,和织物色调对视野的清晰度。我们对50名参与者进行了一项实验,使用根据临床视力测试改编的视觉测试(视敏度,对比敏感度,色彩敏感度)和在受控实验室的计算机显示器上显示的图像。窗膜和EC玻璃色调在视敏度方面优于织物色调,对比敏感度和视图满意度,除了最暗的EC色调状态和深灰色VLT3%的颜色敏感度和视图满意度。EC色调造成内部反射问题,织物色调是视觉隐私的首选。窗膜和EC玻璃阻碍了参与者的蓝绿色辨别,而织物阴影也降低了红黄色辨别。视敏度可预测视图满意度,对比敏感度是视觉隐私的最强预测指标。一般来说,较高的可见光透射率和较低的太阳反射率(较深的颜色)增强了人类的视觉性能。拟议的工作流程提供了一个实验程序,确定主要变量并建立用于评估开窗视图清晰度的预测框架。
    Windows provide access to daylight and outdoor views, influencing building design. Various glazing and window shade materials are used to mitigate glare, overheating and privacy issues, and they affect view clarity. Among them, we evaluated the effect of window films, electrochromic (EC) glass, and fabric shades on view clarity. We conducted an experiment with 50 participants using visual tests adapted from clinical vision tests (visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, color sensitivity) and images displayed on a computer monitor in a controlled laboratory. Window films and EC glass tints outperformed fabric shades in visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and view satisfaction with the exception of the darkest EC tint state and dark grey VLT 3% shade for color sensitivity and view satisfaction. The EC tints pose internal reflection issues and fabric shades are preferred for visual privacy. Window films and EC glass hinder participants\' blue-green color discrimination while fabric shades also decrease red-yellow color discrimination. Visual acuity predicts view satisfaction and contrast sensitivity is the strongest predictor for visual privacy. Generally, higher visible light transmittance and lower solar reflectance (darker color) enhance human visual performance. The proposed workflow provides an experimental procedure, identifies the primary variables and establishes a predictive framework for assessing view clarity of fenestration.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    将颜色感知为与任务相关的刺激会影响侧翼任务中的认知和行为;然而,当它是与任务无关的刺激维度时,它是否具有相同的影响尚不清楚。为此,我们对23名健康的年轻人应用了有或没有彩色(红色/蓝色)的四个字母的侧翼任务,同时记录事件相关电位(ERPs)和行为表现。侧翼任务包括四种颜色类型:非彩色字母(NC),所有颜色字母(AC),侧翼颜色字母(FC),和目标颜色字母(TC),每个侧翼任务包括一致和不一致的条件。行为数据证明了在反应时间(RTs)和准确性方面,所有颜色类型的侧翼任务的经典冲突效应。仅在准确性上观察到颜色类型因素的显着交互作用和主要影响。ERP结果表明,冲突因素(一致,不一致)和颜色类型(NC,AC,FC,和TC),颜色类型因子增强了正面中心P2(180-200毫秒),下降前中心顶叶N2b(260-320毫秒),并增加了前中央P3b(360-520ms)。TC的正面中心P2和正面中心P3b比NC大,AC,和FC在一致的条件下,虽然NC的正面中心P3b比AC小,FC,和TC处于不一致的状态。此外,NC中前顶叶N2b依次下降,AC,FC,和TC在一致和不一致的条件下。总的来说,我们的研究结果表明,与任务无关的颜色刺激维度可以捕获一些注意力资源,并且受到侧翼任务中颜色位置(目标/侧翼)和任务试验类型(全等/不全等)的影响.
