verbal ability

言语能力
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:关于青少年饮酒和认知的研究通常无法将饮酒对认知的潜在因果影响与共同的病因影响分开。包括遗传影响或其他物质使用合并症,也已知与认知有关,例如尼古丁的使用。本研究旨在填补这一空白,并通过考虑已测量和未测量的混杂因素来阐明青少年饮酒与青少年认知之间的关系。
    方法:使用考虑家庭嵌套的随机效应模型来控制测量的混杂因素。接下来,在完整样本和单卵双生对(MZ)中进行了双胞胎比较,以控制双胞胎共有的未测量的遗传和环境混杂因素。
    方法:参与者为812名个体(58.6%为女性,361个完整的对,146MZ对)来自芬兰的纵向FinnTwin12研究。
    方法:用14岁和17岁年龄段的平均使用频率和中毒率对青少年饮酒进行索引。认知结果在平均年龄22岁时进行测量,包括跟踪测试,加州Stroop测试,韦克斯勒成人智力子测验(词汇,块设计,数字符号),WechslerMemoryScale的数字跨度子测试,心理旋转测试和对象位置记忆测试。协变量包括性别,父母教育,一般认知能力,目前使用酒精和尼古丁。
    结果:整个青春期饮酒频率和中毒频率的增加与同胎对照[freq:stndbeta=-0.12,95%置信区间(CI)=-0.234,-0.013]和仅MZ同胎对照模型(freq:stndbeta=-0.0.95%CI=-0.523,-0.087%CI=0.5301,
    结论:在芬兰,似乎几乎没有证据表明青少年饮酒会导致成年后的认知缺陷,除了适度的证据表明青少年饮酒较高与年轻成人词汇得分较低有关。
    OBJECTIVE: Studies on adolescent alcohol use and cognition are often unable to separate the potential causal effects of alcohol use on cognition from shared etiological influences, including genetic influences or other substance use comorbidities also known to be associated with cognition, such as nicotine use. The present study aimed to fill this gap and clarify the relationship between adolescent alcohol use and young adult cognition by accounting for both measured and unmeasured confounders.
    METHODS: A random effects model accounting for nesting in families was used to control for measured confounders. Next, co-twin comparisons were conducted within the full sample and in monozygotic twin pairs (MZ) to control for unmeasured genetic and environmental confounders shared by co-twins.
    METHODS: Participants were 812 individuals (58.6% female, 361 complete pairs, 146 MZ pairs) from the longitudinal FinnTwin12 study in Finland.
    METHODS: Adolescent alcohol use was indexed with measures of frequency of use and intoxication averaged across ages 14 and 17. Cognitive outcomes were measured at average age 22 and included Trail Making Test, California Stroop test, Wechsler Adult Intelligence subtests (Vocabulary, Block Design, Digit Symbol), Digit Span subtest of Wechsler Memory Scale, Mental Rotation Test and Object Location Memory test. Covariates included sex, parental education, general cognitive ability, current alcohol use and nicotine use.
    RESULTS: Greater frequency of alcohol use and frequency of intoxication across adolescence was associated with decreased vocabulary scores in the co-twin control [freq: stnd beta = -0.12, 95% confidence interval (CI) = -0.234, -0.013] and MZ only co-twin control models (freq: stnd beta = -0.305, 95% CI = -0.523, -0.087; intox: stnd beta = -0.301, 95% CI = -0.528, -0.074).
