online testing

在线测试
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们的目标是:(1)描述同性恋,双性恋和其他男男性行为者(GBMSM)和变性者(TG)人群在TESTATE项目中使用基于互联网的自我抽样服务或参加基于社区的性传播感染/艾滋病毒自愿咨询和检测(CBVCT)服务作为替代策略在西班牙国家卫生系统内进行正式艾滋病毒检测,(2)确定与2018年11月至2021年12月重复使用相同筛查策略相关的因素。人口统计,健康,并分析了用户使用互补策略的行为特征。我们进行了一项横断面研究,通过描述性分析,艾滋病毒级联,和多变量逻辑模型,以确定与参与者重复使用相同筛查策略相关的因素。我们包括9939个用户,其中94.1%为GBMSM(n=9348),5.9%为TG(n=580),移民的比例很高。反应结果为3.4%(n=340),GBMSM为3.0%(n=277/9348),TG为10.7%(n=63/591)。73.8%(n=251)被确认为HIV阳性,76.7%(n=194)与卫生服务有关。用户重复在线筛查策略的次数超过CBVCT(44.3%vs.31.8%),但TG人口更多地使用面对面的社区服务(8.4%与0.6%)。影响在线自我抽样策略重复的因素包括年龄较大,非移民身份,以及最近的艾滋病毒检测。在CBVCT战略中,因素包括年龄较大,TG身份,非移民身份,在最后一次性接触期间使用避孕套,以及最近的艾滋病毒检测。总之,在向GBMSM和TG提供HIV检测方面,CBVCT和在线要求的家庭自我取样都是卫生系统的重要替代方案.
    Our aims were: (1) to characterize gay, bisexual and other men who have sex with men (GBMSM) and transgender (TG) populations using internet-based self-sampling services in the TESTATE project or attending community-based STI/HIV voluntary counselling and testing (CBVCT) services as alternative strategies to formal HIV testing within the Spanish national health system, and (2) to identify factors associated with repeat use of the same screening strategy from November 2018 to December 2021. Demographic, health, and behavioral characteristics of users using complementary strategies were analyzed. We developed a cross-sectional study, with descriptive analysis, HIV cascade, and a multivariate logistic model to identify factors associated with participants\' repeated use of the same screening strategy. We included 9939 users, of whom 94.1% were GBMSM (n = 9348) and 5.9% TG (n = 580), with a high representation of migrants. Reactive results were 3.4% (n = 340), with 3.0% in GBMSM (n = 277/9348) and 10.7% in TG (n = 63/591). 73.8% (n = 251) were confirmed HIV positive and 76.7% (n = 194) were linked to health services. Users repeated the online screening strategy more than CBVCT (44.3% vs. 31.8%), but TG population used face-to-face community services more (8.4% vs. 0.6%). Factors influencing the repetition of the online self-sampling strategy included older age, non-migrant status, and recent HIV testing. In the CBVCT strategy, factors included older age, TG identity, non-migrant status, condom use during the last sexual encounter, and recent HIV testing. In conclusion, both CBVCT and online-requested self-sampling at home are important alternatives to the health system for the provision of HIV testing to GBMSM and TG.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:阿尔茨海默病(AD)研究的一个主要限制是缺乏以有力的尺度测量认知表现的能力,远程,而且经常。目前,目前尚无针对AD血浆生物标志物进行验证的在线数字平台.
    方法:我们使用了一个新的基于网络的平台,该平台评估了AD患者(N=46)和老年对照组(N=53)的不同认知功能,这些患者也进行了血浆生物标志物的评估(淀粉样蛋白β42/40比率,磷酸化tau([p-tau]181,胶质纤维酸性蛋白,神经丝轻链)。他们的认知表现被比作一秒,更大的老年对照组(N=352)。
    结果:AD患者在所有数字认知测试中都明显受损,与血浆生物标志物水平相关的性能,特别是p-tau181.p-tau181和单个性能最佳的数字测试的组合在组分类中实现了高精度。
    结论:这些发现表明,现在可以在AD患者中进行在线检测,以有效地测量认知功能,并与疾病的血液生物标志物相关。
    这是第一项将在线数字检测与血浆生物标志物进行比较的研究。对阿尔茨海默病患者和两个独立的老年对照组进行评估。与血浆生物标志物相关的认知表现,特别是磷酸化tau(p-tau)181。胶质纤维酸性蛋白和神经丝轻链,淀粉样蛋白β42/40的比例更低,也与绩效有关。在组分类中,最佳认知指标表现为p-tau181。
    BACKGROUND: A major limitation in Alzheimer\'s disease (AD) research is the lack of the ability to measure cognitive performance at scale-robustly, remotely, and frequently. Currently, there are no established online digital platforms validated against plasma biomarkers of AD.