    Perception of color as a task-relevant stimulus can affect cognition and behavior in the flanker task; however, it remains unclear whether it has the same impact when it is a task-irrelevant stimulus dimension. To this end, we applied four-letter flanker tasks with or without colored (red/blue) to 23 healthy young adults, while recording the event-related potentials (ERPs) and behavioral performance. The flanker task included four kinds of color types: non-color letter (NC), all color letter (AC), flanker color letter (FC), and target color letter (TC), each flanker task included congruent and incongruent conditions. The behavioral data demonstrated the classic conflict effect across all color types of flanker tasks in both reaction times (RTs) and accuracy, the significant interaction and main effect of color type factors were only observed in accuracy. The ERP results showed significant interaction between conflict factor (congruent, incongruent) and color type (NC, AC, FC, and TC), and the color type factor enhanced the fronto-central P2 (180-200 ms), descended the fronto-centro-parietal N2b (260-320 ms), and increased the fronto-central P3b (360-520 ms). The fronto-central P2 and the fronto-central P3b were larger for TC than NC, AC, and FC in the congruent condition, while the fronto-central P3b was smaller for NC than AC, FC, and TC in the incongruent condition. Furthermore, the fronto-centro-parietal N2b was decreased successively in NC, AC, FC, and TC in both congruent and incongruent conditions. Overall, our findings suggested that the task-irrelevant stimuli dimension of color can capture some attentional resources and is affected by the location of color (target/flanker) and the type of task trial (congruent/incongruent) in the flanker task.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:为了评估牙齿阴影的影响,树脂复合材料的类型和空腔配置对单阴影和组阴影树脂复合材料的颜色调节。
    方法:在具有不同阴影的人造丙烯酸下颌右磨牙中制备I类和V类腔-A1,A2,A3,A3.5和A4。三个单阴影(OMN,UNI,CHA)和两组灯罩(FIL,对HAR)树脂复合材料进行了评价。七十五名观察员,包括牙医,牙科学生和外行人(n=25),参与研究。基于视觉评估的每个牙齿和每个修复之间的颜色匹配的心理物理实验从0(优秀匹配)到4(巨大的不匹配),并计算平均频率(%)。复合材料和牙色之间的视觉颜色差异有统计学意义(P<0.05)。
    结果:结果显示,与I类修复相比,V类无法接受的颜色不匹配百分比最高,特别是更多的彩色牙齿色调(A3.5和A4)。外行人组的可接受性评分百分比较高。对于V类,OMN仅在牙齿阴影A1,A2,A3和A4方面与UNI存在显着差异。然而,对于I类,对于A1,A2和A4,OMN与CHA存在显着差异,对于A1,OMN与HAR和FIL存在显着差异。
    结论:I类树脂复合修复体表现出比V类更好的颜色匹配,具有更亮和低色度的牙齿色调,显示更好的颜色调整。对于单阴影和组阴影树脂复合材料,两种腔体配置的颜色匹配均相当。
    结论:腔结构和牙齿色调可能会影响单色调和组色调树脂复合材料的颜色匹配。这项研究表明,对于单和组阴影树脂复合材料,I类配置的颜色匹配优于V类。此外,放置在具有较浅色调的牙齿中的修复体显示出更有利的颜色调整。
    OBJECTIVE: To evaluate the influence of the tooth shade, type of resin composite and cavity configuration on the color adjustment of single-shade and group shade resin composites.
    METHODS: Class I and V cavities were prepared in artificial acrylic mandibular right molars with different shades - A1, A2, A3, A3.5 and A4. Three single-shade (OMN, UNI, CHA) and two group shade (FIL, HAR) resin composites were evaluated. Seventy-five observers, including dentists, dental students and laypersons (n = 25), participated in the study. A psychophysical experiment based on visual assessments of the color matching between each tooth and each restoration was ranked from 0 (excellent match) to 4 (huge mismatch), and mean frequencies (%) were calculated. Visual color differences among composite materials and tooth shades were statistically tested (P < 0.05).
    RESULTS: The results showed highest percentages of unacceptable color mismatches for class V compared to class I restorations, especially for more chromatic tooth shades (A3.5 and A4). The percentage of ratings of acceptability was higher for laypersons group. For class V, OMN presented significant difference only from UNI in tooth shades A1, A2, A3 and A4. However, for class I, OMN presented significant differences with CHA for A1, A2 and A4, and with HAR and FIL for A1.
