stop-signal

停止信号
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    伏核(NAc)被认为通过传递奖励预测线索的价值和预期奖励的传递来促进动机行为。NAc被细分为核心和外壳,每个区域都包含不同的神经元群体,这些神经元增加或减少对奖励事件的激发。虽然关于这些子区域和细胞类型的功能有各种各样的理论,大多数都是在奖励处理的背景下,考虑到NAc可能更广泛地服务于与行动选择相关的功能的情况较少。我们记录了NAc中的单个神经元,因为男女大鼠都执行了STOP变化任务,该任务通常用于研究运动控制和冲动性。在这项任务中,在80%的试验(GO)中,大鼠对空间提示反应迅速,并且在20%的试验(STOP)中必须停止并重新定向计划的运动。我们发现奖励兴奋神经元的活动标志着GO上准确的反应方向,但不是停止试验,并且这些神经元在正确的试验后表现出更高的预提示放电,导致在正确的GO试验和错误的STOP试验中更强的放电,而受奖赏抑制的神经元在仪器反应时在STOP试验中显着代表反应方向。最后,奖励兴奋与奖励抑制神经元的比例和预提示放电的强度随着电极穿过NAc而降低。我们得出结论,奖励激发细胞(在核心中更常见)促进主动行动选择,而奖励抑制细胞(在壳中更常见)有助于在需要反应性抑制和行为重定向的STOP试验中做出准确反应。重要陈述适当适应行为的能力是人类认知的重要组成部分,和一个被许多神经精神疾病扰乱。在这里,我们记录了大鼠进行认知控制任务时的伏隔核(NAc)中的神经元,并发现了细胞类型和子区域特定的放电模式。核心和奖励激发细胞追踪试验结果历史,主动将行为驱动到第一个线索-这是一个适合大多数试验的策略。相反,外壳和奖励抑制神经元在需要重定向行为的试验中发出准确的反应方向。一起,这些数据表明NAc神经元群体对作用选择有不同的贡献.
    The nucleus accumbens (NAc) is thought to contribute to motivated behavior by signaling the value of reward-predicting cues and the delivery of anticipated reward. The NAc is subdivided into core and shell, with each region containing different populations of neurons that increase or decrease firing to rewarding events. While there are numerous theories of functions pertaining to these subregions and cell types, most are in the context of reward processing, with fewer considering that the NAc might serve functions related to action selection more generally. We recorded from single neurons in the NAc as rats of both sexes performed a STOP-change task that is commonly used to study motor control and impulsivity. In this task, rats respond quickly to a spatial cue on 80% of trials (GO) and must stop and redirect planned movement on 20% of trials (STOP). We found that the activity of reward-excited neurons signaled accurate response direction on GO, but not STOP, trials and that these neurons exhibited higher precue firing after correct trials. In contrast, reward-inhibited neurons significantly represented response direction on STOP trials at the time of the instrumental response. Finally, the proportion of reward-excited to reward-inhibited neurons and the strength of precue firing decreased as the electrode traversed the NAc. We conclude that reward-excited cells (more common in core) promote proactive action selection, while reward-inhibited cells (more common in shell) contribute to accurate responding on STOP trials that require reactive suppression and redirection of behavior.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    历史上,眶额叶皮层(OFC)涉及多种行为,从逆转学习和抑制控制到更复杂的奖励值和任务空间表示。虽然对OFC功能的现代解释集中在结果评估中的作用,这些认知过程通常需要有机体来抑制适应不良的反应或策略。在执行停止改变任务的大鼠中,来自OFC的单个单位记录表明,OFC对STOP试验反应强烈。为了研究OFC在停止改变性能中的作用,我们在OFC的兴奋性神经元中表达了halorhodopsin(eNpHR3.0),并在停止改变任务中测试了大鼠。先前的工作表明,OFC根据试验序列区分STOP试验(即,gS试验:STOP试验之前是GO和sS试验:STOP试验之前是STOP)。我们发现,表达eNpHR3.0的神经元的黄光激活仅在GO试验(gS试验)之后的STOP试验中显着降低了准确性。Further,OFC的光遗传学抑制加快了错误试验的反应时间。这表明OFC在抑制控制过程中起作用,并且在对OFC功能的现代解释中需要考虑这一角色。重要性声明眶额皮质(OFC)是一个高度相互联系的大脑区域,被认为对健康的认知功能至关重要。历史上,研究表明OFC抑制了不适当的行为,然而,最近的工作集中在其在指导基于价值的决策中的作用。结合光遗传学使用经典抑制控制任务的修改版本,我们表明,虽然对基于价值的决策很重要,OFC中兴奋性神经元的破坏也会损害抑制控制过程。这凸显了对OFC功能的理论解释以适应其在两种类型的认知过程中的作用的必要性。