    METHODS: We used a novel web-based platform that assessed different cognitive functions in AD patients (N = 46) and elderly controls (N = 53) who were also evaluated for plasma biomarkers (amyloid beta 42/40 ratio, phosphorylated tau ([p-tau]181, glial fibrillary acidic protein, neurofilament light chain). Their cognitive performance was compared to a second, larger group of elderly controls (N = 352).
    RESULTS: Patients with AD were significantly impaired across all digital cognitive tests, with performance correlating with plasma biomarker levels, particularly p-tau181. The combination of p-tau181 and the single best-performing digital test achieved high accuracy in group classification.
    CONCLUSIONS: These findings show how online testing can now be deployed in patients with AD to measure cognitive function effectively and related to blood biomarkers of the disease.
    UNASSIGNED: This is the first study comparing online digital testing to plasma biomarkers.Alzheimer\'s disease patients and two independent cohorts of elderly controls were assessed.Cognitive performance correlated with plasma biomarkers, particularly phosphorylated tau (p-tau)181.Glial fibrillary acidic protein and neurofilament light chain, and less so the amyloid beta 42/40 ratio, were also associated with performance.The best cognitive metric performed at par to p-tau181 in group classification.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人类的社会生活需要了解一个人的社会伙伴的精神状态。两个看着相同物体的人经常会有不同的体验,作为闪烁的光或行星,一个6或9,一个随机的猫或克莱奥,他们的宠物。的确,交流的主要目的是分享物体或事件的独特体验。这里,我们测试幼儿(14-15个月)是否对另一个代理人的独特的图片体验敏感,当确定代理人在最低社会背景下的行动目标时。我们做了九个实验。在这些实验中的七个(n=206),幼儿观看了录像或现场活动,他们的观点与他们自己的不同,达到(i)直立或倒置的人脸图片,或(ii)描绘不同方向的兔子或鸭子的图片。然后演员或幼儿移动到一个新的位置,使他们的观点保持一致,演员交替地到达两张照片中的每一张。通过比较幼儿对后者的影响,我们测试了他们的目标归因是否符合演员对图片对象的经验,根据他们自己对照片对象的经验,或者没有一致性。在任何实验中,幼儿都没有根据演员对照片的体验来编码演员的目标。相比之下,在一个类似的实验中,操纵了图片的可见性,而不是它引发的体验,蹒跚学步的孩子(n=32)正确地期望演员的动作取决于他可见和遮挡的东西,而不是自己。在任务的口头版本中,在这两种情况下,年龄较大的孩子(n=35)都正确地推断了演员的目标。这些发现为两种精神状态推理之间的分离提供了进一步的证据:当幼儿在最小的社会条件下观察演员的目标导向动作时,他们考虑到演员对对象的视觉访问,而不是演员对对象的独特体验。
    Human social life requires an understanding of the mental states of one\'s social partners. Two people who look at the same objects often experience them differently, as a twinkling light or a planet, a 6 or a 9, and a random cat or Cleo, their pet. Indeed, a primary purpose of communication is to share distinctive experiences of objects or events. Here, we test whether toddlers (14-15 months) are sensitive to another agent\'s distinctive experiences of pictures when determining the goal underlying the agent\'s actions in a minimally social context. We conducted nine experiments. Across seven of these experiments (n = 206), toddlers viewed either videotaped or live events in which an actor, whose perspective differed from their own, reached (i) for pictures of human faces that were upright or inverted or (ii) for pictures that depicted a rabbit or a duck at different orientations. Then either the actor or the toddler moved to a new location that aligned their perspectives, and the actor alternately reached to each of the two pictures. By comparing toddlers\' looking to the latter reaches, we tested whether their goal attributions accorded with the actor\'s experience of the pictured objects, with their own experience of the pictured objects, or with no consistency. In no experiment did toddlers encode the actor\'s goal in accord with his experiences of the pictures. In contrast, in a similar experiment that manipulated the visibility of a picture rather than the experience that it elicited, toddlers (n = 32) correctly expected the actor\'s action to depend on what was visible and occluded to him, rather than to themselves. In a verbal version of the tasks, older children (n = 35) correctly inferred the actor\'s goal in both cases. These findings provide further evidence for a dissociation between two kinds of mental state reasoning: When toddlers view an actor\'s object-directed action under minimally social conditions, they take account of the actor\'s visual access to the object but not the actor\'s distinctive experience of the object.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Observational Study
    背景:在安大略省,加拿大我们开发并实施了一种用于HIV自检分布的在线筛查算法,被称为GetaKit。在COVID大流行期间,我们调整了GetaKit算法以根据人群和感染数据筛选COVID,并向符合条件的参与者分发了COVIDrt-LAMP自检(使用LuciraCheck-It®).
