names

Names
  • 文章类型: Historical Article
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:本研究旨在研究可能影响英国神经外科医生发表单作者论文能力的因素。这些包括人口因素(例如性别、肤色),传记因素(例如,他们是否获得博士学位或他们毕业的大学或他们目前所属的大学的排名),和名称特征。
    方法:2023年5月在英国工作的所有神经外科医生的姓名均来自专家信息网站。Scopus用于获取文献计量学。使用公开的在线资源来获取传记和人口统计信息。
    结果:大约三分之一的神经外科医生发表了至少一篇单作者论文。在384名神经外科医生发表的398篇单作者论文中,69是开放通道,44是评论,64是社论,71是文章,219人被归类为“其他”。他们的第一篇单作者论文平均在医学院毕业后15.2年发表,平均在他们第一次发表后9年(任何作者职位)。在13位神经外科医生中,他们的第一作者论文是一篇综述,在14年,这是一篇社论,在24篇文章中,在57年,它被归类为“其他”。他们发表第一篇单作者论文的期刊的影响因子平均为11.1(中位数=2.4)。根据性别或肤色,单作者论文的数量没有差异。然而,全职教授中有更多的单一作者出版物,从顶尖大学获得医学学位的神经外科医生,那些有博士学位的人,以及目前隶属于一所大学的人。更多的高级神经外科医生有更多的单一作者出版物。法医更受欢迎的神经外科医生,其全名的种族是英国/爱尔兰,或者姓氏中有更长的辅音序列,则有更多的单作者论文。
    结论:这是第一项彻底检查一组医学专业人员中的单作者出版物并检查它们是否与某些社会人口统计学和姓名特征相关的研究。
    BACKGROUND: This study aimed to examine factors that may affect UK neurosurgeons\' ability to publish single-author papers. These include demographic factors (e.g. gender, skin colour), biographical factors (e.g. whether they obtained a PhD or the ranking of the university from which they graduated or with which they are currently affiliated), and name characteristics.
    METHODS: Names of all neurosurgeons working in the UK in May 2023 were obtained from the Specialist Info website. Scopus was used to obtain bibliometrics. Publicly available online sources were used to obtain biographical and demographic information.
    RESULTS: Approximately 1 out of 3 neurosurgeons had published at least one single-author paper. Of the 398 single-author papers published by the whole group of 384 neurosurgeons, 69 were open access, 44 were reviews, 64 were editorials, 71 were articles, and 219 were classified as \'other\'. Their first single-author paper was published on average 15.2 years after medical school graduation and on average 9 years after their first publication (any author position). In 13 neurosurgeons their first-single author paper was a review, in 14 it was an editorial, in 24 an article, and in 57 it was classified as \'other\'. The impact factor of the journal in which they published their first single-author paper was on average 11.1 (Median = 2.4). Single-author papers do not differ in number depending on gender or skin colour. However, there were more single-author publications among full professors, neurosurgeons who graduated from a top university for their medical degree, those who had a PhD, and those who are currently affiliated with a university. More senior neurosurgeons had more single-author publications. Neurosurgeons with more popular forenames, whose full name\'s perceived ethnicity was UK/Irish or had longer consonant sequences in their surname had more single-author papers.
    CONCLUSIONS: This is the first study to thoroughly examine single-author publications in a group of medical professionals and examine whether they are associated with certain socio-demographic and name characteristics.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们的名字是我们生命早期的社会标签。这项研究调查了自我实现的预言效应的可能性,其中个人的面部外观随着时间的推移而发展,以类似于与给定名字相关的社会刻板印象。利用面名匹配效应,这展示了一种将成年人的名字与他们的脸相匹配的能力,我们假设个体在成年期会与他们的社会刻板印象(名字)相似,而在童年时期则不会。为了检验这个假设,要求儿童和成人匹配儿童和成人的面孔和名字。结果显示,成人和儿童都正确地将成人面孔与相应的名字相匹配,明显高于机会水平。然而,当谈到孩子们的面孔和名字时,参与者无法做出准确的关联.补充我们的实验室研究,我们采用了一个机器学习框架来处理面部图像数据,结果发现,与不同名字的成年人相比,相同名字的成年人的面部表征更相似.在儿童的面部表情中不存在这种相似性模式,从而加强了自我实现预言假说的理由。此外,面部名称匹配效果是明显的成人,但不是为儿童的脸,人为年龄类似成人,支持社会发展在这一效应中的推测作用。一起,这些发现表明,即使我们的面部外观也会受到社会因素的影响,例如我们的名字,证实了社会期望的强大影响。