    CONCLUSIONS: In Finland, there appears to be little evidence that adolescent alcohol use causes cognitive deficits in young adulthood, except modest evidence for association of higher adolescent alcohol use with lower young adult vocabulary scores.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:作者在多机构结节性硬化症(TSC)自闭症卓越研究网络(TACERN)研究中评估了癫痫手术时机对术后神经认知结果的影响。
    方法:TACERN队列中159例患者中有27例患有药物难治性癫痫并接受了手术。手术年龄范围为15.86至154.14周(中位数为91.93周)。在三个神经心理学测试中,患者术前第一次(10-58周)到术后最后一次(155-188周)得分的变化-早期学习的Mullen量表(MSEL),Vineland适应性行为量表,第2版(VABS-2),和学龄前语言量表,计算第5版(PLS-5)。使用Pearson相关性和多元线性回归模型分别将测试结果与手术年龄和手术前癫痫持续时间相关联。分别对癫痫发作负担减轻的患者(n=21)和癫痫发作负担没有减轻的患者(n=6)进行分析。回归分析特别针对21例成功控制癫痫发作的患者。
    结果:手术时的年龄与MSEL的组合言语子测验的变化呈显着负相关(R=-0.45,p=0.039),并在多元线性回归模型中预测了该得分(β=-0.09,p=0.035)。在PLS-5的总语言得分(R=-0.4,p=0.089;β=-0.12,p=0.014)以及在手术前检查癫痫持续时间的分析中也看到了类似的趋势。手术年龄和手术前癫痫持续时间与VABS-2语言评分变化之间的关联差异更大(R=-0.15,p=0.52;β=-0.05,p=0.482)。
    结论:早期手术和手术前癫痫持续时间较短与TSC患者术后语言改善有关。需要前瞻性或比较有效性临床试验来进一步阐明手术时机对神经认知结果的影响。
    OBJECTIVE: The authors evaluated the impact of the timing of epilepsy surgery on postoperative neurocognitive outcomes in a cohort of children followed in the multiinstitutional Tuberous Sclerosis Complex (TSC) Autism Center of Excellence Research Network (TACERN) study.
    METHODS: Twenty-seven of 159 patients in the TACERN cohort had drug-refractory epilepsy and underwent surgery. Ages at surgery ranged from 15.86 to 154.14 weeks (median 91.93 weeks). Changes in patients\' first preoperative (10-58 weeks) to last postoperative (155-188 weeks) scores on three neuropsychological tests-the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd edition (VABS-2), and the Preschool Language Scales, 5th edition (PLS-5)-were calculated. Pearson correlation and multivariate linear regression models were used to correlate test outcomes separately with age at surgery and duration of epilepsy prior to surgery. Analyses were separately conducted for patients whose seizure burdens decreased postoperatively (n = 21) and those whose seizure burdens did not (n = 6). Regression analysis was specifically focused on the 21 patients who achieved successful seizure control.
    RESULTS: Age at surgery was significantly negatively correlated with the change in the combined verbal subtests of the MSEL (R = -0.45, p = 0.039) and predicted this score in a multivariate linear regression model (β = -0.09, p = 0.035). Similar trends were seen in the total language score of the PLS-5 (R = -0.4, p = 0.089; β = -0.12, p = 0.014) and in analyses examining the duration of epilepsy prior to surgery as the independent variable of interest. Associations between age at surgery and duration of epilepsy prior to surgery with changes in the verbal subscores of VABS-2 were more variable (R = -0.15, p = 0.52; β = -0.05, p = 0.482).
    CONCLUSIONS: Earlier surgery and shorter epilepsy duration prior to surgery were associated with greater improvement in postoperative language in patients with TSC. Prospective or comparative effectiveness clinical trials are needed to further elucidate surgical timing impacts on neurocognitive outcomes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目标:研究表明,受过大学教育,在言语能力测试中,英语占主导地位的双语者相对于英语单一语言者表现不佳。我们调查了接受两种语言的回答是否会改善双语者的表现,以及这种改进是否足以证明与单语者相同的性能水平。方法:研究对象为同一所大学的大学生。在双语言语能力测试(BVAT)中,将西班牙语-英语双语者与英语单一语言者进行了比较,其中包括图片词汇,口语词汇,和言语类比。结果:如果有机会用西班牙语对英语失败的项目做出回应,双语者在所有三个子测试中获得了明显更高的分数,他们的表现与单语者在口语词汇和言语类比上的表现相当。结论:“任一语言”评分方法可以对双语者的言语能力进行最佳测量。我们提供了规范数据,用于在BVAT的子测试中应用两种语言的评分方法。我们将在临床评估的背景下讨论这些发现。
