disgust

厌恶
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    巴甫洛夫恐惧条件和灭绝代表了基于暴露的干预措施的学习机制。虽然越来越多的证据表明厌恶在基于污染的强迫症(C-OCD)的发展中起着关键作用,条件性厌恶获取和维护中的失调,特别是在高阶概念过程的驱动下,没有被检查。这里,我们通过暴露高(HCC,n=41)或低(LCC,n=41)污染与概念级厌恶条件和灭绝范式有关。条件刺激(CS)是来自一个概念类别的图像,部分被非条件厌恶引起的刺激(US)增强,而来自另一类别的图像则用作非强化条件刺激(CS-)。皮肤电导反应(SCR),美国预期和CS效价等级是量化条件厌恶反应的主要结果。相对于LCC,HCC个体表现出增加的美国预期和CS+厌恶经验,但厌恶收购后的SCR水平相当。尽管从采集阶段到灭绝阶段的条件响应有所减少,两组都没有完全消除学习到的厌恶。重要的是,在HCC个体中,获得性厌恶的消退韧性更为明显。一起,我们的发现表明,自我报告污染较高的个体表现出更高的厌恶获取和灭绝抗性。这些发现为语义相关概念的失调厌恶学习机制如何可能导致C-OCD提供了初步证据。
    Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction represent learning mechanisms underlying exposure-based interventions. While increasing evidence indicates a pivotal role of disgust in the development of contamination-based obsessive-compulsive disorder (C-OCD), dysregulations in conditioned disgust acquisition and maintenance, in particular driven by higher-order conceptual processes, have not been examined. Here, we address this gap by exposing individuals with high (HCC, n = 41) or low (LCC, n = 41) contamination concern to a conceptual-level disgust conditioning and extinction paradigm. Conditioned stimuli (CS+) were images from one conceptual category partially reinforced by unconditioned disgust-eliciting stimuli (US), while images from another category served as non-reinforced conditioned stimuli (CS-). Skin conductance responses (SCRs), US expectancy and CS valence ratings served as primary outcomes to quantify conditioned disgust responses. Relative to LCC, HCC individuals exhibited increased US expectancy and CS+ disgust experience, but comparable SCR levels following disgust acquisition. Despite a decrease in conditioned responses from the acquisition phase to the extinction phase, both groups did not fully extinguish the learned disgust. Importantly, the extinction resilience of acquired disgust was more pronounced in HCC individuals. Together, our findings suggest that individuals with high self-reported contamination concern exhibit increased disgust acquisition and resistance to extinction. The findings provide preliminary evidence on how dysregulated disgust learning mechanism across semantically related concepts may contribute to C-OCD.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    恐惧,生气,和厌恶的面部表情在进化上是突出的,并传达不同类型的威胁信号。然而,目前尚不清楚这三种表达是否以相同的方式影响感官知觉和注意力。本ERP研究调查了处理不同类型的威胁面孔所依据的时间动态以及在感知负荷任务期间使用的注意力资源的影响。要求参与者判断叠加在屏幕中央面部上的条形长度。使用质量单变量统计方法来分析EEG数据。行为上,相对于中立的干扰者,暴露于恐惧的面孔后,任务准确性显着降低,独立于感知负荷。ERP结果显示,相对于厌恶的面孔,右半球的P1振幅被发现增强了恐惧,反映了恐惧线索的快速和粗略检测。N170的反应是由恐惧引起的,生气,厌恶的面孔比中立的面孔更大,建议在很大程度上自动和优先处理威胁。此外,早期后验否定(EPN)成分对恐惧和愤怒的面孔产生了更多的反应,表明优先注意代表急性威胁的刺激。此外,感知负荷对EPN和晚期正电位(LPP)产生了显著影响,在低感知负荷条件下观察到较大的响应,指示目标导向的认知加工。总的来说,恐惧的早期感官处理,生气,厌恶的面孔的特点是自动捕捉注意力的敏感度不同,尽管这些面部信号对生存很重要。恐惧的面孔产生强烈的干扰效果,并且比愤怒和厌恶的面孔得到更高的优先级处理。
    Fearful, angry, and disgusted facial expressions are evolutionarily salient and convey different types of threat signals. However, it remains unclear whether these three expressions impact sensory perception and attention in the same way. The present ERP study investigated the temporal dynamics underlying the processing of different types of threatening faces and the impact of attentional resources employed during a perceptual load task. Participants were asked to judge the length of bars superimposed over faces presented in the center of the screen. A mass univariate statistical approach was used to analyze the EEG data. Behaviorally, task accuracy was significantly reduced following exposure to fearful faces relative to neutral distractors, independent of perceptual load. The ERP results revealed that the P1 amplitude over the right hemisphere was found to be enhanced for fearful relative to disgusted faces, reflecting the rapid