    方法:GetaKit/COVID是一项前瞻性观察性研究,发生在2021年9月至2022年4月的7个月内。所有潜在参与者都完成了在线注册和风险评估,包括人口统计信息,COVID症状和危险因素,和疫苗接种状况。对三个结果进行了双变量比较:结果报告状态,疫苗接种状况,和COVID诊断状态。使用卡方分析分类协变量,使用独立样本T检验和Mann-WhitneyU检验分析连续协变量。双变量逻辑回归模型用于检查协变量和结果之间的关联。
    结果:在研究期间,我们向4160名符合条件的参与者分发了6469份COVID自检;46%被确定为黑人,土著或有色人种(BIPOC)。近70%的参与者报告了他们的COVID自检结果;其中304例为阳性。总的来说,91%的人还报告接种了COVID疫苗。统计分析发现与五个或更少的人生活在一起,之前测试过COVID,和完全接种疫苗是结果报告中的积极因素.对于COVID疫苗接种,来自大城市中心的人,他们认定他们的种族是白人,并且报告以前进行COVID检测的人更有可能完全接种疫苗。最后,被确定为COVID检测呈阳性的人的联系人,并且发现存在COVID相关症状是诊断的积极因素。
    结论:虽然大多数使用这项服务的参与者都接种了COVID疫苗,大多数诊断是在有症状的参与者中发现的,或者暴露于,COVID,我们的项目能够根据报告的风险和结果将参与者与推荐的随访适当联系起来.这些发现突出了在线筛查算法提供健康服务的实用性,特别是对于那些在获得医疗保健方面存在历史障碍的人,例如BIPOC或低收入群体。
    BACKGROUND: In Ontario, Canada we developed and implemented an online screening algorithm for the distribution of HIV self-tests, known as GetaKit. During the COVID pandemic, we adapted the GetaKit algorithm to screen for COVID based on population and infection data and distributed COVID rt-LAMP self-tests (using the Lucira Check-It®) to eligible participants.
    METHODS: GetaKit/COVID was a prospective observational study that occurred over a 7-month period from September 2021 to April 2022. All potential participants completed an online registration and risk assessment, including demographic information, COVID symptoms and risk factors, and vaccination status. Bivariate comparisons were performed for three outcomes: results reporting status, vaccination status, and COVID diagnosis status. Data were analysed using Chi-Square for categorial covariates and Independent Samples T-Test and Mann-Whitney U test for continuous covariates. Bivariate logistic regression models were applied to examine associations between the covariates and outcomes.
    RESULTS: During the study period, we distributed 6469 COVID self-tests to 4160 eligible participants; 46% identified as Black, Indigenous or a Person of Colour (BIPOC). Nearly 70% of participants reported their COVID self-test results; 304 of which were positive. Overall, 91% also reported being vaccinated against COVID. Statistical analysis found living with five or fewer people, having tested for COVID previously, and being fully vaccinated were positive factors in results reporting. For COVID vaccination, people from large urban centers, who identified their ethnicity as white, and who reported previous COVID testing were more likely to be fully vaccinated. Finally, being identified as a contact of someone who had tested positive for COVID and the presence of COVID-related symptoms were found to be positive factors in diagnosis.