    Our given name is a social tag associated with us early in life. This study investigates the possibility of a self-fulfilling prophecy effect wherein individuals\' facial appearance develops over time to resemble the social stereotypes associated with given names. Leveraging the face-name matching effect, which demonstrates an ability to match adults\' names to their faces, we hypothesized that individuals would resemble their social stereotype (name) in adulthood but not in childhood. To test this hypothesis, children and adults were asked to match faces and names of children and adults. Results revealed that both adults and children correctly matched adult faces to their corresponding names, significantly above the chance level. However, when it came to children\'s faces and names, participants were unable to make accurate associations. Complementing our lab studies, we employed a machine-learning framework to process facial image data and found that facial representations of adults with the same name were more similar to each other than to those of adults with different names. This pattern of similarity was absent among the facial representations of children, thereby strengthening the case for the self-fulfilling prophecy hypothesis. Furthermore, the face-name matching effect was evident for adults but not for children\'s faces that were artificially aged to resemble adults, supporting the conjectured role of social development in this effect. Together, these findings suggest that even our facial appearance can be influenced by a social factor such as our name, confirming the potent impact of social expectations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:作者姓名不完整,仅引用第一个可用的首字母而不是完整的名字,是MEDLINE中一个长期存在的问题,对生物医学文献系统产生负面影响。这项研究的目的是为MEDLINE创建增强作者姓名(EAN)数据集,以最大程度地增加完整作者姓名的数量。
    方法:EAN数据集是基于从多个文献数据库(如MEDLINE)收集的作者姓名进行大规模名称比较和恢复而构建的。Microsoft学术图,和语义学者。我们通过对EAN和MEDLINE的两个重要任务的作者姓名数据集(MAN)进行比较和统计分析来评估EAN对生物医学文献系统的影响。作者姓名搜索和作者姓名歧义消除。
    结果:评估结果表明,EAN将MEDLINE中的作者全名数量从6973万提高到了11090万。EAN不仅在2002年NLM更改其作者姓名索引策略之前恢复了大量的缩写名称,而且还提高了之后发表的文章中作者姓名的可用性。对作者姓名搜索和作者姓名歧义消除任务的评估表明,与MAN相比,EAN能够显着增强这两个任务。
    结论:EAN对全名的广泛覆盖表明,名称不完整的问题可以在很大程度上得到缓解。这对于开发改进的生物医学文献系统具有重要意义。EAN可在https://zenodo.org/record/10251358获得,更新版本可在https://zenodo.org/records/10663234获得。
    OBJECTIVE: Author name incompleteness, referring to only first initial available instead of full first name, is a long-standing problem in MEDLINE and has a negative impact on biomedical literature systems. The purpose of this study is to create an Enhanced Author Names (EAN) dataset for MEDLINE that maximizes the number of complete author names.
    METHODS: The EAN dataset is built based on a large-scale name comparison and restoration with author names collected from multiple literature databases such as MEDLINE, Microsoft Academic Graph, and Semantic Scholar. We assess the impact of EAN on biomedical literature systems by conducting comparative and statistical analyses between EAN and MEDLINE\'s author names dataset (MAN) on 2 important tasks, author name search and author name disambiguation.
    RESULTS: Evaluation results show that EAN improves the number of full author names in MEDLINE from 69.73 million to 110.9 million. EAN not only restores a substantial number of abbreviated names prior to the year 2002 when the NLM changed its author name indexing policy but also improves the availability of full author names in articles published afterward. The evaluation of the author name search and author name disambiguation tasks reveal that EAN is able to significantly enhance both tasks compared to MAN.