    Historically, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) has been implicated in a variety of behaviors ranging from reversal learning and inhibitory control to more complex representations of reward value and task space. While modern interpretations of the OFC\'s function have focused on a role in outcome evaluation, these cognitive processes often require an organism to inhibit a maladaptive response or strategy. Single-unit recordings from the OFC in rats performing a stop-change task show that the OFC responds strongly to STOP trials. To investigate the role that the OFC plays in stop-change performance, we expressed halorhodopsin (eNpHR3.0) in excitatory neurons in the OFC and tested rats on the stop-change task. Previous work suggests that the OFC differentiates between STOP trials based on trial sequence (i.e., gS trials: STOP trials preceded by a GO vs sS trials: STOP trials preceded by a STOP). We found that yellow light activation of the eNpHR3.0-expressing neurons significantly decreased accuracy only on STOP trials that followed GO trials (gS trials). Further, optogenetic inhibition of the OFC speeded reaction times on error trials. This suggests that the OFC plays a role in inhibitory control processes and that this role needs to be accounted for in modern interpretations of OFC function.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    抑制已经启动的响应的能力对于导航环境至关重要。然而,尚不清楚哪些特征使停止信号更有可能得到有效处理。在连续的三项研究中,我们证明停止信号模式和位置是影响反应性反应抑制的关键因素。研究1表明,在视觉选择反应任务中,与视觉停止信号相比,触觉停止信号具有更好的性能。研究2的结果表明,停止信号的位置很重要。具体来说,如果视觉停止信号出现在与视觉前进信号不同的位置,然后停止性能增强。扩展这些结果,研究3表明,当视觉干扰物出现在前进信号的位置时,触觉停止信号和位置不同的视觉停止信号保持其性能增强效果。总之,这些结果证实了停止信号模式和位置影响反应反应抑制,即使面对并发的干扰者。未来的研究可能会将这些发现扩展并推广到其他跨模式设置。
    The ability to inhibit an already initiated response is crucial for navigating the environment. However, it is unclear which characteristics make stop-signals more likely to be processed efficiently. In three consecutive studies, we demonstrate that stop-signal modality and location are key factors that influence reactive response inhibition. Study 1 shows that tactile stop-signals lead to better performance compared to visual stop-signals in an otherwise visual choice-reaction task. Results of Study 2 reveal that the location of the stop-signal matters. Specifically, if a visual stop-signal is presented at a different location compared to the visual go-signal, then stopping performance is enhanced. Extending these results, study 3 suggests that tactile stop-signals and location-distinct visual stop-signals retain their performance enhancing effect when visual distractors are presented at the location of the go-signal. In sum, these results confirm that stop-signal modality and location influence reactive response inhibition, even in the face of concurrent distractors. Future research may extend and generalize these findings to other cross-modal setups.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    类似于成瘾性障碍,认知控制缺陷可能与暴饮暴食症(BED)的发生和发展有关。然而,目前尚不清楚一般或食物相关的抑制控制损害是否与暴饮暴食和暴饮暴食行为基本相关。因此,本研究旨在研究暴饮暴食行为个体中与食物相关的抑制控制的行为表现和电生理相关性。
    使用典型的停止信号任务评估了60名可能BED(pBED)和60名匹配良好的健康对照(HCs)的个体,修订后的Go/NoGo任务,和食物相关的Go/NoGo任务。此外,另一个单独的样本,包括35名pBED和35名HCs,当用事件相关电位(ERP)记录EEG信号时,完成了与食物相关的Go/NoGo任务。
