sense of agency (SoA)

代理意识 (SoA)
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    控制自己和环境的经验的改变与理解抑郁症的精神病理学高度相关。本研究调查了作用事件序列的时间结合之间的关系,代理意识,重度抑郁症患者的自我效能感和症状严重程度。
    我们采用代理感量表(SoAS)和一般自我效能感量表(GSE)评估了42名被诊断患有重度抑郁症(MDD)[20名女性,19名男性;平均年龄37.8岁(±13.3)]和40名未经精神病诊断(CG)的对照者[22名女性,20为男性;平均年龄38.0岁(±13.3)]。使用BDI-II测量抑郁症状严重程度。我们还执行了时间结合范式,作为与代理意识的潜在关联。参与者参加了一个时间估计任务,判断三个间隔(250ms,450ms,650ms),同时观察或引起刺激呈现。引起刺激呈现的有意动作后对间隔的低估(与仅观察刺激呈现相比)被解释为时间结合。
    SoAS评分与抑郁症状呈负相关(CG:p=.032,R2=.113;MDD:p<.001,R2=.260),并且与GSE评分呈正相关(CG:p<.001,R2=.379;MDD:p<.001,R2=.254)。我们发现健康参与者和重度抑郁症组之间的时间结合存在明显差异,时间结合与SoAS或GSE评分之间没有显着相关性。数据表明,在时间估计方面存在小组差异,特别是与涉及有意行动和日益复杂的多感官刺激的时间间隔有关。
    我们研究了主观控制参数,即代理感和自我效能感。这里,我们能够揭示它们与重度抑郁障碍患者的抑郁症状的反比关系,突出了随着症状负荷增加而失去控制的深刻体验。在经历控制方面存在缺陷,特别是涉及有意的运动动作(和更复杂的多感官刺激),在重度抑郁症中似乎更明显,不仅涉及负面的自我效能期望,而且还涉及改变的代理和时间结合感。时间结合和SoAS评分不相关,越来越多的证据表明,这两项措施可能没有直接关系。我们建议未来的研究应针对重性抑郁障碍中代理感与自我效能感之间的这种连续关系。
    UNASSIGNED: Alterations in the experience of controlling oneself and one\'s environment are of high relevance to understanding the psychopathology of depression. This study investigated the relationship between Temporal Binding for action-event sequences, sense of agency, self-efficacy and symptom severity in Major Depressive Disorder.
    UNASSIGNED: We employed the Sense of Agency Scale (SoAS) and the General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSE) to assess explicit Sense of Agency and self-efficacy in a group of 42 persons diagnosed with Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) [20 identifying as female, 19 as male; mean age 37.8 years (± 13.3)] and 40 control persons without a psychiatric diagnosis (CG) [22 identifying as female, 20 as male; mean age 38.0 years ( ± 13.3)]. Depressive symptom severity was measured using the BDI-II. We additionally performed a temporal binding paradigm as a potential correlate to Sense of Agency. Participants partook in a time estimation task judging three intervals (250ms, 450ms, 650ms) while either observing or causing stimulus presentations. The underestimation of intervals following intentional actions causing stimulus presentations (compared to merely observing the stimulus presentation) is interpreted as temporal binding.
    UNASSIGNED: SoAS scores demonstrated an inverse correlation with depressive symptoms (CG: p=.032, R2=.113; MDD: p<.001, R2=.260) and a positive correlation with GSE scores (CG: p<.001, R2=.379; MDD: p<.001, R2=.254). We found distinct differences in temporal binding between healthy participants and the Major Depressive Disorder group without significant correlation between temporal binding and the SoAS or GSE scores. The data suggest group differences in time estimation particular pertaining to time intervals involving intentional action and increasingly complex multisensory stimuli.