    Objective: Research demonstrates that college educated, English language dominant bilinguals underperform relative to English speaking monolinguals on tests of verbal ability. We investigated whether accepting responses in their two languages would reveal improved performance in bilinguals, and whether such improvement would be of sufficient magnitude to demonstrate the same performance level as monolinguals. Method: Participants were college students attending the same university. Spanish-English bilinguals were compared to English speaking monolinguals on the Bilingual Verbal Ability Tests (BVAT), which include Picture Vocabulary, Oral Vocabulary, and Verbal Analogies. Results: When given the opportunity to respond in Spanish to items failed in English, bilinguals obtained significantly higher scores on all three subtests, and their performance matched that of monolinguals on Oral Vocabulary and Verbal Analogies. Conclusion: An \"either-language\" scoring approach may enable optimal measurement of verbal abilities in bilinguals. We provide normative data for use in applying the either-language scoring approach on subtests of the BVAT. We discuss the findings in the context of clinical assessment.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)是一种神经发育疾病,通常与小脑结构异常有关。小脑灰质体积(GMV)是否与言语障碍有关仍存在争议。这里,研究了ASD患者小脑GMV与言语能力之间的关系。在患有ASD的男性个体(N=144,年龄:8.5-64.0岁)和神经典型对照(N=188;年龄:8.0-56.2岁)中,在ABIDEI数据集的结构性MRI扫描上进行小脑的小叶分割。逐步线性混合效应建模,包括组(ASD与神经典型对照),小叶GMV,和年龄进行了鉴定小脑小叶,这些小脑小叶最好地预测了通过言语智商(VIQ)测量的言语能力。相对于神经典型对照,在ASD中发现了双侧CrusII的VIQ和GMV之间的年龄特异性关联。在患有ASD的儿童中,较高的VIQ与左侧CrusII的GMV较大有关,而右侧CrusII的GMV较小。相比之下,在患有ASD的成年人中,较高的VIQ与左CrusII的GMV较小和右CrusII的GMV较大相关。这些发现表明,相对于对侧半球,最初对语言非特异性左脑半球的依赖被成年期更典型的右侧化所抵消.
    Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition frequently associated with structural cerebellar abnormalities. Whether cerebellar grey matter volumes (GMV) are linked to verbal impairments remains controversial. Here, the association between cerebellar GMV and verbal abilities in ASD was examined across the lifespan. Lobular segmentation of the cerebellum was performed on structural MRI scans from the ABIDE I dataset in male individuals with ASD (N=144, age: 8.5-64.0 years) and neurotypical controls (N=188; age: 8.0-56.2 years). Stepwise linear mixed effects modeling including group (ASD vs. neurotypical controls), lobule-wise GMV, and age was performed to identify cerebellar lobules which best predicted verbal abilities as measured by verbal IQ (VIQ). An age-specific association between VIQ and GMV of bilateral Crus II was found in ASD relative to neurotypical controls. In children with ASD, higher VIQ was associated with larger GMV of left Crus II but smaller GMV of right Crus II. By contrast, in adults with ASD, higher VIQ was associated with smaller GMV of left Crus II and larger GMV of right Crus II. These findings indicate that relative to the contralateral hemisphere, an initial reliance on the language-nonspecific left cerebellar hemisphere is offset by more typical right-lateralization in adulthood.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究的主要目的,通过两次重复测量,在5岁的78名西班牙参与者的样本中分析自传记忆的发展(时间1;M=62.43个月,范围:50-74个月)和12个(时间2;M=142.71个月,范围:132-155个月)。收集了有关自传记忆和言语功能的数据。我们从纵向角度分析了语言与自传体记忆特异性之间的关系,并评估了词汇在两个时间瞬间的年龄与特定记忆之间的关系中的间接影响。结果表明,语言技能与学龄前自传体记忆特异性呈正相关,但不是在第二次测量时。此外,词汇得分似乎调解年龄和自传体特异性之间的关系,当儿童在学龄前,但不是以后。这些发现与先前的研究一致,认为学龄前是自传体记忆发展及其与语言关系的关键时期,但是一旦掌握了语言的基本命令,语言差异对自传特异性的个体差异的影响要小得多。