    数据显示,与HC相比,pBED组在停止信号任务的SSRT更长的情况下表现较差(Cohen\sd=0.58,p=0.002)。此外,与食物相关的Go/NoGo任务,pBED组在无效试验中的抑制成功率较低(Cohen'sd=0.47,p=0.012).ERP数据显示,与HC相比,pBED组在与食物相关的Go/NoGo任务的no-go试验中表现出增加的P300潜伏期(FC1,FC2,F3,F4,FZ)(Cohen'sd0.56-0.73,均p<0.05)。
    这些研究结果表明,暴饮暴食的个体在非特异性和食物相关的抑制控制方面都可能受损,与食物相关的抑制控制的损害可能与P300异常有关,暗示与BED有关的行为神经生物学功能障碍机制。
    UNASSIGNED: Similar to addictive disorders, deficits on cognitive control might be involved in the onset and development of Binge Eating Disorder (BED). However, it remains unclear whether general or food-related inhibitory control impairments would be basically linked to overeating and binge eating behaviors. This study thus aimed to investigate behavioral performance and electrophysiological correlates of food-related inhibitory control among individuals with binge eating behavior.
    UNASSIGNED: Sixty individuals with probable BED (pBED) and 60 well-matched healthy controls (HCs) were assessed using the typical Stop-Signal Task, a revised Go/No Go Task, and a food-related Go/No Go Task. Besides, another separate sample, including 35 individuals with pBED and 35 HCs, completed the food-related Go/No Go Task when EEG signals were recorded with the event-related potentials (ERPs).
    UNASSIGNED: The data revealed that the pBED group performed worse with a longer SSRT on the Stop-Signal Task compared with HCs (Cohen\'s d = 0.58, p = 0.002). Moreover, on the food-related Go/No Go Task, the pBED group had a lower success rate of inhibition in no-go trials (Cohen\'s d = 0.47, p = 0.012). The ERPs data showed that in comparison with HCs, the pBED group exhibited increased P300 latency (FC1, FC2, F3, F4, FZ) in the no-go trials of the food-related Go/No Go Task (Cohen\'s d 0.56-0.73, all p < 0.05).
    UNASSIGNED: These findings suggested that individuals with binge eating could be impaired in both non-specific and food-related inhibitory control aspects, and the impairments in food-related inhibitory control might be linked to P300 abnormalities, implying a behavioral-neurobiological dysfunction mechanism implicated in BED.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    抑制是人类用来实现目标导向行为的关键认知控制机制。当快速施加时,抑制控制具有广泛的,非选择性运动效应,通常使用经颅磁刺激(TMS)引起的皮质脊髓兴奋性测量(CSE)来证明。例如,在快速停止行动期间,CSE在停止和与任务无关的肌肉处都受到抑制。虽然这种基于TMS的CSE测量为抑制控制基础的前基底神经节电路提供了重要的见解,他们有几个缺点。TMS在许多人群中是禁忌的(例如,癫痫或深部脑刺激患者),有限的时间分辨率,产生分散注意力的听觉和触觉刺激,很难与其他成像方法结合,并且需要昂贵的,不动的设备。这里,我们尝试使用不受这些缺点影响的方法来测量抑制控制的非选择性运动效应.30名男性和女性参与者在执行脚部响应停止信号任务时,在高精度手持式力传感器上施加了等距力。的确,当脚部运动成功停止时,与任务无关的手的力输出也被抑制了。此外,这种非选择性等轴力降低与停止信号表现高度相关,并显示出与神经和肌肉记录中通常发现的既定抑制特征相似的频率动力学.一起,这些发现表明,等距力记录可以可靠地捕获运动抑制的非选择性效应,打开大门,许多应用程序是很难或不可能实现与TMS。
    Inhibition is a key cognitive control mechanism humans use to enable goal-directed behavior. When rapidly exerted, inhibitory control has broad, nonselective motor effects, typically demonstrated using corticospinal excitability measurements (CSE) elicited by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). For example, during rapid action-stopping, CSE is suppressed at both stopped and task-unrelated muscles. While such TMS-based CSE measurements have provided crucial insights into the fronto-basal ganglia circuitry underlying inhibitory control, they have several downsides. TMS is contraindicated in many populations (e.g., epilepsy or deep-brain stimulation patients), has limited temporal resolution, produces distracting auditory and haptic stimulation, is difficult to combine with other imaging methods, and necessitates expensive, immobile equipment. Here, we attempted to measure the nonselective motor effects of inhibitory control using a method unaffected by these shortcomings. Thirty male and female human participants exerted isometric force on a high-precision handheld force transducer while performing a foot-response stop-signal task. Indeed, when foot movements were successfully stopped, force output at the task-irrelevant hand was suppressed as well. Moreover, this nonselective reduction of isometric force was highly correlated with stop-signal performance and showed frequency dynamics similar to established inhibitory signatures typically found in neural and muscle recordings. Together, these findings demonstrate that isometric force recordings can reliably capture the nonselective effects of motor inhibition, opening the door to many applications that are hard or impossible to realize with TMS.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)与包括干扰控制在内的抑制功能缺陷有关,抑制强效/自动反应和抑制已经启动的反应。这项研究使用功能近红外光谱(fNIRS)来研究这三种形式的抑制的神经基础,这是通过最近开发的行为方案评估的,该方案结合了25名注意力不集中的年轻人的Stroop匹配/停止信号任务,冲动和/或多动症状。使用成人ADHD自我报告量表(ASRS)测量ADHD症状的严重程度。在背外侧前额叶皮质(DLPFC)中评估氧合血红蛋白(HbO)的浓度,Stroop匹配/停止信号任务期间的额下回(IFG)和颞顶区域(TP)。在使用停止信号任务的阻滞期间,ASRS评分与额叶区域的HbO浓度之间存在显着相关性,即正确的IFG,左DLPFC和左IFG。这项研究表明,在抑制对ADHD症状严重程度的持续反应期间,不同类型的抑制涉及独特的额叶和颞顶活动以及相关的额叶功能障碍。
    Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with deficits in inhibitory functions including interference control, inhibition of prepotent/automatic responses and suppression of already initiated responses. This study used functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to investigate the neural basis of these three forms of inhibition assessed by a recently developed behavioral protocol combining the Stroop-matching/stop-signal task in twenty-five young adults with inattention, impulsivity and/or hyperactivity symptoms. The severity of ADHD symptoms was measured using the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS). The concentration of oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO) was assessed in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and temporoparietal regions (TP) during the Stroop-matching/stop-signal task. Correlations yielded significant associations between ASRS scores and HbO concentration in frontal regions during blocks with stop-signal tasks, namely the right IFG, the left DLPFC and the left IFG. This study revealed that different types of inhibition involve unique frontal and temporoparietal activities and linked frontal dysfunction during the suppression of ongoing responses to the severity of ADHD symptoms.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    抑制或适应不必要的动作或运动的能力是几乎所有行为形式的关键特征。许多人将这种能力归因于额脑区域,例如前扣带回皮质(ACC)和内侧前额叶皮质(mPFC)。但是每个大脑区域的确切贡献经常被争论,因为它们的功能在执行相同任务的动物中没有被检查。最近,我们已经证明,ACC信号需要认知控制,并且对于执行停转任务的大鼠的背内侧纹状体(DMS)中动作选择信号的适应至关重要。这里,我们表明,与ACC不同,mPFC的边缘前区域不会在行为水平或DMS中的下游射击水平上破坏行动计划的抑制或适应。相反,mPFC的病变与DMS信号的变化相关,DMS信号参与GO试验的启动和破坏性能,同时改善STOP试验的性能.