and coarse detection of fearful cues. The N170 responses elicited by fearful, angry, and disgusted faces were larger than those elicited by neutral faces, suggesting the largely automatic and preferential processing of threats. Furthermore, the early posterior negativity (EPN) component yielded increased responses to fearful and angry faces, indicating prioritized attention to stimuli representing acute threats. Additionally, perceptual load exerted a pronounced influence on the EPN and late positive potential (LPP), with larger responses observed in the low perceptual load condition, indicating goal-directed cognitive processing. Overall, the early sensory processing of fearful, angry, and disgusted faces is characterized by differential sensitivity in capturing attention automatically, despite the importance of these facial signals for survival. Fearful faces produce a strong interference effect and are processed with higher priority than angry and disgusted ones.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    虽然厌恶源于硬连线的哺乳动物厌恶反应,人类厌恶的意识体验在很大程度上取决于主观评价,甚至可能延伸到社会道德环境。这里,在一系列研究中,我们将功能磁共振成像与基于机器学习的预测模型相结合,建立了主观厌恶的综合神经生物学模型。开发的神经功能特征准确地预测了发现(n=78)和预注册验证(n=30)队列中的瞬时自我报告的主观厌恶,并在核心厌恶(n=34和n=26)中推广。味觉厌恶(n=30)和社会道德(不公平的报价;n=43)背景。厌恶体验被编码在分布式皮质和皮质下系统中,并表现出独特的和共享的神经表征与主观恐惧或消极影响的情感意识和有意识的评价系统,而签名最准确地预测了各自的目标体验。我们为厌恶提供了准确的功能磁共振成像特征,具有很高的潜力来解决正在进行的进化辩论。
    While disgust originates in the hard-wired mammalian distaste response, the conscious experience of disgust in humans strongly depends on subjective appraisal and may even extend to socio-moral contexts. Here, in a series of studies, we combined functional magnetic resonance imaging with machine-learning-based predictive modelling to establish a comprehensive neurobiological model of subjective disgust. The developed neurofunctional signature accurately predicted momentary self-reported subjective disgust across discovery (n = 78) and pre-registered validation (n = 30) cohorts and generalized across core disgust (n = 34 and n = 26), gustatory distaste (n = 30) and socio-moral (unfair offers; n = 43) contexts. Disgust experience was encoded in distributed cortical and subcortical systems, and exhibited distinct and shared neural representations with subjective fear or negative affect in interoceptive-emotional awareness and conscious appraisal systems, while the signatures most accurately predicted the respective target experience. We provide an accurate functional magnetic resonance imaging signature for disgust with a high potential to resolve ongoing evolutionary debates.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当有人违反社会规范时,其他人可能认为一些制裁是适当的。我们研究了愤怒和厌恶等情绪的体验如何与制裁的判断适当性相关,在对56个社会的大规模研究数据进行的预先注册分析中。在世界各地,我们发现,对违反规范感到愤怒和厌恶的人更有可能支持对抗,排斥和,在较小的程度上,八卦.此外,我们发现愤怒的经历始终是对抗判断的最强预测因子,与其他情绪相比。尽管基于状态的情绪和判断之间的联系似乎是普遍的,它的实力因国家而异。与理论预测一致,这种联系在社会中更牢固,在个人之间,这对个人自主权有更高的价值。因此,自主价值观可能会增加情绪在指导社会制裁判断中的作用。
    When someone violates a social norm, others may think that some sanction would be appropriate. We examine how the experience of emotions like anger and disgust relate to the judged appropriateness of sanctions, in a pre-registered analysis of data from a large-scale study in 56 societies. Across the world, we find that individuals who experience anger and disgust over a norm violation are more likely to endorse confrontation, ostracism and, to a smaller extent, gossip. Moreover, we find that the experience of anger is consistently the strongest predictor of judgments of confrontation, compared to other emotions. Although the link between state-based emotions and judgments may seem universal, its strength varies across countries. Aligned with theoretical predictions, this link is stronger in societies, and among individuals, that place higher value on individual autonomy. Thus, autonomy values may increase the role that emotions play in guiding judgments of social sanctions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    厌恶图像代表了厌恶相关疾病的潜在病理机制。