    UNASSIGNED: We investigated parameters of subjective control, namely Sense of Agency and Self Efficacy. Here, we were able to reveal their inverse relationship with depressive symptoms in patients with major depressive disorder, highlighting a profound experience of loss of control with increasing symptom load. Deficits in experiencing control, particularly involving intentional motor actions (and more complex multisensory stimuli), appear to be more pronounced in Major Depressive Disorder, involving not only negative self-efficacy expectations but also an altered Sense of Agency and temporal binding. Temporal binding and SoAS scores did not correlate, adding to the growing evidence that the two measures may not be directly related. We propose that future research be directed at this contiguous relationship between Sense of Agency and Self Efficacy in Major Depressive Disorder.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Editorial
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    以往关于强制的研究忽视了这样一个事实,即在权威压力下的代理人也可能遭受强制权力,这会引发受害者焦虑般的情绪消极情绪。此外,已经发现高水平的神经质和/或焦虑与各种形式的社会压力的依从性有关。在这项研究中,我们研究抗焦虑GABAA(γ-氨基丁酸)调节剂的作用,劳拉西泮,关于对强制力的行为和神经反应。这里,我们将虚拟服从与劳拉西泮管理(相对于安慰剂)一起应用于权威范式,在功能磁共振成像扫描期间。我们的结果表明,劳拉西m给药对启动伤害与帮助行为时的反应时间(RT)产生不同的影响,与帮助RT相比,伤害RT更长,尽管在感知到的胁迫方面有可比的主观评级。胁迫伤害显著增加杏仁核的活性,海马体,眶额叶皮质,和背外侧前额叶皮层(dlPFC)。劳拉西泮给药减少杏仁核和海马活动,但增加了dlPFC和右颞顶交界处的激活。海马中较低的活动预示着对感知到的胁迫的较高评级。此外,劳拉西m在强制伤害期间显着降低了海马与dlPFC的功能连通性。总之,我们提供证据-通过纳入多峰指数,包括神经成像,神经药理学干预措施,和行为评估-假设GABAA激动剂,劳拉西泮,可能有助于作为一种可能的干预措施,为应对胁迫的策略服务。
    Previous research on coercion has neglected the fact that agents under authoritative pressure may also suffer from coercive power, which can trigger anxiety-like emotional negativity on its victims. Furthermore, high levels of neuroticism and/or anxiety have been found to be associated with the compliance of various forms of social pressure. In this study, we investigate the effects of the anxiolytic GABA A (gamma-Aminobutyric acid) modulator, lorazepam, on behavioral and neural responses to coercive power. Here, we applied a virtual obedience to authority paradigm alongside lorazepam administration (versus placebo), and during functional magnetic resonance imaging scanning. Our results show that lorazepam administration exerted differential effects on the reaction times (RTs) when initiating harming versus helping behaviors, with longer harming RTs compared to helping RTs, despite comparable subjective ratings regarding perceived coercion. Coercive harming significantly increased activity in the amygdala, hippocampus, orbitofrontal cortex, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC). Lorazepam administration decreased amygdala and hippocampus activity, but increased dlPFC and right temporoparietal junction activations. The lower activity in the hippocampus predicted higher ratings for perceived coercion. Furthermore, lorazepam significantly decreased the functional connectivity of the hippocampus with the dlPFC during coercive harming. In conclusion, we provide evidence -by incorporating multimodal indices, including neuroimaging, neuropharmacological interventions, and behavioral assessments- to posit that the GABA A agonist, lorazepam, might aid as a possible intervention in service of coping strategies against coercion.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人机集成是一个新兴领域,随着用户和计算机协同工作并共享代理来执行任务,人类与技术之间的界限变得模糊。代理感(SoA)是一种由自愿运动动作和感官证据相结合而产生的体验,即相应的身体运动是否以某种方式影响了外部事件的过程。SoA不仅是我们日常生活经验的关键部分,也是我们与技术互动的关键部分,因为它给我们的感觉是“我做到了”,而不是“系统做到了”,“因此支持一种被控制的感觉。这种感觉在人机集成中变得至关重要,其中新兴技术直接影响人们的身体,他们的行动,以及由此产生的结果。在这次审查中,我们根据我们目前在文献中对代理的了解来分析和分类当前的集成技术,并提出了身体增强之间的区别,行动增强,和结果增强。对于每个类别,我们描述了机构的考虑因素和差异标记,这些因素说明了援助水平(低,高),代理代表团(人,技术),和集成类型(融合,共生)。最后,我们反思了人类与计算机集成的机遇和挑战,并最终确定了人机集成的扩展定义,包括我们认为特别相关的机构方面。本次审查的目的是为研究人员和从业人员提供指导,以将他们的工作置于集成研究议程中,并考虑任何技术对SoA的影响,以及设计未来技术时的整体用户体验。