    The main aim of this study, with two repeated measurements, was to analyze the development of autobiographical memory in a sample of 78 Spanish participants at ages 5 (Time 1; M = 62.43 months, range: 50-74 months) and 12 (Time 2; M = 142.71 months, range: 132-155 months). Data were collected on autobiographical memory and verbal functions. We analyzed the relation between language and autobiographical memory specificity from a longitudinal perspective and assessed the indirect effect of vocabulary in the relationship between age and specific memory at both temporal moments. The results showed that language skills were positively related with autobiographical memory specificity at preschool age, but not at the second measurement. Furthermore, vocabulary scores appear to mediate the relationship between age and autobiographical specificity when children are in the preschool years, but not later. These findings agree with previous research that consider preschool age to be a crucial period for the development of autobiographical memory and its relations with language, but once basic command of language is acquired, linguistic differences impact much less on individual differences in autobiographical specificity.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)被定义为在神经系统领域广泛研究的神经发育障碍。最近,为了研究ASD患者的脑形态学改变,已经进行了神经影像学研究,在该疾病的临床表现之前表现出非典型的大脑发育。本研究调查了ASD儿童的皮质厚度(CT)和局部玻璃化指数(LGI)分布,目的是评估大脑测量与个体特征之间的可能关系(即,智商和言语能力)。获取并处理来自129个ASD和58个年龄匹配的健康对照(HC)的3DT1-w序列,以评估每个受试者的CT和LGI。调查了ASD和HC之间的组间差异,包括对2个ASD亚组的分析,根据患者的言语能力和智商进行拆分。与HC相比,ASD显示几个大脑区域的CT和LGI增加,作为一个整体群体和作为言语能力的智商亚组。此外,在比较ASD受试者的语言特点时,那些具有言语能力的患者表现出明显的CT,并且在右半球的枕叶内发现LGI增加。根据智商值比较ASD患者时,没有显着结果。这些结果支持自儿童早期以来ASD中异常脑成熟的假设,临床亚组之间的差异表明,异常连接的潜在解剖基础不同。
    Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is defined as a neurodevelopmental disorder largely investigated in the neurologic field. Recently, neuroimaging studies have been conducted in order to investigate cerebral morphologic alterations in ASD patients, demonstrating an atypical brain development before the clinical manifestations of the disorder. Cortical Thickness (CT) and Local Gyrification Index (LGI) distribution for ASD children were investigated in this study, with the aim to evaluate possible relationship between brain measures and individual characteristics (i.e., IQ and verbal ability). 3D T1-w sequences from 129 ASD and 58 age-matched Healthy Controls (HC) were acquired and processed in order to assess CT and LGI for each subject. Intergroup differences between ASD and HC were investigated, including analyses of 2 ASD subgroups, split according to patient verbal ability and IQ. When compared to HC, ASD showed increased CT and LGI within several brain areas, both as an overall group and as verbal ability an IQ subgroups. Moreover, when comparing language characteristics of the ASD subjects, those patients with verbal ability exhibit significant CT and LGI increase was found within the occipital lobe of right hemisphere. No significant results occurred when comparing ASD patients according to their IQ value. These results support the hypothesis of abnormal brain maturation in ASD since early childhood with differences among clinical subgroups suggesting different anatomical substrates underlying an aberrant connectivity.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    关于弗林效应的研究,智商的长期上升,主要基于连续男性出生队列的应征数据。这不可避免地意味着两种不同的现象是混合的:智商组的生育率差异(“组成弗林效应”),以及父母和子女之间的任何差异(“家庭内弗林效应”)。两者都会影响认知能力的趋势。我们专注于后一种现象,在一代人的时间内探索青春期认知能力的变化,在同一个家庭的两代人之间。我们在斯德哥尔摩多代研究中确定了三个关联世代的决定因素和结果。学校和应征入伍的数据涵盖了13岁母亲,13岁和18岁父亲以及18岁儿子的逻辑/数字和言语得分。原始分数,以及原始分数的变化,在线性回归中用作结果。父母双方在13岁时的能力对儿子在18岁时的能力同样重要。来自弱势背景的男孩在青春期赶上了其他男孩。比较父亲和儿子,在逻辑/数字和言语能力方面似乎存在积极的弗林效应。如果父亲有工人阶级背景或许多兄弟姐妹,这个数字更大。弗林效应仅在父亲18岁时一般认知能力较低的家庭中可见。我们得出的结论是,从一代到下一代,逻辑/数字和口头技能都有普遍的提高,主要基于弱势家庭的改善。20世纪后期瑞典的弗林效应似乎代表了社会类别之间的缩小。