然而,关于厌恶是否可以以厌恶引起的心理意象作为无条件刺激(美国)为条件,这仍然存在争议。因此,我们使用条件性学习范式结合事件相关电位(ERP)分析对35名健康大学生进行了研究.结果表明,最初的中性面孔(条件刺激,CS+)变得更加厌恶,不愉快,在与令人厌恶的意象(令人厌恶的CS+)配对后被唤醒,与中性(中性CS+)和无(CS-)图像配对相比。此外,我们观察到基于心理意象的厌恶条件对灭绝具有抵抗力。尽管在采集过程中,厌恶CS引起的P3和正电位幅度比CS-更大,厌恶CS+和中性CS+之间没有发现显著差异,表明自我报告和神经生理反应之间的分离。未来的研究可能还会获得面部肌电图作为条件性厌恶的内隐指标。这项研究提供了第一个神经生物学证据,证明联想厌恶学习可以在没有厌恶的物理刺激的情况下发生。与理解厌恶相关的疾病如何在没有外部感知厌恶经验的情况下表现或恶化有关,例如强迫症(OCD)。
    Disgust imagery represents a potential pathological mechanism for disgust-related disorders. However, it remains controversial as to whether disgust can be conditioned with disgust-evoking mental imagery serving as the unconditioned stimulus (US). Therefore, we examined this using a conditioned learning paradigm in combination with event-related potential (ERP) analysis in 35 healthy college students. The results indicated that the initial neutral face (conditioned stimulus, CS+) became more disgust-evoking, unpleasant, and arousing after pairing with disgust-evoking imagery (disgust CS+), compared to pairing with neutral (neutral CS+) and no (CS-) imagery. Moreover, we observed that mental imagery-based disgust conditioning was resistant to extinction. While the disgust CS + evoked larger P3 and late positive potential amplitudes than CS- during acquisition, no significant differences were found between disgust CS+ and neutral CS+, indicating a dissociation between self-reported and neurophysiological responses. Future studies may additionally acquire facial EMG as an implicit index of conditioned disgust. This study provides the first neurobiological evidence that associative disgust learning can occur without aversive physical stimuli, with implications for understanding how disgust-related disorders may manifest or deteriorate without external perceptual aversive experiences, such as in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    幸灾乐祸是对他人不幸的快乐情绪反应。个人对小组内和小组外成员的不幸幸灾乐祸可能会有所不同,具体取决于小组互动的方式。因此,本研究考察了不同的社会互动模式和群体认同对幸灾乐祸的影响及其机制。研究参与者是中国大学生。研究1(n=83)调查了在两种不同的社会互动模式下,个体对群体和群体外成员的幸灾乐祸是否存在差异:合作和竞争。在本研究的基础上,研究2(n=73)集中在潜在的机制的影响个人\'幸灾乐祸对内部和群体成员在竞争情况下,以及厌恶的中介作用。研究发现,群体认同和社会互动模式之间存在相互作用,人们对外群成员的不幸表现出更多的幸灾乐祸,竞争形势对外群成员越来越厌恶和幸灾乐祸。然而,在合作情况下,个体对群体内和群体外成员的幸灾乐祸没有发现差异。这在现实生活中的群体间关系以及社会互动模式方面具有指导意义。这证明了合作和团体接触是减少幸灾乐祸的一种方式。
    Schadenfreude is a joyful emotional response to the misfortune of others. Individuals\' schadenfreude over the misfortunes of ingroup and outgroup members may vary depending on how groups interact. Accordingly, this study examines the effects of different social interaction patterns and group identity on schadenfreude and their mechanisms. The study participants were Chinese college students. Study 1 (n = 83) investigated whether there are differences in individuals\' schadenfreude towards ingroup and outgroup members under two different patterns of social interaction: cooperation and competition. On the basis of this study, Study 2 (n = 73) focused on the mechanisms underlying the influence of individuals\' Schadenfreude on ingroup and outgroup members in competitive situations, and the mediating role of disgust. It was found that there was an interaction between group identity and social interaction patterns, with people showing more schadenfreude over the misfortunes of outgroup members than ingroup members, and competitive situations increasing disgust and schadenfreude over outgroup members. However, no differences were found in individuals\' schadenfreude towards ingroup and outgroup members in cooperative situations. This is instructive in terms of real-life intergroup relations as well as patterns of social interaction. This proves that cooperation and group contact is a way to reduce schadenfreude.