Form Perception

形式感知
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在视觉感知的时间机制研究和视觉掩蔽研究中,已经研究了时间整合和隔离。尽管这两个研究路线在理论上都有相同的观点,方法论,和经验相似性,它们之间几乎没有重叠,并且它们的时间处理模型不兼容。作为两个研究领域统一的第一步,我们在超超掩蔽范式中研究了时间整合和分离的电生理相关性。参与者在每个试验中报告了他们在记录EEG时是否将目标掩模序列感知为同时或时间分离的感知。两种时间报告类别的比较导致了刺激呈现后的ERP差异(200-450ms),与轮廓整合的负性非常相似。此外,我们发现,在刺激呈现之前,在α(450-250ms)和β(225-125ms)频段,相位状态在感知报告类别之间发生了偏移,并在之后的时间报告比例中诱发了正弦周期性.因此,我们证明了时间整合和分离的神经相关可以推广到元模型掩蔽。这些发现强调了时间机制在掩蔽现象出现中的潜在作用。此外,我们的研究结果通过证明与基于绩效的任务相似的时间整合和隔离的神经相关性来验证我们的现象学方法.未来的研究可能会受益于我们的现象学方法,以解开时间和掩蔽机制之间的(神经)相互作用。
    Temporal integration and segregation have been investigated both in the research on the temporal mechanisms in visual perception and in the research on visual masking. Although both research lines share theoretical, methodological, and empirical similarities, there is little overlap between them and their models of temporal processing are incompatible. As a first step toward the unification of both lines of research, we investigated the electrophysiological correlates of temporal integration and segregation in a metacontrast masking paradigm. Participants reported in each trial whether they perceived the target-mask sequence as a simultaneous or temporally segregated percept while their EEG was recorded. A comparison of both temporal report categories resulted in an ERP difference after stimulus presentation (200-450 ms) that closely resembles the contour integration negativity. Moreover, we found that phase states were shifted between perceptual report categories in the alpha (450-250 ms) and beta (225-125 ms) frequency band before stimulus presentation and induced a sinusoidal periodicity in later temporal report proportions. Thus, we show that neural correlates of temporal integration and segregation can be generalized to metacontrast masking. These findings emphasize the potential role of temporal mechanisms in the emergence of the masking phenomenon. Additionally, our findings validate our phenomenological approach by demonstrating similar neural correlates of temporal integration and segregation as in performance-based tasks. Future research may profit from our phenomenological approach to disentangle the (neural) interplay between temporal and masking mechanisms.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    通过采取各种研究方法,可以更好地阐明感知对象材料所涉及的复杂视觉处理。共享刺激和反应数据是一种有效的策略,可以使不同研究的结果直接具有可比性,并且可以帮助具有不同背景的研究人员跳入该领域。这里,我们构建了一个数据库,其中包含几组带有视觉辨别性能注释的材料图像。我们使用基于物理的计算机图形技术创建了材料图像,并在实验室和众包环境中进行了心理物理实验。观察者的任务是区分六个维度之一的材料(光泽对比,图像的光泽清晰度,半透明vs.不透明,金属vs.塑料,金属vs.玻璃,和有光泽的vs.painted).照明一致性和物体几何形状也不同。我们使用了一个适用于不同用例的非语言过程(一个奇怪的任务),比如跨文化,跨物种,临床,或发展研究。结果表明,材料的辨别取决于照度和几何形状,并且在光泽度感知中辨别镜面高光的空间一致性的能力比其他任务显示出更大的个体差异。此外,视觉特征分析表明,高阶颜色纹理统计量的参数可以部分,但不完全是,解释任务绩效。通过众包获得的结果与实验室获得的结果高度相关,这表明即使在实验室中没有严格控制实验条件的情况下,我们的数据库也可以使用。使用我们的数据集的几个项目正在进行中。
    Complex visual processing involved in perceiving the object materials can be better elucidated by taking a variety of research approaches. Sharing stimulus and response data is an effective strategy to make the results of different studies directly comparable and can assist researchers with different backgrounds to jump into the field. Here, we constructed a database containing several sets of material images annotated with visual discrimination performance. We created the material images using physically based computer graphics techniques and conducted psychophysical experiments with them in both laboratory and crowdsourcing settings. The observer\'s task was to discriminate materials on one of six dimensions (gloss contrast, gloss distinctness of image, translucent vs. opaque, metal vs. plastic, metal vs. glass, and glossy vs. painted). The illumination consistency and object geometry were also varied. We used a nonverbal procedure (an oddity task) applicable for diverse use cases, such as cross-cultural, cross-species, clinical, or developmental studies. Results showed that the material discrimination depended on the illuminations and geometries and that the ability to discriminate the spatial consistency of specular highlights in glossiness perception showed larger individual differences than in other tasks. In addition, analysis of visual features showed that the parameters of higher order color texture statistics can partially, but not completely, explain task performance. The results obtained through crowdsourcing were highly correlated with those obtained in the laboratory, suggesting that our database can be used even when the experimental conditions are not strictly controlled in the laboratory. Several projects using our dataset are underway.