    CONCLUSIONS: Class I resin composite restorations exhibit superior color matching than Class V, with lighter and low chroma tooth shades demonstrating better color adjustment. The color matching of both cavity configurations is comparable for single and group shade resin composites.
    CONCLUSIONS: Cavity configuration and tooth shade may impact color matching of single and group shade resin composites. This study demonstrates superior color matching in class I configurations over class V for both single and group shade resin composites. Additionally, restorations placed in teeth with lighter shades exhibit more favorable color adjustment.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    全报告工作记忆任务为试验中的所有刺激提供了回忆的量度,并提供了单报告延迟估计任务无法进行的单试验分析。然而,大多数全报告研究假设试验刺激是独立编码和报告的,他们不考虑在同一试验中呈现和报告的刺激之间的关系.这里,我们提供了两个独立进行的完整报告实验的结果.第一个数据集是亚当记录的,沃格尔,和Awh(2017),并要求参与者使用连续反应轮报告颜色和方向刺激。我们记录了第二个数据集,这要求参与者使用一组离散按钮报告颜色刺激。我们发现参与者通常按颜色相似性对他们的报告进行分组,与大多数工作记忆编码模型中隐含的独立性假设相矛盾。接下来,我们表明,当报告颜色而不是方向时,这种行为在参与者和实验中是一致的,两个循环变量通常被认为是等价的。最后,我们实现了独立编码的替代方法,其中刺激被编码为分层贝叶斯集成,并且发现该模型可以预测在任何一个数据集中都不存在的偏差。我们的结果表明,在单报告延迟估计任务的背景下开发的独立和分层集成编码模型所做的假设不适用于整个报告任务。这种概括的失败凸显了在推断视觉工作记忆的基本原理时需要考虑任务结构的变化。
    Whole-report working memory tasks provide a measure of recall for all stimuli in a trial and afford single-trial analyses that are not possible with single-report delayed estimation tasks. However, most whole-report studies assume that trial stimuli are encoded and reported independently, and they do not consider the relationships between stimuli presented and reported within the same trial. Here, we present the results of two independently conducted whole-report experiments. The first dataset was recorded by Adam, Vogel, and Awh (2017) and required participants to report color and orientation stimuli using a continuous response wheel. We recorded the second dataset, which required participants to report color stimuli using a set of discrete buttons. We found that participants often group their reports by color similarity, contradicting the assumption of independence implicit in most encoding models of working memory. Next, we showed that this behavior was consistent across participants and experiments when reporting color but not orientation, two circular variables often assumed to be equivalent.Finally, we implemented an alternative to independent encoding where stimuli are encoded as a hierarchical Bayesian ensemble and found that this model predicts biases that are not present in either dataset. Our results suggest that assumptions made by both independent and hierarchical ensemble encoding models-which were developed in the context of single-report delayed estimation tasks-do not hold for the whole-report task. This failure to generalize highlights the need to consider variations in task structure when inferring fundamental principles of visual working memory.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    伽玛节律(30-80Hz)在视觉处理中的作用存在争议;像光栅和色调块这样的刺激会产生强烈的伽玛,但许多自然图像没有。可以通过将图像近似为光栅或色调块来预测图像伽马响应吗?令人惊讶的是,这个问题仍然没有答案,因为对伽马对多个特征的联合依赖性知之甚少。我们记录了两只雌性猴子的局部场电位和皮质电图,同时呈现了沿多个特征维度变化的自然图像和参数刺激。对不同光栅/色调特征的伽马响应是可分离的,允许基于单个特征的乘法模型。通过在感受域周围的图像上拟合色调补丁,这个简单的模型可以以相当高的精度预测跨尺度的彩色图像的伽马响应。我们的结果提供了一个简单的“基线”模型来从局部图像属性预测伽马,可以测试更复杂的自然视觉模型。
