speech production

【语音制作】
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在整个动物王国,听觉皮层的神经反应在发声过程中受到抑制,人类也不例外。一个常见的假设是抑制增加了对听觉反馈的敏感性,能够检测发声错误。这一假设先前已在非人灵长类动物中得到证实,然而,听觉抑制和人类语音监测灵敏度之间的直接联系仍然难以捉摸。为了解决这个问题,我们获得了35名神经外科参与者在演讲过程中的颅内脑电图(iEEG)记录.我们首先描述了听觉抑制的详细地形,在上颞回(STG)中变化。接下来,我们执行了延迟听觉反馈(DAF)任务,以确定抑制部位是否也对听觉反馈改变敏感.的确,重叠的地点对反馈的反应增强,表明灵敏度。重要的是,听觉抑制程度和反馈灵敏度之间有很强的相关性,暗示抑制可能是语音监控的关键机制。Further,我们发现,当参与者产生同时听觉反馈的语音时,如果参与者参与DAF范式,则有选择地激活后STG,提示注意负荷的增加可以调节听觉反馈的灵敏度。
    大脑降低了对我们自己产生的输入的反应,如移动或说话。本质上,当我们执行这些动作时,我们的大脑“知道”接下来会发生什么,因此不需要对意外事件做出强烈的反应。这就是为什么我们不能挠自己,以及为什么大脑对我们自己的声音的反应不如对别人的声音的反应。安静大脑的反应也使我们能够专注于新事物或重要事物,而不会被自己的动作或声音分散注意力。对非人类灵长类动物的研究表明,当动物发出声音时,听觉皮层(负责处理声音的大脑区域)中的神经元显示出抑制的活动水平。有趣的是,当灵长类动物听到自己声音的改变版本时,许多相同的神经元变得更加活跃。但目前还不清楚这是否也发生在人类身上。为了调查,Ozker等人.使用一种叫做脑电图的技术来记录参与者说话时人脑不同区域的神经活动。结果表明,当个体说话时,参与听觉处理的大脑大多数区域的活动受到抑制。然而,当人们听到自己声音的改变版本时,出现了意想不到的延迟,这些相同的区域显示出更多的活动。此外,Ozker等人.发现听觉皮层的抑制水平越高,这些领域对一个人说话的变化越敏感。这些发现表明,抑制大脑对自我生成语音的反应可能有助于检测语音生成过程中的错误。言语障碍在各种神经系统疾病中很常见,比如口吃,帕金森病,和失语症.Ozker等人.假设这些缺陷可能是因为个体无法抑制大脑听觉区域的活动,在检测和纠正自己讲话中的错误时引起了斗争。然而,需要进一步的实验来检验这一理论。
    Across the animal kingdom, neural responses in the auditory cortex are suppressed during vocalization, and humans are no exception. A common hypothesis is that suppression increases sensitivity to auditory feedback, enabling the detection of vocalization errors. This hypothesis has been previously confirmed in non-human primates, however a direct link between auditory suppression and sensitivity in human speech monitoring remains elusive. To address this issue, we obtained intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG) recordings from 35 neurosurgical participants during speech production. We first characterized the detailed topography of auditory suppression, which varied across superior temporal gyrus (STG). Next, we performed a delayed auditory feedback (DAF) task to determine whether the suppressed sites were also sensitive to auditory feedback alterations. Indeed, overlapping sites showed enhanced responses to feedback, indicating sensitivity. Importantly, there was a strong correlation between the degree of auditory suppression and feedback sensitivity, suggesting suppression might be a key mechanism that underlies speech monitoring. Further, we found that when participants produced speech with simultaneous auditory feedback, posterior STG was selectively activated if participants were engaged in a DAF paradigm, suggesting that increased attentional load can modulate auditory feedback sensitivity.