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    产品的命名和标签会影响消费者的态度和随后的行为,特别是在市场上的新食品的情况下。本研究探讨了名称框架对消费者对养殖肉(CM)的态度的影响,目前正处于发展的早期阶段。以1532名中国消费者为样本,我们通过检查三个不同的术语(“培养的,\"\"人工,\"和\"基于单元格的\")用于CM。结果表明,“培养肉”和“基于细胞的肉”比“人造肉”更具吸引力。“CM的名称框架影响消费者对利益的感知大于对风险的感知。我们的综合模型确定了诱发情绪(感知厌恶)和自然性是态度的两个关键预测因子。这两个预测因子也是感知利益的实质性中介,它们激活了感知风险的中介作用(认知过程中微不足道的中介作用)。此外,感知的自然性主要通过感知的厌恶来介导名字框架效应。我们的发现对未来向公众传播新食物(如CM)的策略有影响。
    The naming and labeling of products can affect consumer attitudes and subsequent behavior, particularly in the case of new food products in the market. The present study explores the effects of name framing on consumer attitudes towards cultured meat (CM), which is currently in the early stages of development. With a sample of 1532 Chinese consumers, we integrated several pathways to explain the name-framing effect by examining three different terms (\"cultured,\" \"artificial,\" and \"cell-based\") for CM. Results indicate that \"cultured meat\" and \"cell-based meat\" are more appealing than \"artificial meat.\" Name framings of CM affect consumers\' perception of benefits more than that of risks. Our comprehensive model identified evoked affect (perceived disgust) and naturalness as two crucial predictors of attitudes. These two predictors also act as substantial mediators of perceived benefits, and they activate the mediation of perceived risks (an insignificant mediator in cognitive processing). In addition, perceived naturalness mediates the name-framing effect mainly through perceived disgust. Our findings have implications for future strategies for communicating about novel foods (like CM) to the public.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    基于光纤的柔性传感器在人体运动和医疗保健监测方面具有广阔的应用潜力,由于它们重量轻的优点,灵活,并且易于处理。现在,具有高灵敏度的高性能弹性光纤应变传感器,一个大的工作范围,和优异的耐久性是在很大的需求。在这里,通过在MXene/水性聚氨酯(WPU)分散溶液中浸涂高度可拉伸的聚氨酯(PU)弹性纤维,我们可以轻松快速地制备出高度灵敏且耐用的基于纤维的应变传感器。受益于混合溶液中带负电荷的WPU和MXene片材之间的静电斥力,成功获得了非常均匀和稳定的MXene/WPU分散体,并且互连的导电网络相应地形成在涂覆的MXene/WPU壳层中,这使得所制备的应变传感器表现出超过960的应变系数,超过90%的大传感范围,和检测极限低至0.5%应变。由于弹性纤维和混合溶液具有相同的聚合物构成,并实现了MXene/WPU导电复合材料在PU纤维上的紧密粘合,使制备的应变传感器能够承受超过2500个拉伸释放周期,因此显示出良好的耐久性。应变传感器还进行了全面的人体运动检测,和身体姿势监测,分析,并通过将基于纤维的应变传感器嵌入毛衣中开发了校正原型系统,强烈表明在锻炼中具有巨大的应用前景,体育,和医疗保健。
    Fiber-based flexible sensors have promising application potential in human motion and healthcare monitoring, owing to their merits of being lightweight, flexible, and easy to process. Now, high-performance elastic fiber-based strain sensors with high sensitivity, a large working range, and excellent durability are in great demand. Herein, we have easily and quickly prepared a highly sensitive and durable fiber-based strain sensor by dip coating a highly stretchable polyurethane (PU) elastic fiber in an MXene/waterborne polyurethane (WPU) dispersion solution. Benefiting from the electrostatic repulsion force between the negatively charged WPU and MXene sheets in the mixed solution, very homogeneous and stable MXene/WPU dispersion was successfully obtained, and the interconnected conducting networks were correspondingly formed in a coated MXene/WPU shell layer, which makes the as-prepared strain sensor exhibit a gauge factor of over 960, a large sensing range of over 90%, and a detection limit as low as 0.5% strain. As elastic fiber and mixed solution have the same polymer constitute, and tight bonding of the MXene/WPU conductive composite on PU fibers was achieved, enabling the as-prepared strain sensor to endure over 2500 stretching-releasing cycles and thus show good durability. Full-scale human motion detection was also performed by the strain sensor, and a body posture monitoring, analysis, and correction prototype system were developed via embedding the fiber-based strain sensors into sweaters, strongly indicating great application prospects in exercise, sports, and healthcare.