    Research about the Flynn effect, the secular rise in IQ, is heavily based on conscript data from successive male birth cohorts. This inevitably means that two distinct phenomena are mixed: fertility differences by IQ group (\'compositional Flynn effect\'), and any difference between parents and children (\'within-family Flynn effect\'). Both will influence trends in cognitive ability. We focused on the latter phenomenon, exploring changes in cognitive abilities during adolescence within one generation, and between two successive generations within the same family. We identified determinants and outcomes in three linked generations in the Stockholm Multigenerational Study. School and conscript data covered logical/numerical and verbal scores for mothers at age 13, fathers at 13 and 18, and their sons at 18. Raw scores, and change in raw scores, were used as outcomes in linear regressions. Both parents\' abilities at 13 were equally important for sons\' abilities at 18. Boys from disadvantaged backgrounds caught up with other boys during adolescence. Comparing fathers with sons, there appeared to be a positive Flynn effect in logical/numeric and verbal abilities. This was larger if the father had a working-class background or many siblings. A Flynn effect was only visible in families where the father had low general cognitive ability at 18. We conclude that there is a general improvement in logical/numeric and verbal skills from one generation to the next, primarily based on improvement in disadvantaged families. The Flynn effect in Sweden during the later 20th century appears to represent a narrowing between social categories.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    使用空间线索,如形状,定位,和模式有助于视觉空间工作记忆,因为它允许减少这种认知资源负荷的策略。一个这样的战略,即利用模式化的空间分布,迄今为止仍未得到充分研究。此策略要求跟踪已搜索的位置,并将其排除在进一步搜索之外,因此与视觉空间工作记忆相关。使用这种策略应该,原则上,在儿童早期发展,但是因为大多数研究都集中在分块上,在幼儿中,对减少工作记忆负担的其他策略的开发研究不足。因此,在这项研究中,我们测试了2至4.5岁(N=97)的儿童是否可以在搜索中利用空间线索,以及这种能力是否与他们的年龄相关,言语能力,和视觉空间工作记忆。结果表明,使用模式化空间分布(从一侧到另一侧搜索一行位置而不是随机搜索)的能力随着视觉空间工作记忆而显着提高,而不是年龄或言语能力。这些结果表明,视觉空间能力可能会在2至4.5岁之间迅速发展。考虑到它们对后来数学成就的影响,认知发展研究和幼儿教育的需求增加。
    Using spatial cues such as shape, orientation, and pattern aids visuospatial working memory because it allows strategies that reduce the load on this cognitive resource. One such strategy, namely taking advantage of patterned spatial distributions, remains understudied to date. This strategy demands keeping track of already-searched locations and excluding them from further search and so correlates with visuospatial working memory. The use of such strategies should, in principle, develop in early childhood, but because most studies focus on chunking, the development of other strategies reducing the load on working memory is understudied in young children. Therefore, in this study we tested whether children aged 2 to 4.5 years (N = 97) could take advantage of spatial cues in their search and whether this ability correlated with their age, verbal ability, and visuospatial working memory. The results showed that the ability to use a patterned spatial distribution (searching a row of locations from one side to the other instead of a random search) significantly improved with visuospatial working memory but not with age or verbal ability. These results suggest that visuospatial abilities may rapidly develop from 2 to 4.5 years of age, and given their impact on later mathematic achievement, demand increased attention in cognitive developmental research and early childhood education.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    威廉姆斯综合征(WS)是一种罕见的遗传综合征。和所有罕见综合征一样,获得足够动力的样本量是一个挑战。在这里,我们展示了来自七个英国实验室的遗留数据,能够在迄今为止最大的WS个体样本中表征言语和非言语发育的横截面和纵向发育轨迹。在研究1中,我们报告了N=102和N=209患有WS的儿童和成人之间关于言语和非言语能力测量的横截面数据。在研究2中,我们报告了从N=17到N=54患有WS的儿童和成人的纵向数据,这些数据至少在这些测量的三个时间点进行了测试。数据支持语言能力强于非语言能力的WS特征认知概况,和两个领域的浅层发育进展。横截面和纵向数据都表明,在我们的样本中,儿童参与者的发育速度比青少年和成年人更快。横截面数据表明,语言能力的发展比非语言能力的发展更陡,语言和非语言能力之间的差异在很大程度上取决于智力功能水平。言语和非言语能力之间的发展差异,虽然边缘,在纵向数据中没有统计镜像。参考使用纵向数据验证横截面发育模式以及个体差异在理解发育过程中的重要性,讨论了横截面和纵向数据。