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在数字地面组织中,该图被定义为一个既“成形”又更近的区域。“在这里,我们测试任务集和指令的变化是否可以改变构成图形分配基础的图形先验之间的跨境竞争的结果。极值边缘(EE),相对距离先验,当任务是报告“哪一边更近?”时,已经被确立为强大的形象先验。在三个使用双向刺激的实验中,EE竞争和配合熟悉的配置,在“哪一侧是成形的?”任务中进行图形分配的形状之前。“实验1显示了熟悉的配置对绘制直立熟悉物体的显示器的影响很小但很重要,尽管“形状侧”反应主要由EEs决定。在实验2中,添加了有关感知熟悉形状的可能性的说明。现在,尽管EE仍然占主导地位,在所有显示类型的试验中,在边界的熟悉构型一侧观察到该数据的比例明显更高.在实验3中,任务集(较近/成形)以及强调可能存在熟悉对象的指令的存在与不存在都在受试者中进行了操作。如此熟悉,“准备好了,“当EE和熟悉的配置偏爱相反的一面时,任务集的效果就出现了。因此,更改指令可以以与任务集交互的方式在图形分配中调节图形先验的形状与距离的权重。此外,我们表明,当指令强调熟悉的物体可能存在时,熟悉的部分会出现在没有内侧颞叶/周围皮层脑损伤的参与者中。
    In figure-ground organization, the figure is defined as a region that is both \"shaped\" and \"nearer.\" Here we test whether changes in task set and instructions can alter the outcome of the cross-border competition between figural priors that underlies figure assignment. Extremal edge (EE), a relative distance prior, has been established as a strong figural prior when the task is to report \"which side is nearer?\" In three experiments using bipartite stimuli, EEs competed and cooperated with familiar configuration, a shape prior for figure assignment in a \"which side is shaped?\" task.\" Experiment 1 showed small but significant effects of familiar configuration for displays sketching upright familiar objects, although \"shaped-side\" responses were predominantly determined by EEs. In Experiment 2, instructions regarding the possibility of perceiving familiar shapes were added. Now, although EE remained the dominant prior, the figure was perceived on the familiar-configuration side of the border on a significantly larger percentage of trials across all display types. In Experiment 3, both task set (nearer/shaped) and the presence versus absence of instructions emphasizing that familiar objects might be present were manipulated within subjects. With familiarity thus \"primed,\" effects of task set emerged when EE and familiar configuration favored opposite sides as figure. Thus, changing instructions can modulate the weighing of figural priors for shape versus distance in figure assignment in a manner that interacts with task set. Moreover, we show that the influence of familiar parts emerges in participants without medial temporal lobe/ perirhinal cortex brain damage when instructions emphasize that familiar objects might be present.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Trial-by-trial texture classification analysis and identifying salient texture related EEG features during active touch that are minimally influenced by movement type and frequency conditions are the main contributions of this work. A total of twelve healthy subjects were recruited. Each subject was instructed to use the fingertip of their dominant hand\'s index finger to rub or tap three textured surfaces (smooth flat, medium rough, and rough) with three levels of movement frequency (approximately 2, 1 and 0.5 Hz). EEG and force data were collected synchronously during each touch condition. A systematic feature selection process was performed to select temporal and spectral EEG features that contribute to texture classification but have low contribution towards movement type and frequency classification. A tenfold cross validation was used to train two 3-class (each for texture and movement frequency classification) and a 2-class (movement type) Support Vector Machine classifiers. Our results showed that the total power in the mu (8-15 Hz) and beta (16-30 Hz) frequency bands showed high accuracy in discriminating among textures with different levels of roughness (average accuracy > 84%) but lower contribution towards movement type (average accuracy < 65%) and frequency (average accuracy < 58%) classification.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    To investigate cognitive control, researchers have repeatedly employed task switching paradigms. The comparison of switch relative to repeat trials reveals longer response times and higher error rates, a pattern that has been interpreted as switching costs. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shown the involvement of different brain modules in switching conditions, including prefrontal and parietal regions together with other sub-cortical structures. In this study, the aim was to shed light on the brain basis of cognitive control using an approach that proved useful in previous studies investigating language control in bilinguals. We examined adult participants in one simple color naming context and two task selection mixed contexts. In the first mixed selection context, participants named the color or the shape of the stimulus based on a cue word. In the second, they named the color or the size of the stimulus. It was assumed that the comparison of brain responses to the same color naming in mixed selection contexts vs. in non-selection context will reveal the of engagement of cognitive control/task selection processes. Whole brain analysis of color naming in the different contexts showed a significant main effect of context. The comparison of brain responses in several frontal, parietal and sub-cortical regions, of which some are supposedly involved in cognitive control, demonstrated an increased activation during color naming in mixed relative the simple non-mixed context. The different cognitive control modules described in this study fit with recent bilingual language control and domain general cognitive models.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    This study evaluated the functional role of behavioral patterns in relational behavior in humans. The participants were five children, 9-11 years-old. A modified transposition task (size) was used, requiring active comparison patterns in order to match two relational stimulus compounds with two sample stimulus compounds. Comparison patterns were analyzed in terms of the variety of sequences and exceeding movements, the choice of relevant stimuli, and their correspondence with the size relationship between stimulus instances. Results suggest that variation in sequences and movements as well as choice for relevant stimuli influenced the establishment of relational behavior. The neglect of active comparison patterns in accounting for relational behavior is discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    儿童早期的运动经验和主动探索可能会影响各种感知和认知能力的个体差异。在目前的研究中,我们建议对对象的主动探索有助于处理对象形式和大小的能力,进而影响数字感知的发展。我们通过对59名8个月大的婴儿进行预先注册的主动探索干预来检验我们的假设。最小的干预措施包括每天一次积极地玩和探索积木,持续8周。为了控制可能的训练对注意力的影响,我们使用读书作为控制条件。使用眼动追踪的测试前和测试后评估表明,方块游戏改善了视觉形式感知,婴儿在检测异常形状方面变得更好。此外,使用三个控制任务,我们发现,干预特别改善了婴儿处理视觉形式的能力,这种效果不能用注意力或视觉感知的总体改善来解释。我们发现干预并没有改善数字感知,并表明由于我们假设的顺序性质,可能需要更长的时间范围才能看到这种能力的改进。我们的发现表明,如果给婴儿更多的玩耍和探索的机会,这将对他们的视觉形态感知产生积极影响,这反过来可以帮助他们理解几何概念。
    Motor experiences and active exploration during early childhood may affect individual differences in a wide range of perceptual and cognitive abilities. In the current study, we suggest that active exploration of objects facilitates the ability to process object forms and magnitudes, which in turn impacts the development of numerosity perception. We tested our hypothesis by conducting a preregistered active exploration intervention with 59 8-month-old infants. The minimal intervention consisted of actively playing with and exploring blocks once a day for 8 weeks. In order to control for possible training effects on attention, we used book reading as a control condition. Pre- and post-test assessments using eye-tracking showed that block play improved visual form perception, where infants became better at detecting a deviant shape. Furthermore, using three control tasks, we showed that the intervention specifically improved infants\' ability to process visual forms and the effect could not be explained by a domain general improvement in attention or visual perception. We found that the intervention did not improve numerosity perception and suggest that because of the sequential nature of our hypothesis, a longer time frame might be needed to see improvements in this ability. Our findings indicate that if infants are given more opportunities for play and exploration, it will have positive effects on their visual form perception, which in turn could help their understanding of geometrical concepts.