    The brain lowers its response to inputs we generate ourselves, such as moving or speaking. Essentially, our brain ‘knows’ what will happen next when we carry out these actions, and therefore does not need to react as strongly as it would to unexpected events. This is why we cannot tickle ourselves, and why the brain does not react as much to our own voice as it does to someone else’s. Quieting down the brain’s response also allows us to focus on things that are new or important without getting distracted by our own movements or sounds. Studies in non-human primates showed that neurons in the auditory cortex (the region of the brain responsible for processing sound) displayed suppressed levels of activity when the animals made sounds. Interestingly, when the primates heard an altered version of their own voice, many of these same neurons became more active. But it was unclear whether this also happens in humans. To investigate, Ozker et al. used a technique called electrocorticography to record neural activity in different regions of the human brain while participants spoke. The results showed that most areas of the brain involved in auditory processing showed suppressed activity when individuals were speaking. However, when people heard an altered version of their own voice which had an unexpected delay, those same areas displayed increased activity. In addition, Ozker et al. found that the higher the level of suppression in the auditory cortex, the more sensitive these areas were to changes in a person’s speech. These findings suggest that suppressing the brain’s response to self-generated speech may help in detecting errors during speech production. Speech deficits are common in various neurological disorders, such as stuttering, Parkinson’s disease, and aphasia. Ozker et al. hypothesize that these deficits may arise because individuals fail to suppress activity in auditory regions of the brain, causing a struggle when detecting and correcting errors in their own speech. However, further experiments are needed to test this theory.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究扩展了以前的研究报告,即正字法形式,例如使用单个字母或两个字母来表示相同的声音,影响第二语言(L2)制作中的声音持续时间。母语(L1)韩语L2英语顺序双语者对包含用一个或两个字母拼写的相同辅音或元音的单词对执行了延迟重复任务。韩语提供了一个有趣的案例,因为(1)它具有字母拼字系统,但没有罗马字母,因此,可能没有字间干扰,并且(2)它没有元音的音素长度对比度,尽管在辅音长度的对比上存在一些分歧,这可能导致元音和辅音之间的字素到音素对应不对称。结果表明,韩国学习者用单个字母拼写时产生的英语元音持续时间短,用双字母或由两个不同字母组成的有向符拼写时产生的元音持续时间长;当产生用单个或两个字母拼写的英语辅音时,这种持续时间的变化不会出现。这项研究进一步研究了抑制性控制的个体差异是否会影响韩国学习者产生英语元音的拼音效果。抑制性控制的个体差异与正字法对元音产生的影响无关。
    The present study extends previous research reporting that orthographic forms, such as the use of a single letter or two letters to indicate the same sound, affect sound duration in second-language (L2) production. Native-language (L1) Korean L2 English sequential bilinguals performed a delayed repetition task for word pairs containing the same consonant or vowel spelled with one or two letters. Korean provided an interesting case because (1) it has an alphabetic orthographic system but not a Roman alphabet and thus, there may be no interorthographic interference and (2) it has no phonemic length contrast for vowels, whereas there is some disagreement on the contrastiveness of the consonant length, which can lead to an asymmetry in the grapheme-to-phoneme correspondence between vowels and consonants. The results showed that Korean learners produced the same English vowel with a short duration when spelled with a single letter and with a long duration when spelled with double letters or digraphs composed of two different letters; this variation in duration did not appear when producing English consonants spelled with a single or two letters. This study further examined whether individual differences in inhibitory control influenced the magnitude of orthographic effects in the production of English vowels by Korean learners. Individual differences in inhibitory control were not strongly related to the influence of orthography on vowel production.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    过去的研究探索了共振峰中心,在对假定的目标元音共振峰的话语持续时间内,收敛的纠正行为。在这项研究中,我们建立了在健康的老年人对照中,音高存在类似的居中现象,并研究了在阿尔茨海默病(AD)中这种矫正行为是如何改变的。我们发现,在校正低于和高于目标(中位数)音高的音高误差时,健康老年人的音高居中响应相似。相比之下,AD患者表现出不对称性,对低于目标发声的基音误差的校正比高于目标发声的基音误差的校正更大.这些发现表明,音高居中是人类语音中的一种稳健补偿行为。我们的发现还探讨了影响AD语言的神经退行性过程对音高居中的潜在影响。