    The role of gamma rhythm (30-80 Hz) in visual processing is debated; stimuli like gratings and hue patches generate strong gamma, but many natural images do not. Could image gamma responses be predicted by approximating images as gratings or hue patches? Surprisingly, this question remains unanswered, since the joint dependence of gamma on multiple features is poorly understood. We recorded local field potentials and electrocorticogram from two female monkeys while presenting natural images and parametric stimuli varying along several feature dimensions. Gamma responses to different grating/hue features were separable, allowing for a multiplicative model based on individual features. By fitting a hue patch to the image around the receptive field, this simple model could predict gamma responses to chromatic images across scales with reasonably high accuracy. Our results provide a simple \"baseline\" model to predict gamma from local image properties, against which more complex models of natural vision can be tested.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们研究了在幼稚的Cotesiavestalis中交叉模式效应的可能性,小菜蛾幼虫的类寄生虫黄蜂,通过使用四种颜色的人造花模型(蓝色,绿色,黄色,和红色)在不存在或存在从芸苔花序中收集的花香的情况下。在四选择测试中,不管花香,非饥饿的雌性黄蜂访问绿色和黄色模型的频率明显高于蓝色和红色模型,尽管在访问绿色和黄色模型之间没有观察到显着差异。他们很少参观蓝色和红色模型。当挨饿时,黄蜂变得更加特别,参观黄色明显比绿色模特更频繁,不管花香的存在,这表明他们更喜欢在食物搜索中使用黄色视觉线索。此外,方差分析显示,模型颜色和花香之间的相互作用对黄蜂对花模型的访问有显着影响。花香引起饥饿和非饥饿的黄蜂访问黄色和绿色模型的频率大约是没有气味的两倍。嗅觉感知对黄蜂使用色度信息的跨模式影响可能使它们能够有效地搜索食物来源。
    We examined the possibility of a cross-modal effect in naïve Cotesia vestalis, a parasitoid wasp of diamondback moth larvae, by using artificial flower models of four colours (blue, green, yellow, and red) in the absence or presence of floral scent collected from Brassica rapa inflorescences. In a four-choice test, regardless of the floral scent, non-starved female wasps visited green and yellow models significantly more often than blue and red ones, although no significant difference was observed between visits to the green and yellow models. They seldom visited blue and red models. When starved, the wasps became even more particular, visiting yellow significantly more frequently than green models, irrespective of the presence of the floral scent, indicating that they preferred to use yellow visual cues in their food search. Furthermore, a factorial analysis of variance revealed a significant effect of the interaction between model colour and floral scent on the wasps\' visits to flower models. The floral scent induced starved and non-starved wasps to visit yellow and green models about twice as often as without the scent. A cross-modal effect of olfactory perception on the use of chromatic information by wasps may allow them to search efficiently for food sources.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在人类中,瞳孔对视网膜感觉到的物理光和大脑产生的光的心理表征都有反应。值得注意的是,当视觉刺激虚幻地感觉到明亮时,我们的学生就会收缩,即使视网膜照明是恒定的。然而,目前尚不清楚这种瞳孔反应的感知穿透力是否是人类特有的附加现象,或者它是否代表了与其他动物共享的适应性机制,以预测连续眼睛固定之间视网膜照明的变化.为了解决这个问题,我们测量了暴露于三种彩色版本(青色,洋红色,和黄色)的朝日亮度错觉。我们发现,刺激虚幻地感觉到更亮或更暗触发了猕猴和人类参与者非常相似的差异瞳孔反应。此外,我们表明,这种现象在两种灵长类动物中都表现出类似的青色偏向。除了证明猕猴是研究瞳孔反应的感知穿透力的相关模型外,我们的研究结果表明,这种现象是调整到生态条件,因为暴露于一个\"明亮的青色-蓝色的天空\"可能会增加风险的眩目和视网膜损伤。
    In humans, the eye pupils respond to both physical light sensed by the retina and mental representations of light produced by the brain. Notably, our pupils constrict when a visual stimulus is illusorily perceived brighter, even if retinal illumination is constant. However, it remains unclear whether such perceptual penetrability of pupil responses is an epiphenomenon unique to humans or whether it represents an adaptive mechanism shared with other animals to anticipate variations in retinal illumination between successive eye