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    People have been shown to link particular sounds with particular shapes. For instance, the round-sounding nonword bouba tends to be associated with curved shapes, whereas the sharp-sounding nonword kiki is deemed to be related to angular shapes. People\'s tendency to associate sounds and shapes has been observed across different languages. In the present study, we reexamined the claim by Hung, Styles, and Hsieh (2017) that such sound-shape mappings can occur before an individual becomes aware of the visual stimuli. More precisely, we replicated their first experiment, in which congruent and incongruent stimuli (e.g., bouba presented in a round shape or an angular shape, respectively) were rendered invisible through continuous flash suppression. The results showed that congruent combinations, on average, broke suppression faster than incongruent combinations, thus providing converging evidence for Hung and colleagues\' assertions. Collectively, these findings now provide a solid basis from which to explore the boundary conditions of the effect.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    轮廓整合,处理自然场景中的遮挡和不连续性的关键视觉功能,对人类生存至关重要。然而,个人并不具备这种能力。特别是,轮廓整合缺陷通常在精神障碍患者中检测到,尤其是精神分裂症。要了解这些个体差异的潜在来源,本研究调查了人类轮廓整合的遗传基础。总共测试了2619名正常参与者检测嵌入在杂乱背景中的连续轮廓的能力。进行了定量基因组分析,涉及基于单核苷酸多态性(SNP)的遗传力估计和SNP的关联测试,基因,和路径水平。遗传力估计表明,常见的SNP贡献了49.5%(平均值的标准误差=15.6%)的总体表型变异,表明等高线整合的中等遗传力。两阶段全基因组关联分析(GWAS)检测到四个SNP,在发现测试(N=1931)中达到全基因组显著性,但未通过复制测试(N=688)。基因水平分析进一步揭示了microRNA编码基因MIR1178在发现和复制队列中的显著全基因组关联。另一个基因poly(A)-结合蛋白核1样,细胞质(PABPN1L)在发现队列中显示出暗示性意义(p<1×10-4),并在复制队列中复制(p=0.009)。途径分析未检测到任何显著途径。一起来看,这项研究确定了与轮廓整合的重要基因关联,并为在环境中感知连续轮廓的能力的遗传传递提供了支持。
    Contour integration, a key visual function to deal with occlusion and discontinuity in natural scenes, is essential to human survival. However, individuals are not equally well equipped with this ability. In particular, contour integration deficiencies are commonly detected in patients with mental disorders, especially schizophrenia. To understand the underlying sources of these individual differences, the current study investigated the genetic basis of contour integration in humans. A total of 2619 normal participants were tested on their ability to detect continuous contours embedded in a cluttered background. Quantitative genomic analysis was performed, involving heritability estimation based on single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and association testing at SNP, gene, and pathway levels. Heritability estimation showed that common SNPs contributed 49.5% (standard error of the mean = 15.6%) of overall phenotypic variation, indicating moderate heritability of contour integration. Two-stage genome-wide association analysis (GWAS) detected four SNPs reaching genome-wide significance in the discovery test (N = 1931) but not passing the replication test (N = 688). Gene-level analysis further revealed a significant genome-wide association of a microRNA-encoding gene MIR1178 in both the discovery and replication cohorts. Another gene poly(A)-binding protein nuclear 1 like, cytoplasmic (PABPN1L) showed suggestive significance in the discovery cohort (p < 1 × 10-4) and was replicated in the replication cohort (p = 0.009). The pathway analysis did not detect any significant pathway. Taken together, this study identified significant gene associations with contour integration and provided support for a genetic transmission of the ability to perceive continuous contours in the environment.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Angle perception is an important middle-level visual process, combining line features to generate an integrated shape percept. Previous studies have proposed two theories of angle perception-a combination of lines and a holistic feature following Weber\'s law. However, both theories failed to explain the dual-peak fluctuations of the just-noticeable difference (JND) across angle sizes. In this study, we found that the human visual system processes the angle feature in two stages: first, by encoding the orientation of the bounding lines and combining them into an angle feature; and second, by estimating the angle in an orthogonal internal reference frame (IRF). The IRF model fits well with the dual-peak fluctuations of the JND that neither the theory of line combinations nor Weber\'s law can explain. A statistical image analysis of natural images revealed that the IRF was in alignment with the distribution of the angle features in the natural environment, suggesting that the IRF reflects human prior knowledge of angles in the real world. This study provides a new computational framework for angle discrimination, thereby resolving a long-standing debate on angle perception.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

公众号