    Past studies have explored formant centering, a corrective behavior of convergence over the duration of an utterance toward the formants of a putative target vowel. In this study, we establish the existence of a similar centering phenomenon for pitch in healthy elderly controls and examine how such corrective behavior is altered in Alzheimer\'s Disease (AD). We found the pitch centering response in healthy elderly was similar when correcting pitch errors below and above the target (median) pitch. In contrast, patients with AD showed an asymmetry with a larger correction for the pitch errors below the target phonation than above the target phonation. These findings indicate that pitch centering is a robust compensation behavior in human speech. Our findings also explore the potential impacts on pitch centering from neurodegenerative processes impacting speech in AD.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人们普遍认为,言语感知和言语产生是紧密相连的,每种模式都可以帮助学习其他模式。这种积极的关系通常被概括为感知和生产是“同一枚硬币的两面”。\"有,的确,许多情况表明了这种相互支持的关系。然而,越来越多的研究显示出非常不同的结果,在模式独立运行的情况下,甚至是彼此对立。我们回顾了现在相当大的文献,这些文献证明了语音产生对语音感知学习的负面影响,在多个级别(特别是在词法和副词法级别)。通过将显示这种模式的情况与发生更多积极互动的情况进行比较,我们提供了为什么发现不同结果的初步说明,确定导致生产对感知的正面或负面影响的因素。审查澄清了这两种模式之间存在的复杂关系:它们确实是联系在一起的,但是他们的关系比他们是同一枚硬币的两面所暗示的要复杂得多。
    A widely held belief is that speech perception and speech production are tightly linked, with each modality available to help with learning in the other modality. This positive relationship is often summarized as perception and production being \"two sides of the same coin.\" There are, indeed, many situations that have shown this mutually supportive relationship. However, there is a growing body of research showing very different results, with the modalities operating independently, or even in opposition to each other. We review the now-sizeable literature demonstrating the negative effect that speech production can have on perceptual learning of speech, at multiple levels (particularly at the lexical and sublexical levels). By comparing the situations that show this pattern with ones in which more positive interactions occur, we provide an initial account of why the different outcomes are found, identifying factors that lead to either positive or negative effects of production on perception. The review clarifies the complex relationship that exists between the two modalities: They are indeed linked, but their relationship is more complicated than is suggested by the notion that they are two sides of the same coin.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在功能磁共振成像研究中,我们对皮质-小脑网络中的言语错误监测进行了两项对比检查:(a)正确试验,发音错误概率高与低;(b)明显错误与正确试验.在两种对比中,认知小脑区域CrusI的参与表明该区域参与了总体性能监测。小脑运动区的激活(小脑上内侧,小叶VI和VIII)表明另外存在感觉运动驱动的对照实施。补充前运动区域(跨对比活动)和前扣带皮质(仅在涉及明显错误的对比中活动)激活的组合模式表明,在内侧额叶皮质中进行感觉运动驱动的反馈监测,通过明显的错误利用本体感觉和听觉反馈。不同的时间和顶叶皮层激活差异表明,除了感觉运动驱动的反馈之外,还涉及到语音产生模型,该模型将这些区域与听觉目标处理和内部类似建模的机制联系起来。这些结果突出了多重的存在,可能是分层相互依存的,支持语音生成优化的机制。