    The ability to inhibit or adapt unwanted actions or movements is a critical feature of almost all forms of behavior. Many have attributed this ability to frontal brain areas such as the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), but the exact contribution of each brain region is often debated because their functions are not examined in animals performing the same task. Recently, we have shown that ACC signals a need for cognitive control and is crucial for the adaptation of action selection signals in dorsomedial striatum (DMS) in rats performing a stop-change task. Here, we show that unlike ACC, the prelimbic region of mPFC does not disrupt the inhibition or adaption of an action plan at either the level of behavior or downstream firing in DMS. Instead, lesions to mPFC correlate with changes in DMS signals involved in action initiation and disrupt performance on GO trials while improving performance on STOP trials.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    反应抑制中的缺陷,定义为无法阻止不再相关的行为,是创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)的特征。鉴于受损的反应抑制与更差的症状恢复有关,并且越来越多的证据表明认知控制训练在减少PTSD症状方面的有效性,具有中度至重度PTSD总严重程度(创伤后诊断量表总分≥21)和训练前反应抑制缺陷(M≤75%成功抑制Go/No-go)的个体完成了3小时,自适应Go/No-Go训练,旨在提高抑制强效运动反应的能力。然后,49名参与者随机接受适应性反应抑制训练(n=24,M=19.27年,SD=0.70)或等待名单条件(n=25,M=18.31年,SD=4.80)。在训练前和训练后评估行为反应抑制和自我报告的创伤相关症状。与等待组相比,反应抑制训练与未经训练的转移停止信号任务的反应抑制和PTSD症状减轻有关,在培训后。有,然而,从训练前到训练后,减少了对修改后的Go/No-Go任务的抑制作用。总的来说,反应抑制缺陷和PTSD症状可以使用反应抑制训练进行自上而下的补救。我们的研究为以反应抑制缺陷为特征的PTSD样本中反应抑制训练的可行性提供了初步证据。
    Deficits in response inhibition, defined as an inability to stop a behavior that is no longer relevant, are characteristic of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Given that impaired response inhibition is associated with worse symptom recovery and accumulating evidence pointing to the effectiveness of cognitive control trainings in reducing PTSD symptoms, individuals with moderate to severe PTSD total severity (Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale total score ≥ 21) and pre-training response inhibition deficits (M ≤ 75% successful inhibition on the Go/No-go) completed a 3-h, adaptive Go/No-go training designed to improve ability to withhold prepotent motor responses. Then forty-nine participants were randomized to an adaptive response inhibition training (n = 24, M = 19.27 years, SD = 0.70) or a waitlist condition (n = 25, M = 18.31 years, SD = 4.80). Behavioral response inhibition and self-reported trauma-related symptoms were assessed at pre- and post-training. Response inhibition training was associated with improved response inhibition on an untrained transfer Stop-Signal task and symptom reduction in PTSD compared to a waitlist group, at post-training. There was, however, reduced inhibition on a modified Go/No-go task from pre-to post-training. Overall, response inhibition deficits and PTSD symptoms are amenable to top-down remediation using response inhibition training. Our study provides preliminary evidence for the feasibility of response inhibition training in a PTSD sample characterized by response inhibition deficits.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在人类运动控制期间,运动控制的三个途径协调完成人的反应和抑制控制,因此,不同类型的运动技能训练是否会影响运动控制的三种途径是本研究的主要问题。磁共振成像与行为评估相结合,分析不同专项训练对额叶和基底节运动控制网络的影响,探讨中枢神经系统在运动行为调节中的作用。停止信号范式用于测量反应和抑制能力,功能磁共振成像用于全脑扫描,收集静息状态数据。与对照组相比,竞技健美操运动员有更好的反应能力,而足球运动员有更好的反应能力和抑制控制能力。此外,我们发现,两组技能训练导致不同静息状态的脑功能参数与对照组相比有显著差异。此外,在功能连接方面,三组之间的运动控制的直接和间接途径存在显著差异.开放式技能训练可以提高反应能力,而封闭式技能训练可以提高反应和抑制能力。这些结果表明,右额下回和左壳核之间的功能连接的强度可能是提高抑制性的关键,左辅助运动区-双侧丘脑回路可能在运动控制中起抑制作用。