    CONCLUSIONS: While most participants who accessed this service were vaccinated against COVID and the majority of diagnoses were identified in participants who had symptoms of, or an exposure to, COVID, our program was able to appropriately link participants to recommended follow-up based on reported risks and results. These findings highlight the utility of online screening algorithms to provide health services, particularly for persons with historical barriers to healthcare access, such as BIPOC or lower-income groups.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在线方法允许测试更大的,更多样化的人口,与实验室测试相比,工作量要少得多。然而,许多心理物理学测量,包括视觉拥挤,需要准确的眼睛固定,这通常是通过只测试有经验的观察者来实现的,他们已经学会了可靠地固定,或通过使用凝视跟踪器将测试限制在固定准确的时刻。唉,这两种方法在网上都是不切实际的,因为在线观察者往往缺乏经验,和在线凝视跟踪,使用内置的网络摄像头,具有低精度(±4度)。EasyEyes开源软件可靠地在线测量外围阈值,以新颖的方式实现精确固定,没有凝视跟踪。它告诉观察者使用光标来跟踪移动的十字准线。在成功跟踪期间的随机时间,在外围提出了一个简短的目标。观察者通过识别目标来响应。为了评估EasyEyes固定的准确性和阈值,我们以平衡的顺序以三种方式测试了12个幼稚的观察者:第一,在实验室里,使用视而不见的刺激呈现;第二,在实验室里,使用EasyEyes,同时使用EyeLink1000独立监测凝视;第三,在家上网,使用EasyEyes。我们发现拥挤阈值是一致的,个体差异是保守的。在目标呈现期间的小的均方根(RMS)固定误差(0.6deg)消除了对注视跟踪的需要。因此,这种方法可以在线进行固定相关的测量,便于对更大和更多样化的人群进行测试。
    Online methods allow testing of larger, more diverse populations, with much less effort than in-lab testing. However, many psychophysical measurements, including visual crowding, require accurate eye fixation, which is classically achieved by testing only experienced observers who have learned to fixate reliably, or by using a gaze tracker to restrict testing to moments when fixation is accurate. Alas, both approaches are impractical online as online observers tend to be inexperienced, and online gaze tracking, using the built-in webcam, has a low precision (±4 deg). EasyEyes open-source software reliably measures peripheral thresholds online with accurate fixation achieved in a novel way, without gaze tracking. It tells observers to use the cursor to track a moving crosshair. At a random time during successful tracking, a brief target is presented in the periphery. The observer responds by identifying the target. To evaluate EasyEyes fixation accuracy and thresholds, we tested 12 naive observers in three ways in a counterbalanced order: first, in the laboratory, using gaze-contingent stimulus presentation; second, in the laboratory, using EasyEyes while independently monitoring gaze using EyeLink 1000; third, online at home, using EasyEyes. We find that crowding thresholds are consistent and individual differences are conserved. The small root mean square (RMS) fixation error (0.6 deg) during target presentation eliminates the need for gaze tracking. Thus, this method enables fixation-dependent measurements online, for easy testing of larger and more diverse populations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对他人思想的知识是否从具体经验发展到抽象概念?认知科学家假设婴儿的第一人称体验,按照自己的目标行事,引导他们理解他人的行动和目标。的确,经典的发育研究表明,在婴儿到达物体之前,他们不认为其他人达到了目标。在五个实验中(N=117),我们检验了另一个假设:年幼的婴儿认为伸手是出于目的,但对伸手行动旨在实现的特定目标持开放态度。我们首先展示了3个月大的婴儿,无法触及物体的人,缺乏对观察到的到达行为将指向物体而不是地方的期望。同龄的婴儿学得很快,然而,特定代理的到达动作被定向到对象或位置,在看到代理人伸手去拿同一个物体后,不管它在哪里,或者去同一个地方,不管那里有什么。在进一步的实验中,3个月大的婴儿在观察到演员从事被动运动时没有表现出这种推论。因此,在婴儿学会自己接触和操纵物体之前,他们推断达成行动是以目标为导向的,他们愿意学习行动的目标是一个对象或一个地方。亮点:在目前的实验中,3个月大的预言性婴儿学会了将对象目标或目标归因于其他人的达成行为。预言性婴儿将代理人的行动视为目标导向的,但不要期望这些行为针对特定对象,而不是去特定的地方。前瞻性的婴儿对达成行动旨在实现的特定目标状态持开放态度。