    In a functional magnetic resonance imaging study, we examined speech error monitoring in a cortico-cerebellar network for two contrasts: (a) correct trials with high versus low articulatory error probability and (b) overtly committed errors versus correct trials. Engagement of the cognitive cerebellar region Crus I in both contrasts suggests that this region is involved in overarching performance monitoring. The activation of cerebellar motor regions (superior medial cerebellum, lobules VI and VIII) indicates the additional presence of a sensorimotor driven implementation of control. The combined pattern of pre-supplementary motor area (active across contrasts) and anterior cingulate cortex (only active in the contrast involving overt errors) activations suggests sensorimotor driven feedback monitoring in the medial frontal cortex, making use of proprioception and auditory feedback through overt errors. Differential temporal and parietal cortex activation across contrasts indicates involvement beyond sensorimotor driven feedback in line with speech production models that link these regions to auditory target processing and internal modeling-like mechanisms. These results highlight the presence of multiple, possibly hierarchically interdependent, mechanisms that support the optimizing of speech production.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    流畅的语音制作是一项复杂的任务,跨越多个过程,从概念框架和词汇访问,通过语音编码,关节控制。在大多数情况下,影像研究描绘了言语流畅性的神经相关性倾向于检查临床人群维持言语障碍,并专注于无论是词汇访问或发音控制,但不是两者都有。这里,我们通过测量45名神经典型成人样本中小脑柄对言语流畅度的贡献,评估了小脑柄对言语流畅度的贡献.参与者进行了非结构化访谈,以评估他们的自然口语率和发音率,并完成了定时的语义和语音流畅性任务,以评估他们的言语流畅性。磁共振弥散成像和概率纤维束成像用于分割双侧小脑小脑(CPs)和额倾斜束(FAT),以前与临床人群的言语产生有关。我们的结果表明,具有不同流利度成分的白质关联的不同模式。具体来说,言语流畅性与正确的上级CP有关,而说话率与右侧中间CP和双侧FAT有关。在这些途径中没有发现与衔接率相关,与以前对口吃者的发现相反。我们的发现支持小脑对言语产生方面的贡献,超出了发音控制,比如词汇访问,语用或句法生成。Further,我们证明,不同的小脑通路分离了典型的神经性说话者言语流畅性的不同成分。
    Fluent speech production is a complex task that spans multiple processes, from conceptual framing and lexical access, through phonological encoding, to articulatory control. For the most part, imaging studies portraying the neural correlates of speech fluency tend to examine clinical populations sustaining speech impairments and focus on either lexical access or articulatory control, but not both. Here, we evaluated the contribution of the cerebellar peduncles to speech fluency by measuring the different components of the process in a sample of 45 neurotypical adults. Participants underwent an unstructured interview to assess their natural speaking rate and articulation rate, and completed timed semantic and phonemic fluency tasks to assess their verbal fluency. Diffusion magnetic resonance imaging with probabilistic tractography was used to segment the bilateral cerebellar peduncles (CPs) and frontal aslant tract (FAT), previously associated with speech production in clinical populations. Our results demonstrate distinct patterns of white matter associations with different fluency components. Specifically, verbal fluency is associated with the right superior CP, whereas speaking rate is associated with the right middle CP and bilateral FAT. No association is found with articulation rate in these pathways, in contrast to previous findings in persons who stutter. Our findings support the contribution of the cerebellum to aspects of speech production that go beyond articulatory control, such as lexical access, pragmatic or syntactic generation. Further, we demonstrate that distinct cerebellar pathways dissociate different components of speech fluency in neurotypical speakers.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们使用(i)涉及描述图片中简单事件的句子产生任务;(ii)涉及相同句子衔接但不涉及句子表述的听觉句子重复任务;(iii)涉及相同图形事件且没有明显衔接的听觉句子到图片匹配任务。将每个任务的激活与等效的文字处理任务进行了比较:名词产生,动词产生,听觉名词重复,和听觉名词到图片的匹配。我们将双侧小脑小叶VIIb的激活与将单词排序为句子相关联,因为与所有其他条件相比,它在句子产生方面增加了,并且与单词匹配相比,单词产生也被激活。我们将右小脑小叶VIIIb的副翼部分与言语的明显运动执行相关联,因为在(i)与相应的名词条件相比,句子的产生和重复以及(ii)与所有匹配任务相比,名词和动词的产生过程中激活更高,在任何无声(不说话)匹配任务期间,相对于固定没有激活。我们将右小脑CrusII内的激活与隐蔽的发音活动相关联,因为它激活了(i)所有语音产生比匹配任务更多,并且(ii)在无声(非讲话)匹配以及句子产生和句子重复期间与名词相比的句子。我们的研究偶然地分开了,第一次,小脑在通用语音产生中的三个不同的功能角色,它展示了句子生成如何增强对这些小脑区域的需求。