    Human-computer integration is an emerging area in which the boundary between humans and technology is blurred as users and computers work collaboratively and share agency to execute tasks. The sense of agency (SoA) is an experience that arises by a combination of a voluntary motor action and sensory evidence whether the corresponding body movements have somehow influenced the course of external events. The SoA is not only a key part of our experiences in daily life but also in our interaction with technology as it gives us the feeling of \"I did that\" as opposed to \"the system did that,\" thus supporting a feeling of being in control. This feeling becomes critical with human-computer integration, wherein emerging technology directly influences people\'s body, their actions, and the resulting outcomes. In this review, we analyse and classify current integration technologies based on what we currently know about agency in the literature, and propose a distinction between body augmentation, action augmentation, and outcome augmentation. For each category, we describe agency considerations and markers of differentiation that illustrate a relationship between assistance level (low, high), agency delegation (human, technology), and integration type (fusion, symbiosis). We conclude with a reflection on the opportunities and challenges of integrating humans with computers, and finalise with an expanded definition of human-computer integration including agency aspects which we consider to be particularly relevant. The aim this review is to provide researchers and practitioners with guidelines to situate their work within the integration research agenda and consider the implications of any technologies on SoA, and thus overall user experience when designing future technology.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自产生的听觉输入被感知的声音比外部产生的相同声音少。这种现象的存在,称为感官衰减(SA),已经研究了几十年,通常用基于电机的正向模型来解释。SA研究的最新进展,然而,挑战这些模式。我们回顾了有关感官衰减的意义及其在人类行为和功能中的作用的理论意义的知识现状。专注于听觉领域的行为和电生理结果,我们概述了现有SA范式的特点和局限性,并强调了将SA与其他预测机制分离的问题.最后,我们探索不同的假设,试图解释异质的实证发现,以及预测编码框架在这一研究领域的影响。
    Self-generated auditory input is perceived less loudly than the same sounds generated externally. The existence of this phenomenon, called Sensory Attenuation (SA), has been studied for decades and is often explained by motor-based forward models. Recent developments in the research of SA, however, challenge these models. We review the current state of knowledge regarding theoretical implications about the significance of Sensory Attenuation and its role in human behavior and functioning. Focusing on behavioral and electrophysiological results in the auditory domain, we provide an overview of the characteristics and limitations of existing SA paradigms and highlight the problem of isolating SA from other predictive mechanisms. Finally, we explore different hypotheses attempting to explain heterogeneous empirical findings, and the impact of the Predictive Coding Framework in this research area.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在本研究中,我们测试了右中额叶/辅助运动区(SMA)和下顶叶皮层(IPC)之间耦合的变化是否反映了代理感(SoA)。12名健康的成人志愿者参与了这项研究。他们执行了线条绘制任务的变体(尼尔森,1963年;Fourneret和Jeannerod,1998),其中,他们用右手在数字平板电脑上移动光标,而没有看到手。计算机显示器上显示的视觉反馈与实际运动相对应或偏离。这使参与者不确定运动的代理人,并且当运动是计算机生成的时,他们在大约50%的试验中报告了SoA。我们测试了IPC-preSMA耦合是否与SoA相关,使用动态因果模型(DCM)进行诱导反应(Chen等人。,2008;Herz等人。,2012).为任务的早期和后期阶段建造了9种不同的DCM,分别。所有模型都包括两个区域:上内侧回(preSMA)区域和右上侧上回(IPC)区域。贝叶斯模型选择(Stephan等人,,2009)在后期任务阶段,偏爱具有IPC输入和SMA前向连接调制的模型,在早期任务阶段,支持具有预SMA输入和后向连接调制的模型。分析表明,与任务后期没有SoA相比,在存在SoA的情况下,在50-60Hz范围内的IPC源活动调制了在40-70Hz范围内的preSMA源活动,但是早期任务阶段的测试没有发现SoA的存在和不存在之间的任何差异。我们表明,在后期任务阶段,SoA与从IPC到preSMA的频率耦合之间的特定方向相关。这表明SoA是一种回顾性的看法,这在很大程度上取决于对所执行动作结果的解释。
    In the present study we tested whether sense of agency (SoA) is reflected by changes in coupling between right medio-frontal/supplementary motor area (SMA) and inferior parietal cortex (IPC). Twelve healthy adult volunteers participated in the study. They performed a variation of a line-drawing task (Nielsen, 1963; Fourneret and Jeannerod, 1998), in which they moved a cursor on a digital tablet with their right hand without seeing the hand. Visual feedback displayed on a computer monitor was either in correspondence with or deviated from the actual movement. This made participants uncertain as to the agent of the movement and they reported SoA in approximately 50% of trials when the movement was computer-generated. We tested whether IPC-preSMA coupling was associated with SoA, using dynamic causal modeling (DCM) for induced responses (Chen et al., 2008; Herz et al., 2012). Nine different DCMs were constructed for the early and late phases of the task, respectively. All models included two regions: a superior medial gyrus (preSMA) region and a right supramarginal gyrus (IPC) region. Bayesian models selection (Stephan et al., 2009) favored a model with input to IPC and modulation of the forward connection to SMA in the late task phase, and a model with input to preSMA and modulation of the backward connection was favored for the early task phase. The analysis shows that IPC source activity in the 50-60 Hz range modulated preSMA source activity in the 40-70 Hz range in the presence of SoA compared with no SoA in the late task phase, but the test of the early task phase did not reveal any differences between presence and absence of SoA. We show that SoA is associated with a directionally specific between frequencies coupling from IPC to preSMA in the higher gamma (ɣ) band in the late task phase. This suggests that SoA is a retrospective perception, which is highly dependent on interpretation of the outcome of the performed action.