    CONCLUSIONS: The extensive coverage of full names in EAN suggests that the name incompleteness issue can be largely mitigated. This has significant implications for the development of an improved biomedical literature system. EAN is available at https://zenodo.org/record/10251358, and an updated version is available at https://zenodo.org/records/10663234.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    研究表明,感知连续听觉刺激的顺序可能会受到其名称性的影响。本研究重新检验了这一假设,使用要求参与者报告连续呈现(没有刺激间隙)环境的顺序的任务(即,容易命名的刺激)和抽象(即,难以命名的刺激)持续时间短的声音(即200ms)。使用相同的序列,我们还检查了通过管理枚举任务感知的声音的准确性。数据分析表明,对于环境和抽象声音,订购任务的准确性同样低。而与后者相比,前者的枚举任务准确性更高。重要的是,枚举任务的总体准确性没有达到上限水平,暗示了对连续呈现的刺激的感知的一些限制。总的来说,命名流畅性似乎会影响声音枚举,但是订单感知没有效果。此外,注意到序列中每个声音的位置对订购准确性的影响。我们的结果质疑早期的观念,这些观念表明秩序感知是由刺激的可命名性介导的,并留下了记忆容量限制可能发挥作用的可能性。
    Research has shown that perceiving the order of successive auditory stimuli could be affected by their nameability. The present research re-examined this hypothesis, using tasks requiring participants to report the order of successively presented (with no interstimulus gaps) environmental (i.e., easily named stimuli) and abstract (i.e., hard-to-name stimuli) sounds of short duration (i.e., 200 ms). Using the same sequences, we also examined the accuracy of the sounds perceived by administering enumeration tasks. Data analyses showed that accuracy in the ordering tasks was equally low for both environmental and abstract sounds, whereas accuracy in the enumeration tasks was higher for the former as compared to the latter sounds. Importantly, overall accuracy in the enumeration tasks did not reach ceiling levels, suggesting some limitations in the perception of successively presented stimuli. Overall, naming fluency seemed to affect sound enumeration, but no effects were obtained for order perception. Furthermore, an effect of each sound\'s location in a sequence on ordering accuracy was noted. Our results question earlier notions suggesting that order perception is mediated by stimuli\'s nameability and leave open the possibility that memory capacity limits may play a role.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这种互文分析的总体目标是探索Wolde的小说《捍卫名字》(1969年),以期识别和解释贯穿整个叙事的几个主题和风格互文。根据现有的研究,很少有批判性研究利用互文性理论来分析和解释埃塞俄比亚的英语散文小说,特别是在小说类型中。本研究旨在部分填补这一关键空白。在这样做的时候,互文性理论被用作研究的理论分析框架。这种互文分析的发现集中在整本书《捍卫名字》情节中编织的主题和风格互文上。这些互文性包括圣经典故,殖民文学装置,当代理论思想著作,以及文化和历史话语,这本书除了其他文学和非文学作品外,还互文参与其中。本研究通过互文分析提供了有关捍卫名称的主题多样性及其参与多种互文的深刻信息。它增强了读者对故事的理解,字符,以及小说所解决的更大的社会政治局势,展示了作者融合各种文学作品的技巧,圣经,意识形态,和文化方面。
    The general objective of this intertextual analysis\'s was to explore Wolde\'s novel Defend the Name (1969) with the view to identify and interpret the several thematic and stylistic intertexts that are woven throughout the narrative. Based on available research, there is a scarcity of critical studies that have utilized the theory of intertextuality for the analysis and interpretation of Ethiopian prose fiction in English, particularly within the novel genre. The current study was aimed to partially fill in this critical gap. In doing so, the theory of intertextuality is employed as theoretical-analytical framework of the study. The findings of this intertextual analysis concentrated on the thematic and stylistic intertexts that were woven throughout the plot of the book Defend the Name. These intertexts included biblical allusions, colonial literary devices, contemporary theoretical and ideological works, and cultural and historical discourses that the book intertextually engages with in addition to other literary and nonliterary works. This study provides insightful information about the thematic diversity of Defend the Name and its involvement with multiple intertexts through its intertextual analysis. It enhances the reader\'s comprehension of the story, characters, and larger sociopolitical situations that the novel addresses, demonstrating the author\'s skill in fusing together a variety of literary, scriptural, ideological, and cultural aspects.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Letter
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在学习过程中准确评估自己的记忆表现的能力对于适应行为至关重要,但是这种元记忆功能背后的大脑机制还没有得到很好的理解。我们在健康的年轻人(n=32)中使用脑磁图研究了面名联想学习任务中记忆准确性和回顾性记忆置信度的神经相关性。我们发现,与低置信度相比,高回顾性置信度与编码期间更强的枕骨事件相关领域和检索期间广泛的事件相关领域相关。另一方面,在编码和检索过程中,记忆准确性与内侧时间活动有关,但只有在低置信度试验中。检索过程中顶叶区域α/β带振荡功率的降低与较高的记忆置信度有关。此外,在单次试验水平的代表性相似性分析显示,在编码和检索过程中,与记忆准确性和置信度相关的分布式但可区分的神经活动。总之,我们的研究揭示了联想学习过程中与记忆置信度和准确性相关的不同神经活动模式,并强调了顶叶区域在元记忆中的关键作用。
