warmth

温暖
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    美国境内的拉丁裔移民经历各种压力源,这与拉丁裔青年内在化症状的发展和恶化有关。因此,探索可能缓冲移民家庭压力影响的因素至关重要。父亲可能会通过积极的育儿行为影响孩子的结果,然而拉丁裔父亲在研究中的代表性不足。这项研究调查了父亲的温暖和支持如何调节低收入人群中压力积累和儿童内化症状之间的联系,拉丁裔移民家庭。参与者包括62名6至10岁的儿童及其父亲照顾者(94%的父亲;90%出生在墨西哥)。本研究采用了包括家长报告在内的多方法方法,儿童报告和观察措施。参与者在家访期间完成了问卷调查和视频记录的家庭互动任务。压力与儿童报告的抑郁症呈正相关。自我报告的父亲温暖与父母报告的儿童内化症状减少有关,并减轻了压力对症状的影响。分析表明,对于父母温暖程度最低的儿童,父亲的压力与儿童内在化症状之间存在显着正相关。然而,当父亲的温暖很高时,应激积累与儿童内化症状无关.因此,高水平的温暖缓冲了压力对内在化症状的影响。这项研究强调了拉丁裔父亲的育儿行为对儿童内化症状的重要性。
    Latino immigrants within the United States experience various stressors, which have been linked to the development and exacerbation of internalizing symptoms among Latino youth. Therefore, it is crucial to explore factors that may buffer the impact of stress among immigrant families. Fathers may influence child outcomes through positive parenting behaviors, yet Latino fathers have been underrepresented in research. This study examined how paternal warmth and support may moderate the association between the accumulation of stress and child internalizing symptoms among low-income, Latino immigrant families. Participants included 62 children between the ages of 6 and 10, and their paternal caregivers (94% fathers; 90% born in Mexico). This study utilized a multi-method approach including parent-report, child-report and observational measures. Participants completed questionnaires and video-recorded family interaction tasks during home visits. Stress was positively associated with child-reported depression. Self-reported paternal warmth was associated with fewer parent-reported child internalizing symptoms and moderated the effects of stress on symptoms. Analyses showed a significant positive association between paternal stress and child internalizing symptoms for children with the lowest levels of parental warmth. However, when paternal warmth was high, accumulation of stress and child internalizing symptoms were not related. Thus, high levels of warmth buffered the impact of stress on internalizing symptoms. This study highlights the importance of Latino fathers\' parenting behaviors on child internalizing symptoms.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    一眼足以将心理属性赋予他人。吸引力与积极的属性(\'美丽是好\'刻板印象)相关。这里,我们提出了一个类似但负面偏见的问题。面部异常的人是否与消极的个人特征有关?我们假设,由于负面的刻板印象(温暖和能力不足)和非人性化的形式(动物性和机械性),会产生对异常面孔的偏见。我们招募了1493名mTurk参与者(排除后N=1306),以使用整形手术前后的60对同一人的照片评估被拍照者的31个特征。一半的异常面孔有疤痕,另一半有麻痹。计算温暖和能力,我们对31个属性进行了主成分分析.通过平均与道德敏感性和理性/逻辑相对应的反向评分来评估动物的非人性化,通过平均对应于情绪反应和人际温暖的反向评分评分,实现机械性的非人性化。我们发现两种异常的面孔都被认为不那么温暖,称职,被非人化。我们的发现表明,无论异常的病因如何,“异常坏”的刻板印象都是普遍的。这种效应可能与反向晕轮效应有关,也就是说,号角效应。
    A glance is enough to assign psychological attributes to others. Attractiveness is associated with positive attributes (\'beauty-is-good\' stereotype). Here, we raise the question of a similar but negative bias. Are people with facial anomalies associated with negative personal characteristics? We hypothesized that biases against faces with anomalies arise because of negative stereotypes (less warmth and competence) and forms of dehumanization (animalistic and mechanistic). We enrolled 1493 mTurk participants (N = 1306 after exclusion) to assess 31 traits of photographed people using 60 pairs of photographs of the same person before and after plastic surgery. Half anomalous faces had a scar and the other half had a palsy. To calculate warmth and competence, we conducted a principal components analysis of the 31 attributes. Animalistic dehumanization was assessed by averaging reverse-scored ratings corresponding to moral sensibility and rationality/logic, and mechanistic dehumanization by averaging across reverse-scored ratings corresponding to emotional responsiveness and interpersonal warmth. We found that both kinds of anomalous faces were seen as less warm, competent and were dehumanized. Our findings suggest that an \'anomalous-is-bad\' stereotype generalizes regardless of the aetiology of the anomaly. This effect may be related to a reverse halo effect, that is, the horn effect.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    风险沟通涉及向受众传达潜在风险。它对塑造行为和影响个人福祉至关重要。以前的研究主要集中在风险沟通的口头和书面方面,不太强调像声调这样的非语言线索。解决这个差距,我们的研究探讨了在两种风险决策范式中,有能力和温暖的声调对风险沟通的影响,研究1中的气球模拟风险任务(BART)和研究2中的赌博任务。结果表明,称职和温暖的声调比中性声调更有说服力,在不同的决策方案中,它们的有效性也不同。此外,参与者的感知能力和声调的温暖调解了这种说服力。这项研究通过纳入人声音调的影响,增强了我们对风险沟通的理论理解。此外,它对营销人员和从业者都有实际意义,证明了在现实世界中使用语音作为说服媒介的重要性。