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    以面部情绪为代表的非认知因素在教育评价中的应用近年来备受关注。现有关于面部情绪辅助教育评价的研究较多,但大多数都是基于虚拟学习环境,这意味着关于离线学习环境中面部情绪和学习效果的研究很少。为了解决这个问题,本研究设计了一个基于离线学习环境的情感观察实验,获得了127名大学生的学习者面部情绪类型和学习效果,并进一步探讨了两者之间的关系。结果表明:1)我们通过组合描述方法获得了八种类型的学习者情绪:喜悦,放松,惊喜,温柔,蔑视,厌恶,悲伤,焦虑和他们各自的PAD情感平均。2)我们得到了快乐的六种情绪的相关结果,放松,惊喜,温柔,蔑视,和焦虑与学习效果和学习效果的预测值有关。3)构建了基于离线学习环境的学习者情绪和学习效果的解释模型。
    The application of non-cognitive factors represented by facial emotion in educational evaluation has attracted much attention in recent years. There are many existing studies on facial emotion assisted education evaluation, but most of them are based on virtual learning environments, which means that the research on facial emotion and learning effect in offline learning environments is sparse. In order to solve this problem, this study designed an emotion observation experiment based on the offline learning environment, obtained the type of learner facial emotion and learning effect of 127 college students, and further explored the relationship between the two. The results show that: 1) We obtained eight types of learner emotion through the combined description method: joy, relaxation, surprise, meekness, contempt, disgust, sadness, anxiety and their respective PAD emotional mean. 2) We obtained the correlation results of the six emotions of joy, relaxation, surprise, meekness, contempt, and anxiety with the learning effect and the predicted value of the learning effect. 3) We then constructed an explanatory model of learner emotion and learning effect based on the offline learning environment.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    并非YouTube串流视频广告中显示的所有元素都可归因于广告本身。其中一些是由平台自动引入的,例如倒数计时器和时间进度条。近年来,一些作者开始探索与此类非广告项目的存在相关的影响,提供有价值的发现。然而,在这种情况下,缺乏对观众视觉注意力的客观评估以及情感调查。此外,先前的研究表明,在流媒体广告的背景下,对看似微不足道的细节的操纵如何产生截然不同的结果。为了扩展知识,作者通过在以YouTube15-s为重点的受试者间实验设计中采用眼睛跟踪和面部编码技术与自我报告相结合,探索了非广告项目存在的影响,中卷,不可跳过的流式广告格式。结果表明,YouTube目前采用的广告格式比在所有调查措施中没有非广告项目的情况下表现更差,并且比在面部编码厌恶中实验性地减少了非广告项目的存在,自我报告的厌恶,广告刺激,和广告态度。强调了有关流式广告和神经营销未来的管理见解和挑战。
    Not all elements displayed in a YouTube in-stream video ad are attributable to the ad itself. Some of those are automatically introduced by the platform, such as the countdown timer and the time progress bar. In recent years, some authors started exploring the effects associated with the presence of such non-ad items, providing valuable findings. However, objective evaluation of viewers\' visual attention is lacking in this context as well as emotional investigation. In addition, previous research showed how the manipulation of seemingly negligible details can yield dramatically different outcomes in the context of in-stream advertising. To extend knowledge, the authors explored the effects of the non-ad items\' presence by employing eye-tracking and facial coding techniques in combination with self-reports in a between-subjects experimental design focusing on the YouTube 15-s, mid-roll, non-skippable in-stream ad format. Results showed that the ad format currently employed by YouTube performs worse than its equivalent without the non-ad items on all the investigated measures and than its equivalent in which the non-ad items\' presence was experimentally reduced on facial coding disgust, self-reported disgust, ad irritation, and ad attitude. Managerial insights and challenges concerning the future of in-stream advertising and neuromarketing are highlighted.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号