    We investigated which parts of the cerebellum are involved in formulating and articulating sentences using (i) a sentence production task that involved describing simple events in pictures; (ii) an auditory sentence repetition task involving the same sentence articulation but not sentence formulation; and (iii) an auditory sentence-to-picture matching task that involved the same pictorial events and no overt articulation. Activation for each of these tasks was compared to the equivalent word processing tasks: noun production, verb production, auditory noun repetition, and auditory noun-to-picture matching. We associate activation in bilateral cerebellum lobule VIIb with sequencing words into sentences because it increased for sentence production compared to all other conditions and was also activated by word production compared to word matching. We associate a paravermal part of right cerebellar lobule VIIIb with overt motor execution of speech, because activation was higher during (i) production and repetition of sentences compared to the corresponding noun conditions and (ii) noun and verb production compared to all matching tasks, with no activation relative to fixation during any silent (nonspeaking) matching task. We associate activation within right cerebellar Crus II with covert articulatory activity because it activated for (i) all speech production more than matching tasks and (ii) sentences compared to nouns during silent (nonspeaking) matching as well as sentence production and sentence repetition. Our study serendipitously segregated, for the first time, three distinct functional roles for the cerebellum in generic speech production, and it demonstrated how sentence production enhanced the demands on these cerebellar regions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    功能磁共振成像(fMRI)可以提高我们对服从运动语音功能的神经过程的理解。然而,其再现性仍不清楚。这项研究旨在评估使用单词重复任务在两个时间点的fMRI的可重复性。
    使用多水平一般线性模型分析来自14个健康对照的成像数据。
    在任务期间,在右半球小脑小叶IV-V中观察到明显的激活,右壳核,和双侧感觉运动皮质。发现时间点之间的激活在小脑中但在其他脑区域中随时间是中等可再现的。
    初步发现强调了在运动言语任务期间小脑和连接的大脑区域的参与。需要更多的工作来确定语音功能磁共振成像的再现性程度,然后才能将其用作大脑活动变化的可靠标记。
    UNASSIGNED: Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can improve our understanding of neural processes subserving motor speech function. Yet its reproducibility remains unclear. This study aimed to evaluate the reproducibility of fMRI using a word repetition task across two time points.
    UNASSIGNED: Imaging data from 14 healthy controls were analysed using a multi-level general linear model.
    UNASSIGNED: Significant activation was observed during the task in the right hemispheric cerebellar lobules IV-V, right putamen, and bilateral sensorimotor cortices. Activation between timepoints was found to be moderately reproducible across time in the cerebellum but not in other brain regions.
    UNASSIGNED: Preliminary findings highlight the involvement of the cerebellum and connected cerebral regions during a motor speech task. More work is needed to determine the degree of reproducibility of speech fMRI before this could be used as a reliable marker of changes in brain activity.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    名称协议(NA)是指发言者对图片名称的同意程度。一个有力的发现是,说话者比那些具有低一致性(LA)的人更快地命名具有高一致性(HA)的图片。这种NA效应被认为是因为LA图片强烈激活了几个名字,所以演讲者需要时间来选择一个。HA图片,相比之下,强烈激活单个名称,因此不需要从多个备选方案中选择一个名称。最近的词汇访问模型表明,心理词汇的结构随经验而变化。因此,演讲者在命名洛杉矶图片时应该考虑一系列的名字,但是他们认为每个名字的程度应该随着经验而改变。我们在两个图片命名实验中测试了这些假设。在实验1中,参与者在为每张图片命名一次时,命名LA的速度比HA图片快。重要的是,他们比洛杉矶图片的替代名称更快地产生模态名称(由大多数参与者提供),与扬声器为LA图片激活多个名称的观点一致。在实验2中,参与者在实验前熟悉了模态名称,并为每张图片命名了三次。尽管参与者第一次命名图片时仍然存在NA效果,与实验1相比,它减少了,并且随着每次图片重复而进一步减少。因此,熟悉和重复降低了NA效应,但没有消除它,建议演讲者激活一系列合理的名字。