    Risk communication involves conveying potential risks to the audience. It\'s crucial for shaping behavior and influencing individual well-being. Previous research predominantly focused on verbal and written aspects of risk communication, with less emphasis on nonverbal cues like vocal tone. Addressing this gap, our study explores the impact of competent and warm vocal tones on risk communication across two risky decision-making paradigms, the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) in Study 1 and the Gambling Task in Study 2. Results show that competent and warm vocal tones are more persuasive than neutral tones, and their effectiveness varies in different decision-making scenarios. Additionally, participants\' perceived competence and warmth of vocal tones mediate this persuasiveness. This study enhances our theoretical understanding of risk communication by incorporating the impact of vocal tones. Also, it carries practical implications for marketers and practitioners, demonstrating the importance of using voice as a medium to persuade in real-world scenarios.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Systematic Review
    刻板印象内容模型表明两个特质(即,温暖和能力)支配个人的印象形成。自2000年代初以来,使用该理论的大量研究导致需要对先前发现进行结构化概述。这项研究的目的是提供研究流的简明地图,并提出研究议程。我们对955篇文献进行了系统的文献综述。书目耦合分析表明有四个簇,即,(1)供应链管理的一般理论基础,(2)关键定型观念(强调性别)的社会影响,(3)临床心理学与儿童发育研究,(4)营销。仔细研究市场营销研究(使用共现分析),确定了六个研究流,包括关于品牌的研究,原产国,一线服务提供商,亲社会消费者行为,对代言人的看法,and,最近,人工智能(AI)。该综述介绍了这些主题的主要发现和研究差距。最后,我们回顾了几篇文章,虽然没有落入这些溪流,开辟了重要的研究脉络。这一进程为提出一个有希望和完整的研究议程提供了必要的信息,继续在不同的环境中建立具有影响力的知识。
    The stereotypes content model indicates that two traits (i.e., warmth and competence) govern individuals\' impression formation. The great variety of research that has used this theory since the early 2000s leads to a need for a structured overview of prior findings. The goal of this study is to provide a concise map of research streams and present a research agenda. We conducted a systematic literature review of 955 articles. A bibliographic coupling analysis showed four clusters, i.e., (1) the general theoretical foundations of the SCM, (2) the societal impact of key stereotypes (with emphasis on gender), (3) research in clinical psychology and child development, and (4) marketing. Taking a closer look at research in marketing (using co-occurrence analysis), six research streams were identified, including research on branding, country-of-origin, front-line service providers, prosocial consumer behavior, perception of endorsers, and, more recently, on artificial intelligence (AI). The review presents key findings and research gaps across these topics. Finally, we reviewed the few articles that, although not falling into these streams, opened important research veins. This process provided the essential information to present a promising and complete research agenda, to continue building knowledge with impactful implications in different contexts.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人工智能(AI)的快速发展在许多方面对社会产生了影响。伴随着这一进展,隐私侵犯等问题,歧视性偏见,安全隐患也浮出水面,强调道德发展的必要性,负责任,对社会有益的AI。作为回应,值得信赖的人工智能的概念已经变得越来越突出,并提出了一些开发可信人工智能的指导方针。在这种背景下,我们证明了心理学研究在识别有助于AI信任形成的因素方面的重要性。具体来说,我们回顾了关于人际关系的研究结果,人类自动化,从三维框架的角度(即,信任者,受托人,以及它们的交互式上下文)。该框架综合了与不同信任类型之间的信任形成和维护相关的公共因素。这些因素指出了构建值得信赖的AI的基本要求,并为其发展提供了关键指导,其中也涉及通信。教育,对用户进行培训。最后,我们讨论了信任研究中的见解如何帮助增强AI的可信性并促进其采用和应用。