    Williams syndrome (WS) is a rare genetic syndrome. As with all rare syndromes, obtaining adequately powered sample sizes is a challenge. Here we present legacy data from seven UK labs, enabling the characterisation of cross-sectional and longitudinal developmental trajectories of verbal and non-verbal development in the largest sample of individuals with WS to-date. In Study 1, we report cross-sectional data between N = 102 and N = 209 children and adults with WS on measures of verbal and non-verbal ability. In Study 2, we report longitudinal data from N = 17 to N = 54 children and adults with WS who had been tested on at least three timepoints on these measures. Data support the WS characteristic cognitive profile of stronger verbal than non-verbal ability, and shallow developmental progression for both domains. Both cross-sectional and longitudinal data demonstrate steeper rates of development in the child participants than the adolescent and adults in our sample. Cross-sectional data indicate steeper development in verbal than non-verbal ability, and that individual differences in the discrepancy between verbal and non-verbal ability are largely accounted for by level of intellectual functioning. A diverging developmental discrepancy between verbal and non-verbal ability, whilst marginal, is not mirrored statistically in the longitudinal data. Cross-sectional and longitudinal data are discussed with reference to validating cross-sectional developmental patterns using longitudinal data and the importance of individual differences in understanding developmental progression.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    经历家庭功能障碍的儿童通常会报告更多的发育问题和较低的教育程度。一个问题,然而,这些较低的结果是否是由家庭功能障碍本身引起的,或其他(预先存在的)因素,比如在贫困中长大。基于大家庭压力模型,我们得出了家庭功能障碍对儿童发育的影响的假设。此外,我们考虑了父母财力在家庭功能障碍对儿童发育影响中的中介和调节作用。我们研究了这些关系,同时使用英国千年队列研究的数据并采用描述性和固定效应分析,严格考虑了差异选择以经历家庭功能障碍。我们发现,在5岁以后经历家庭功能障碍的儿童在经历这些经历之前已经有更多的行为问题。这强调了在经历家庭功能障碍时考虑差异选择的重要性。我们还发现,从5岁以后开始的家庭功能障碍会导致更多的行为问题,但不会影响儿童的言语能力。家庭功能失调后,父母的经济资源下降,尤其是高收入家庭。然而,我们发现只有微弱的证据表明金融资源的中介作用,财政资源的大幅下降并没有转化为高收入家庭儿童家庭功能障碍的更大后果。因此,财政资源似乎主要在选择经历家庭功能障碍方面发挥重要作用。
    Children who experience household dysfunction often report more developmental problems and lower educational attainment. A question, however, is whether these lower outcomes are caused by the household dysfunction itself, or by other (pre-existing) factors, such as growing up in poverty. Based on the extended family stress model, we derived hypotheses on the consequences of household dysfunction for child development. Furthermore, we considered the mediating and moderating role of parents\' financial resources in the impact of household dysfunction on children\'s development. We studied these relationships while rigorously accounting for differential selection into experiencing household dysfunction using data from the British Millennium Cohort Study and employing descriptive and fixed-effects analyses. We found that children who experienced household dysfunction after age 5 already had more behavioural problems prior to these experiences. This underscores the importance of accounting for differential selection into experiencing household dysfunction. We also found that household dysfunction beginning after age 5 led to more behavioural problems but did not impact children\'s verbal ability. Parents\' financial resources declined after household dysfunction, particularly among high-income households. However, we found only weak evidence of a mediating effect of financial resources, and larger declines in financial resources did not translate into larger consequences of household dysfunction among children from high-income households. Financial resources thus mainly seemed to play an important role for selection into experiencing household dysfunction.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号