    The ability to accurately assess one\'s own memory performance during learning is essential for adaptive behavior, but the brain mechanisms underlying this metamemory function are not well understood. We investigated the neural correlates of memory accuracy and retrospective memory confidence in a face-name associative learning task using magnetoencephalography in healthy young adults (n = 32). We found that high retrospective confidence was associated with stronger occipital event-related fields during encoding and widespread event-related fields during retrieval compared to low confidence. On the other hand, memory accuracy was linked to medial temporal activities during both encoding and retrieval, but only in low-confidence trials. A decrease in oscillatory power at alpha/beta bands in the parietal regions during retrieval was associated with higher memory confidence. In addition, representational similarity analysis at the single-trial level revealed distributed but differentiable neural activities associated with memory accuracy and confidence during both encoding and retrieval. In summary, our study unveiled distinct neural activity patterns related to memory confidence and accuracy during associative learning and underscored the crucial role of parietal regions in metamemory.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    与听众相关的听觉刺激即使在无人看管的情况下也有可能引起注意力集中,听众自己的名字是一个特别有效的刺激。我们报告了两个实验,以测试听众自己名字在正常语音和时间压缩语音中的注意力捕捉潜力。在实验1中,通过视觉单词分类任务对39名参与者进行了测试,其中使用未压缩的口语名称作为背景听觉干扰因素。当参与者听到自己的名字而不是其他名字时,单词分类表现较慢,在最后的测试中,他们比其他名字更快发现自己的名字。实验2使用相同的任务范式,但是听觉干扰者是时间压缩的名字。在每种情况下对25名参与者测试了三个压缩水平。当听到自己的名字时,参与者的单词分类表现再次比听到其他名字时慢;轻微压缩时,减慢的速度最强,强烈压缩时最弱。个人相关的时间压缩语音有可能吸引注意力,但是捕获的程度取决于压缩的程度。时间压缩语音的注意力捕获具有实际意义,并为听觉分心的双工机制提供了部分证据。
    Auditory stimuli that are relevant to a listener have the potential to capture focal attention even when unattended, the listener\'s own name being a particularly effective stimulus. We report two experiments to test the attention-capturing potential of the listener\'s own name in normal speech and time-compressed speech. In Experiment 1, 39 participants were tested with a visual word categorization task with uncompressed spoken names as background auditory distractors. Participants\' word categorization performance was slower when hearing their own name rather than other names, and in a final test, they were faster at detecting their own name than other names. Experiment 2 used the same task paradigm, but the auditory distractors were time-compressed names. Three compression levels were tested with 25 participants in each condition. Participants\' word categorization performance was again slower when hearing their own name than when hearing other names; the slowing was strongest with slight compression and weakest with intense compression. Personally relevant time-compressed speech has the potential to capture attention, but the degree of capture depends on the level of compression. Attention capture by time-compressed speech has practical significance and provides partial evidence for the duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    心理学研究最近提出了Alexinomia,其特征是在说名字时表现出抑制行为,作为一种独特的社会心理现象。Alexinomia与焦虑和回避行为有关,因此严重影响每天的社交互动和关系。这项研究旨在探索这种新建立且鲜为人知的心理现象的患病率,并进一步确定其对日常生活的影响。为此,系统地搜索了在线建议和讨论论坛,以查找有关说出名字的问题的线索和提及。我们从英语评论中分析了广泛的数据集,这些评论讨论了与Alexinomia相关的经验和行为,包括不同的人口统计和地理区域。基于对257个独特来源的定性分析的发现表明,暗恋是一种普遍现象。此外,分析的在线材料显示,受影响的个人使用各种有效和无效的应对策略,并经历不同程度的严重程度,这可能会随着训练而减少。因此,这项研究的结果强调了摘除术是相关的,然而高度研究不足,研究领域,并增加我们对日常生活中的Alexinomia经历及其潜在起源的了解,特别是与社交焦虑和早年家庭动态有关。
    Psychological research has recently proposed alexinomia, characterised by an inhibited behaviour in saying names, as a distinct psychosocial phenomenon. Alexinomia is associated with anxiety and avoidance behaviours with regards to saying names and thus severely impacts every day social interactions and relationships. This study aimed to explore the prevalence of this newly established and poorly understood psychological phenomenon and to further determine its impact on everyday life. For this purpose, online advice and discussion forums were systematically searched for threads on and mentions of problems with saying names. We analysed a broad dataset from English-language comments discussing alexinomia-related experiences and behaviours, inclusive of varied demographics and geographical regions. The findings based on the qualitative analysis of 257 unique sources show that alexinomia is a widespread phenomenon. Moreover, the analysed online materials showed affected individual\'s use of a variety of effective and ineffective coping strategies and experience varying degrees of severity, which can potentially diminish with training. The study\'s results therefore highlight alexinomia as a relevant, yet highly under researched, field of study, and add to our knowledge on the experience of alexinomia in everyday life and its potential origins, especially relating to social anxiety and early-life familial dynamic.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号