    The rapid advancement of artificial intelligence (AI) has impacted society in many aspects. Alongside this progress, concerns such as privacy violation, discriminatory bias, and safety risks have also surfaced, highlighting the need for the development of ethical, responsible, and socially beneficial AI. In response, the concept of trustworthy AI has gained prominence, and several guidelines for developing trustworthy AI have been proposed. Against this background, we demonstrate the significance of psychological research in identifying factors that contribute to the formation of trust in AI. Specifically, we review research findings on interpersonal, human-automation, and human-AI trust from the perspective of a three-dimension framework (i.e., the trustor, the trustee, and their interactive context). The framework synthesizes common factors related to trust formation and maintenance across different trust types. These factors point out the foundational requirements for building trustworthy AI and provide pivotal guidance for its development that also involves communication, education, and training for users. We conclude by discussing how the insights in trust research can help enhance AI\'s trustworthiness and foster its adoption and application.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    同行评估用于各种军事背景。当前的研究调查了可能影响同龄人相互评估的因素,包括年龄,感知到的温暖和能力,控制源,和身体健康。初级领导者培训课程的参与者根据课程要求在课程的三个时间点完成了同行评估。参与者还对同伴的温暖和能力进行了评估,并对控制措施做出了回应。课程绩效指标,还获得了包括身体素质在内的评分。能力等级是所有三个时间点同行评估的唯一重要预测因素。对等体之间的相互关联,自我,还检查了教师对领导力和课程表现的评估。讨论了这些发现的含义。
    Peer assessments are used in a variety of military contexts. The current study examined factors that may influence how peers assess each other, including age, perceived warmth and competence, locus of control, and physical fitness. Participants in a junior leader training course completed peer assessments at three time points during the course per curriculum requirements. Participants also rated their peers on measures of warmth and competence and responded to a locus of control measure. Course performance metrics, including physical fitness scores were also obtained. Ratings of competence were the only significant predictors of peer assessments over all three time points. The inter-correlations between peer, self, and instructor assessments of leadership and course performance were also examined. The implications of these findings are discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    许多科学传播者的目标是培养对科学家及其工作的信任。这项研究调查了科学家在常规活动过程中创建的现有文本资源是否可以得到改善,以增强人们对科学家值得信赖的看法。建立在Mayer等人的基础上。组织信任的整合模型,我们研究如何通过简短的传记来传达仁慈可以使用3(仁慈信息的程度:高,未指定,低)×3(研究领域:作物遗传学,玉米和大豆遗传学,生物技术使用)调查设计。我们发现,仁慈信息的程度显着影响对仁慈和正直的看法,以及信任的意愿,这些影响在不同的研究领域是一致的。然而,所传达的仁慈程度对科学家的感知能力没有显着影响。这些发现强调了在交流中强调仁慈以积极影响可信度的重要性,从而为科学传播实践提供见解。
    A goal of many science communicators is to foster trust in scientists and their work. This study investigates if existing textual resources that scientists create in the course of their regular activities can be improved to enhance perceptions of scientists as trustworthy. Building on Mayer et al.\'s integrative model of organizational trust, we examine how communicating benevolence through short biographies can affect trustworthiness perceptions using a 3 (degree of benevolence information: high, unspecified, low) × 3 (research area: crop genetics, corn and soy genetics, biotechnology use) survey design. We find that the degree of benevolence information significantly influences perceptions of benevolence and integrity, as well as willingness to trust, with these effects being consistent across different research areas. However, the degree of benevolence communicated had no significant effect on the perceived competence of the scientists. These findings underscore the importance of highlighting benevolence in communication to positively influence trustworthiness perceptions, thus offering insights for science communication practices.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    社交焦虑障碍(SAD)与人际关系障碍有关。这种功能障碍的一个可能原因是,与没有这种疾病的人相比,患有SAD的人在温暖和优势方面对他人的评价不同。在目前的研究中,我们检查了SAD的两个核心结构,害怕负面评价和害怕正面评价,影响人们基于温暖和支配地位对群体的判断。我们还调查了种族相似性是否(即,某人是否与他们互动的种族相同)和种族身份(即,一个人对特定社会群体的归属感)在人们做出的评价类型中发挥了作用。我们创造了关于不同温暖和优势的群体的小插曲,以及种族化妆不同的照片。我们向参与者展示了照片-插图对,并要求他们对与照片-插图中描绘的组进行互动的愿望进行评分。参与者普遍报告说,与较温暖和较少占主导地位的群体互动的愿望更大。对负面评价有较高恐惧的人报告说,与温暖群体互动的愿望更高,那些对积极评价有较高恐惧的人报告说,与较少占主导地位的群体互动的愿望更高。我们没有找到任何支持我们的假设,即种族认同更强的人会表现出更大的愿望与更相似的种族群体互动。讨论了治疗的意义和进一步研究的方向。
    Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is associated with interpersonal impairment. One possible reason for this dysfunction is that people with SAD evaluate others differently on dimensions of warmth and dominance compared to individuals without the disorder. In the current study, we examined whether two core constructs of SAD, fear of negative evaluation and fear of positive evaluation, affect the judgments that people make about groups based on warmth and dominance. We also investigated whether racial similarity (i.e., whether someone is the same race as those they\'re interacting with) and ethnic identity (i.e., one\'s sense of belonging to a particular social group) played a role in the types of evaluations people made. We created vignettes about groups varying in warmth and dominance, as well as photos varying in racial makeup. We presented photo-vignette pairs to participants and asked them to rate their desire to interact with the groups depicted in the photo-vignette. Participants in general reported greater desire to interact with warmer and less dominant groups. People with higher fear of negative evaluation reported higher desire for interaction with warmer groups, and those with higher fear of positive evaluation reported higher desire to interact with less dominant groups. We did not find any support for our hypothesis that people with stronger ethnic identity would show greater desire to interact with groups that were more similar to their race. Implications for treatment and directions for further research are discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尽管欺骗盛行,人们很少怀疑别人的真诚。然而,即使没有对真实性的怀疑,对说谎者和说实话者的间接评估也可能有所不同。在三项研究中,我们为两个目标刺激样本和三个参与者法官样本中的真理吸引效应提供了证据。目标人在说实话时与说谎时相比更具吸引力,一种由目标温暖和开放性介导的效应。真相吸引效应对女性目标更强(与男性);然而,它不受法官性别的影响。研究结果表明,人们可能更有可能接近讲真话的人而不是说谎者,即使不积极判断真实性。我们讨论了在线性混合效应分析中将目标和参与者视为随机因素的挑战和好处,并加入了呼吁的合唱,以增加欺骗研究中目标刺激的数量。
    Despite the prevalence of deception, people rarely doubt others\' sincerity. However, indirect evaluations of liars and truth-tellers may differ even in the absence of suspicion about veracity. Across three studies, we provide evidence for the truth attraction effect in two samples of target stimuli and three samples of participant judges. Target people are perceived as more attractive when telling the truth versus when they lie, an effect mediated by target warmth and openness. The truth attraction effect is stronger for female targets (vs. males); however, it is unaffected by the gender of the judge. Findings suggest people may be more likely to approach truth-tellers versus liars, even when not actively judging veracity. We discuss the challenges and benefits of treating both targets and participants as random factors in linear mixed-effect analyses and join the chorus of calls to increase the number of target stimuli in deception research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究考察了父母的温暖作为父母对二元应对和青少年外部化结果的看法之间关系的中介。来自472名青少年的数据,母亲们,父亲是在三年的时间里从中国的家庭中收集的,肯尼亚,瑞典,和泰国。对于所有四个站点中的母亲和三个站点中的父亲,13岁时更好的父母二重应对预示着14岁时父母温暖的水平更高。对于所有四个网站的母亲来说,母亲温暖水平较高与15岁时青年外化行为较少相关,13岁时二元应对水平较高与15岁时青年外化行为较少相关。情绪安全理论有助于解释二元应对与青少年外化行为相关的过程。结果对基于父母和家庭的干预具有重要意义。
    This study examined parental warmth as a mediator of relations between mothers\' and fathers\' perceptions of dyadic coping and adolescent externalizing outcomes. Data from 472 adolescents, mothers, and fathers were collected over a three-year period from families in China, Kenya, Sweden, and Thailand. For mothers in all four sites and fathers in three sites, better parental dyadic coping at youth age 13 predicted higher levels of parental warmth at youth age 14. For mothers in all four sites, higher levels of maternal warmth were in turn related to less youth externalizing behavior at age 15, and higher levels of dyadic coping at youth age 13 were related to less youth externalizing behavior at age 15 indirectly through maternal warmth. Emotional Security Theory helps explain the process by which dyadic coping is related to adolescent externalizing behavior. The results have important implications for parent and family-based interventions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号