    Name agreement (NA) refers to the degree to which speakers agree on a picture\'s name. A robust finding is that speakers are faster to name pictures with high agreement (HA) than those with low agreement (LA). This NA effect is thought to occur because LA pictures strongly activate several names, and so speakers need time to select one. HA pictures, in contrast, strongly activate a single name and so there is no need to select one name out of several alternatives. Recent models of lexical access suggest that the structure of the mental lexicon changes with experience. Thus, speakers should consider a range of names when naming LA pictures, but the extent to which they consider each of these names should change with experience. We tested these hypotheses in two picture-naming experiments. In Experiment 1, participants were faster to name LA than HA pictures when they named each picture once. Importantly, they were faster to produce modal names (provided by most participants) than alternative names for LA pictures, consistent with the view that speakers activate multiple names for LA pictures. In Experiment 2, participants were familiarised with the modal name before the experiment and named each picture three times. Although there was still an NA effect when participants named the pictures the first time, it was reduced in comparison to Experiment 1 and was further reduced with each picture repetition.Thus, familiarisation and repetition reduced the NA effect, but did not eliminate it, suggesting speakers activate a range of plausible names.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:开发LittlEARS®早期言语制作问卷(LEESPQ)旨在为专业人士提供有关儿童最早语言发展的有价值信息,并已成功地用多种语言进行验证。这项研究旨在验证塞尔维亚版本的LEESPQ在典型发育儿童中的应用,并将结果与其他语言的验证研究进行比较。
    方法:英文版的LEESPQ被回译为塞尔维亚语。父母在访问儿科诊所或通过个人联系以纸质或电子形式填写问卷。共收集了206份完整的问卷。使用二阶多项式模型计算18个月以下儿童的标准化期望值,以创建塞尔维亚语的规范曲线。然后将结果用于确定置信区间,下限是典型的语音语言发展的关键限制。最后,将结果与德国和加拿大英语的发展规范进行比较。
    结果:塞尔维亚LEESPQ版本显示出高同质性(r=.622)和内部一致性(α=.882),这表明它几乎完全衡量言语能力。男女婴儿总分无显著差异(U=4429.500,p=.090),所以它可以被认为是性别独立的问卷。塞尔维亚语和德语(U=645.500,p=.673)与塞尔维亚语和英语规范曲线(U=652.000,p=.725)之间的比较结果表明,LEESPQ可以应用于不同的人口群体,不管语言,文化或社会学差异。
    结论:LEESPQ是有效的,适合评估出生至18个月儿童早期言语发育的年龄依赖性和性别无关问卷。
    OBJECTIVE: The LittlEARS® Early Speech Production Questionnaire (LEESPQ) was developed to provide professionals with valuable information about children\'s earliest language development and has been successfully validated in several languages. This study aimed to validate the Serbian version of the LEESPQ in typically developing children and compare the results with validation studies in other languages.
    METHODS: The English version of the LEESPQ was back-translated into Serbian. Parents completed the questionnaire in paper or electronic form either during the visit to the paediatric clinic or through personal contact. A total of 206 completed questionnaires were collected. Standardized expected values were calculated using a second-order polynomial model for children up to 18 months of age to create a norm curve for the Serbian language. The results were then used to determine confidence intervals, with the lower limit being the critical limit for typical speech-language development. Finally, the results were compared with German and Canadian English developmental norms.
    RESULTS: The Serbian LEESPQ version showed high homogeneity (r = .622) and internal consistency (α = .882), indicating that it almost exclusively measures speech production ability. No significant difference in total score was found between male and female infants (U = 4429.500, p = .090), so it can be considered a gender-independent questionnaire. The results of the comparison between Serbian and German (U = 645.500, p = .673) and Serbian and English norm curves (U = 652.000, p = .725) show that the LEESPQ can be applied to different population groups, regardless of linguistic, cultural or sociological differences.
    CONCLUSIONS: The LEESPQ is a valid, age-dependent and gender-independent questionnaire suitable for assessing early speech development in children aged from birth to 18 months.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号