    Does knowledge of other people\'s minds grow from concrete experience to abstract concepts? Cognitive scientists have hypothesized that infants\' first-person experience, acting on their own goals, leads them to understand others\' actions and goals. Indeed, classic developmental research suggests that before infants reach for objects, they do not see others\' reaches as goal-directed. In five experiments (N = 117), we test an alternative hypothesis: Young infants view reaching as undertaken for a purpose but are open-minded about the specific goals that reaching actions are aimed to achieve. We first show that 3-month-old infants, who cannot reach for objects, lack the expectation that observed acts of reaching will be directed to objects rather than to places. Infants at the same age learned rapidly, however, that a specific agent\'s reaching action was directed either to an object or to a place, after seeing the agent reach for the same object regardless of where it was, or to the same place regardless of what was there. In a further experiment, 3-month-old infants did not demonstrate such inferences when they observed an actor engaging in passive movements. Thus, before infants have learned to reach and manipulate objects themselves, they infer that reaching actions are goal-directed, and they are open to learning that the goal of an action is either an object or a place. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: In the present experiments, 3-month-old prereaching infants learned to attribute either object goals or place goals to other people\'s reaching actions. Prereaching infants view agents\' actions as goal-directed, but do not expect these acts to be directed to specific objects, rather than to specific places. Prereaching infants are open-minded about the specific goal states that reaching actions aim to achieve.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    越来越多的文献证明了工作记忆(WM)和噪声语音(SiN)感知之间的联系。然而,这种相关性的性质以及WM的哪些组成部分可能是其基础,正在辩论。我们研究了SiN接收如何与听觉感觉记忆(aSM)联系在一起,听觉感觉记忆是支持短期维持时间展开声音的低级过程。在线招募了大量的老年(N=199,60-79yo)和年轻(N=149,20-35yo)参与者,并执行了基于协调响应措施的语音语音任务,该任务利用听众在背景噪声中跟踪语音目标的能力。我们使用两个任务来研究隐式和显式aSM。两者都基于在处理语音时标上重叠的音调模式(音调的呈现速率为20Hz;模式为2Hz)。我们假设,由于SiN接收和aSM的年龄相关减少,SiN和aSM之间的联系在老年听众中可能特别明显。我们证实了较老队列中SiN的接收受损,并证明了这些听众的aSM表现降低。然而,SiN和aSM不共享变异性。在两个年龄组中,SiN性能通过双耳加工测试和年龄预测。结果表明,先前观察到的WM和SiN之间的联系可能与所使用任务的执行组件和其他认知需求有关。这一发现有助于限制对解释SiN性能个体差异的感知和认知因素的搜索。
    A growing literature is demonstrating a link between working memory (WM) and speech-in-noise (SiN) perception. However, the nature of this correlation and which components of WM might underlie it, are being debated. We investigated how SiN reception links with auditory sensory memory (aSM) - the low-level processes that support the short-term maintenance of temporally unfolding sounds. A large sample of old (N = 199, 60-79 yo) and young (N = 149, 20-35 yo) participants was recruited online and performed a coordinate response measure-based speech-in-babble task that taps listeners\' ability to track a speech target in background noise. We used two tasks to investigate implicit and explicit aSM. Both were based on tone patterns overlapping in processing time scales with speech (presentation rate of tones 20 Hz; of patterns 2 Hz). We hypothesised that a link between SiN and aSM may be particularly apparent in older listeners due to age-related reduction in both SiN reception and aSM. We confirmed impaired SiN reception in the older cohort and demonstrated reduced aSM performance in those listeners. However, SiN and aSM did not share variability. Across the two age groups, SiN performance was predicted by a binaural processing test and age. The results suggest that previously observed links between WM and SiN may relate to the executive components and other cognitive demands of the used tasks. This finding helps to constrain the search for the perceptual and cognitive factors that explain individual variability in SiN performance.