    During human motor control, the three pathways of motor control coordinate to complete human response and inhibition control, so whether different types of motor skills training will affect the three pathways of motor control is the main question in this study. Magnetic resonance imaging was combined with behavioural evaluation to analyse the effects of different special training sessions on the motor control network of the frontal lobe and basal ganglia and to explore the role of the central nervous system in the regulation of motor behaviour. A Stop-signal paradigm was used to measure reaction and inhibition capacity, functional magnetic resonance imaging was used for whole brain scanning, and resting state data were collected. Compared to the control group, the competitive aerobics athletes had better reflexes while the soccer players had both better reflexes and inhibitory control. Furthermore, we found that training in the two sets of skills resulted in significant differences in different resting state brain function parameters compared with the control group. Additionally, there were significant differences among the three groups in the direct and indirect pathways of motor control in terms of functional connectivity. Open skill training may improve reaction ability while closed skill training improve both reaction and inhibition ability. These results suggest that the strength of the functional connectivity between the right inferior frontal gyrus and the left putamen may be a key to improving the inhibitory, and the left supplementary motor area- bilateral thalamic loop may play an inhibitory role in motor control.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对思想的抑制控制,情感,对于患有创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)的人来说,行动是具有挑战性的。在PTSD中,抑制性控制的特定方面是否受到差异影响仍然是一个悬而未决的问题。在这里,我们检查了28名患有PTSD的战斗退伍军人和27名控制退伍军人的两种流行的反应抑制任务的性能。我们使用了一种混合变体,混合了75%的Go试验,12.5%NoGo试验,和12.5%停止试验。来自非高斯种族模型的参数(Matzke等人,,2017)提供了停止速度(μStop)和停止变异性(τStop)的估计。患有PTSD的参与者在NoGo试验中的错误率更高,复制以前的结果。在PTSD患者中,Stop试验中“触发失败”(启动抑制控制失败)的估计概率也更高,这表明持续关注是这两项任务中的共同缺陷。PTSD参与者的停止变异性也增加,这支持了维持任务目标的困难。相反,停止速度在患者和对照组之间没有差异,表明核心抑制过程是完整的。这些结果证明了PTSD中运动反应抑制的速度和可靠性之间的分离,并提示自顶向下的抑制控制在PTSD参与者中的部署不那么一致。
    Inhibitory control over thoughts, emotions, and actions is challenging for people with Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Whether specific aspects of inhibitory control are differentially affected in PTSD remains an open question. Here we examined performance on two popular response inhibition tasks in 28 combat Veterans with PTSD and 27 control Veterans. We used a Hybrid variant that intermixed 75% Go trials, 12.5% NoGo trials, and 12.5% Stop trials. Parameters from an ex-Gaussian race model (Matzke et al., 2017) provided estimates of stopping speed (μ Stop) and stopping variability (τ Stop). Participants with PTSD had higher error rates on NoGo trials, replicating previous results. The estimated probability of \"trigger failures\" (failures to launch inhibitory control) on Stop trials was also higher in PTSD patients, suggesting that sustained attention was a common deficit in the two tasks. Stopping variability was also increased in participants with PTSD, which supports a difficulty with maintaining task goals. Conversely, stopping speed did not differ between patients and controls, suggesting that core inhibitory processes were intact. These results demonstrate a dissociation between the speed and reliability of motor response inhibition in PTSD, and suggest that top-down inhibitory control was deployed less consistently in participants with PTSD.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号