fixations. To address this issue, we measured the pupil responses of both humans and macaque monkeys exposed to three chromatic versions (cyan, magenta, and yellow) of the Asahi brightness illusion. We found that the stimuli illusorily perceived brighter or darker trigger differential pupil responses that are very similar in macaques and human participants. Additionally, we show that this phenomenon exhibits an analogous cyan bias in both primate species. Beyond evincing the macaque monkey as a relevant model to study the perceptual penetrability of pupil responses, our results suggest that this phenomenon is tuned to ecological conditions because the exposure to a \"bright cyan-bluish sky\" may be associated with increased risks of dazzle and retinal damages.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    结论:对FarnsworthD15测试施加时间限制可能会防止患者损害测试。
    目的:本研究旨在研究测试时间对未实践和实践受试者的FarnsworthD15色觉测试的影响,并确定最佳测试时间。
    方法:21名受试者(平均/标准差年龄,33.1/9.3岁)的一系列先天性色觉缺陷参与了研究。假等色平板筛选,为了分类的目的,进行了FarnsworthD15和异形镜测试。每次两次访问,在30秒至10分钟的测试时间范围内进行了10次FarnsworthD15试验。在访问之间,受试者练习测试。主要交叉被用作结果测量。重复测量方差分析比较了各试验的分数。事后Dunnett的测试分析了成对数据。
    结果:尽管在首次就诊的10项试验中发现主要交叉的平均数量没有显着差异(F(9,180)=1.30,p=0.24),第二次访视有显著差异(F(9,180)=4.77,p<0.001).第二次访问的主要交叉的平均数量范围为1.71至5.1,30秒的试验导致主要交叉的数量最多,最长的试验导致主要交叉的数量最少。分析显示,2分钟的时间限制导致了FarnsworthD15结果,这是根据大多数受试者的测验仪预期的。
    结论:在这项研究中,发现测试时间会影响实践受试者的表现,但不会影响未实践受试者的表现。基于这项研究,我们建议执行2分钟的时间限制,以阻止那些试图通过FarnsworthD15通过练习的人。其他措施,比如记录病人的行为,也可以采取。
    CONCLUSIONS: Imposing a time limit on the Farnsworth D15 test may prevent patients from compromising the test.
    OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to investigate the effect of test time on the Farnsworth D15 color vision test in unpracticed and practiced subjects and determine an optimal test time.
    METHODS: Twenty-one subjects (mean/standard deviation age, 33.1/9.3 years) with a range of congenital color vision deficiency participated in the study. Pseudoisochromatic plate screening, Farnsworth D15, and anomaloscope testing were performed for classification purposes. At each of 2 visits, 10 trials of the Farnsworth D15 were performed with a range in test times from 30 seconds to 10 minutes. Between visits, subjects practiced the test. Major crossovers were used as the outcome measure. A repeated-measures analysis of variance compared the scores across trials. Post hoc Dunnett\'s testing analyzed the pairwise data.
    RESULTS: Although no significant difference in the mean number of major crossovers was found across the 10 trials for the first visit ( F (9, 180) = 1.30, p=0.24), a significant difference was found for the second visit ( F (9, 180) = 4.77, p<0.001). The range of mean number of major crossovers for the second visit was 1.71 to 5.1, with the 30-second trial resulting in the largest number of major crossovers and the longest trial resulting in the smallest number of major crossovers. Analysis showed that a 2-minute time limit resulted in a Farnsworth D15 outcome that would be expected based on the anomaloscope for a majority of subjects.
    CONCLUSIONS: In this study, test time was found to affect performance in practiced subjects but not in unpracticed subjects. Based on this study, we recommend enforcing a time limit of 2 minutes to discourage those who try to pass the Farnsworth D15 through practice. Additional measures, such as recording patient behavior, can also be taken.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号