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    大量的功能磁共振成像和病变文献提供了证据,表明下壁皮层(IPC)对于感觉运动整合和代理感(SoA)很重要。我们使用重复经颅磁刺激(rTMS)来探索IPC在经过验证的SoA检测任务中的作用。12健康,包括惯用右手的成年人。探讨了rTMS在自我控制运动过程中对受试者SoA的影响。实验包括1/3个自我控制的运动和(2)/3个计算机操作的运动,这给受试者是否是观察到的运动的代理人带来了不确定性。受试者完成了三场会议,其中受试者在正确的IPC(活动状况)上在线接受rTMS,在顶点(CZ)(假条件)或没有TMS,但声音匹配的控制。我们发现,右IPC上的rTMS显着改变了非扰动运动的SoA。与对照部位(P=0.002)和无刺激对照(P=0.042)相比,在IPC刺激之后,受试者更有可能经历自我控制的运动,因为受到外部干扰。数据支持在感觉运动比较过程中IPC激活的重要性,以便正确确定运动的代理。
    A large body of fMRI and lesion-literature has provided evidence that the Inferior Parietal Cortex (IPC) is important for sensorimotor integration and sense of agency (SoA). We used repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to explore the role of the IPC during a validated SoA detection task. 12 healthy, right-handed adults were included. The effects of rTMS on subjects\' SoA during self-controlled movements were explored. The experiment consisted of 1/3 self-controlled movements and (2)/3 computer manipulated movements that introduced uncertainty as to whether the subjects were agents of an observed movement. Subjects completed three sessions, in which subjects received online rTMS over the right IPC (active condition), over the vertex (CZ) (sham condition) or no TMS but a sound-matched control. We found that rTMS over right IPC significantly altered SoA of the non-perturbed movements. Following IPC stimulation subjects were more likely to experience self-controlled movements as being externally perturbed compared to the control site (P = 0.002) and the stimulation-free control (P = 0.042). The data support the importance of IPC activation during sensorimotor comparison in order to correctly determine the agent of movements.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Existing accounts of the sense of agency tend to focus on the proximal causal history of the feeling. That is, they explain the sense of agency by describing the cognitive mechanism that causes the sense of agency to be elicited. However, it is possible to elicit an unconscious representation of one\'s own agency that plays a different role in a cognitive system. I use the \"occasionality problem\" to suggest that taking this distinction seriously has potential theoretical pay-offs for this reason. We are faced, then, with a need to distinguish instances of the representation of one\'s own agency in which the subject is aware of their sense of own agency from instances in which they are not. This corresponds to a specific instance of what Dennett calls the \"Hard Question\": once the representation is elicited, then what happens? In other words, how is a representation of one\'s own agency used in a cognitive system when the subject is aware of it? How is this different from when the representation of own agency remains unconscious? This phrasing suggests a Functionalist answer to the Hard Question. I consider two single function hypotheses. First, perhaps the representation of own agency enters into the mechanisms of attention. This seems unlikely as, in general, attention is insufficient for awareness. Second, perhaps, a subject is aware of their sense of agency when it is available for verbal report. However, this seems inconsistent with evidence of a sense of agency in the great apes. Although these two single function views seem like dead ends, multifunction hypotheses such as the global workspace theory remain live options which we should consider. I close by considering a non-functionalist answer to the Hard Question: perhaps it is not a difference in the use to which the representation is put, but a difference in the nature of the representation itself. When it comes to the sense of agency, the Hard Question remains, but there are alternatives open to us.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

公众号