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本文旨在比较一种新的基于网络摄像头的眼动追踪系统,集成到Labvanced平台进行在线实验,基于实验室的“黄金标准”眼动仪(EyeLink1000-SRResearch)。具体来说,我们在五个不同的任务中同时使用两个眼睛追踪器记录数据,分析其实时性能。这些任务是眼睛追踪器的标准化测试电池的子集,包括一个大网格任务,平滑的追求眼球运动,查看自然图像,和两个头部移动任务(滚动,yaw).结果表明,基于网络摄像头的系统实现了1.4°的整体精度,精度为1.1°(受试者之间的标准偏差(SD)),比EyeLink系统大约0.5°的误差。有趣的是,对于集中呈现的目标,精度(1.3°)和精度(0.9°)都略好,许多心理物理实验中感兴趣的区域。值得注意的是,EyeLink和基于网络摄像头的原始凝视样本的相关性对于大网格任务约为90%,对于自由查看和平滑追踪约为80%。总的来说,这些结果使基于网络摄像头的系统的性能与移动眼睛跟踪设备大致相当(Ehinger等人。PeerJ,7,e7086,2019;Tonsen等人。,2020年),并与现有的网络摄像头眼动追踪解决方案相比表现出实质性的改进(Papoutsaki等人。,2017)。
    This paper aims to compare a new webcam-based eye-tracking system, integrated into the Labvanced platform for online experiments, to a \"gold standard\" lab-based eye tracker (EyeLink 1000 - SR Research). Specifically, we simultaneously recorded data with both eye trackers in five different tasks, analyzing their real-time performance. These tasks were a subset of a standardized test battery for eye trackers, including a Large Grid task, Smooth Pursuit eye movements, viewing natural images, and two Head Movements tasks (roll, yaw). The results show that the webcam-based system achieved an overall accuracy of 1.4°, and a precision of 1.1° (standard deviation (SD) across subjects), an error of about 0.5° larger than the EyeLink system. Interestingly, both accuracy (1.3°) and precision (0.9°) were slightly better for centrally presented targets, the region of interest in many psychophysical experiments. Remarkably, the correlation of raw gaze samples between the EyeLink and webcam-based was at about 90% for the Large Grid task and about 80% for Free View and Smooth Pursuit. Overall, these results put the performance of the webcam-based system roughly on par with mobile eye-tracking devices (Ehinger et al. PeerJ, 7, e7086, 2019; Tonsen et al., 2020) and demonstrate substantial improvement compared to existing webcam eye-tracking solutions (Papoutsaki et al., 2017).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    听觉系统在感觉系统中是独特的,因为它能够锁相并精确地跟踪声音驱动的耳蜗振动相位的非常快速的逐周期波动。然而,对这种时间精细结构(TFS)码的感知作用进行了辩论。这种根本差距归因于我们无法在不改变其他感知相关线索的情况下实验性地操纵TFS线索。这里,我们通过利用200名参与者的个体差异来系统地比较TFS敏感性在一系列语音感知任务中对性能的变化,从而规避了这一限制。结果表明,稳健的TFS灵敏度不会从音调或空间线索中提供额外的掩蔽释放,但似乎赋予了抵抗混响影响的韧性。然而,跨条件,我们还发现,更大的TFS灵敏度与更快的响应时间有关,与减少倾听的努力相一致。这些发现强调了TFS编码对日常听力的感知意义。
    The auditory system is unique among sensory systems in its ability to phase lock to and precisely follow very fast cycle-by-cycle fluctuations in the phase of sound-driven cochlear vibrations. Yet, the perceptual role of this temporal fine structure (TFS) code is debated. This fundamental gap is attributable to our inability to experimentally manipulate TFS cues without altering other perceptually relevant cues. Here, we circumnavigated this limitation by leveraging individual differences across 200 participants to systematically compare variations in TFS sensitivity to performance in a range of speech perception tasks. TFS sensitivity was assessed through detection of interaural time/phase differences, while speech perception was evaluated by word identification under noise interference. Results suggest that greater TFS sensitivity is not associated with greater masking release from fundamental-frequency or spatial cues, but appears to contribute to resilience against the effects of reverberation. We also found that greater TFS sensitivity is associated with faster response times, indicating reduced listening effort. These findings highlight the perceptual significance of TFS coding for everyday hearing.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号