music notation

音乐符号
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当视觉阅读分数时,定时符需要实时决定在解释音乐材料时使用哪种坚持来演奏特定的音符。我们的主要调查点旨在了解表演者采用的粘贴模式以及节奏稳定性如何影响它们。本文分析了双手动测序(即,坚持)31名定时主义者在视力阅读任务中的模式。与打击乐教学文献中常见的模型粘贴模式相比,我们分析了他们的结果。结果表明,虽然手部优势在个体的粘附模式中起着至关重要的作用,节奏模式的稳定性也可能极大地影响观察到的特定粘附策略。在节奏稳定的地区,表演者在很大程度上坚持文献中的两种常规粘贴模式之一(优势手引导和交替)。节奏模式变得更加不稳定的地方,表演者分成不同的群体。此外,几个表演者展示了粘附模式,这些模式是模型粘附模式的混合或相反,对他们的视力阅读能力的成功没有任何影响。总的来说,没有两个表演者表现出相同的粘附模式。就打击乐教学法而言,我们的研究结果表明,表演者可能会受益于对模型粘贴策略适应性的认识。最后,通过定位符号和听觉复杂性之间的差异,我们为进一步研究节律稳定性和双手动排序提供了依据。
    When sight-reading a score, a timpanist needs to decide in real-time which stick to use to play a specific note while interpreting the musical material. Our main point of inquiry seeks to understand which sticking patterns performers employ and how they are affected by rhythmic stability. This paper analyzes the bi-manual sequencing (i.e., sticking) patterns of 31 timpanists in a sight-reading task. We analyze their results compared to model sticking patterns common in percussion pedagogical literature. Results show that while hand dominance plays an essential role in an individual\'s sticking pattern, the stability of a rhythmic pattern may also dramatically influence the observed particular sticking strategies. In areas of rhythmic stability, performers largely adhered to one of two conventional sticking patterns in the literature (dominant hand lead & alternating). Where rhythmic patterns became more unstable, the performers separated into diverse sticking groups. Moreover, several performers demonstrated sticking patterns which were hybrids or an inverse of the model sticking patterns, without any impact on the success of their sight-reading abilities. Overall, no two individual performers demonstrated the same sticking pattern. In terms of percussion pedagogy, our findings suggest that performers may benefit from an awareness of the adaptability of model sticking strategies. Lastly, we make the case for further study of rhythmic stability and bi-manual sequencing by locating the difference between notational and aural complexity.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在这次审查中,我们专注于音乐领域的眼动追踪研究的方法论方面,在1994年至2017年之间发布和/或可用,我们确定了潜在的富有成果的下一步,以增加这一新兴领域的连贯性和系统性。我们回顾并讨论音乐刺激的选择,执行这些操作的条件(即控制演奏节奏和音乐阅读协议),表演者的音乐专业知识水平,以及处理性能错误和眼动数据。我们建议,尽管迄今为止的研究缺乏方法论上的连贯性,仔细思考早期的方法选择可以帮助制定未来的研究问题和定位新的工作。这些步骤将代表着累积研究传统的进步,通过系统和一致地使用刺激来建立共同的理解,研究背景和分析方法。
    In this review, we focus on the methodological aspects of eye-tracking research in the domain of music, published and/or available between 1994 and 2017, and we identify potentially fruitful next steps to increase coherence and systematicity within this emerging field. We review and discuss choices of musical stimuli, the conditions under which these were performed (i.e. control of performance tempo and music-reading protocols), performer\'s level of musical expertise, and handling of performance errors and eye-movement data. We propose that despite a lack of methodological coherence in research to date, careful reflection on earlier methodological choices can help in formulating future research questions and in positioning new work. These steps would represent progress towards a cumulative research tradition, where joint understanding is built by systematic and consistent use of stimuli, research settings and methods of analysis.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    响应代码的空间数值关联(SNARC)表明存在数量大小和响应位置之间的关联,对小数字有更快的左按键响应,对大数字有更快的右按键响应。我们调查了板条上的音符与反应执行空间之间是否存在类似的关联,涉及业余和专家音乐家(实验1)。此外,在实验2中,我们进一步研究了两组专家音乐家(钢琴和横向长笛演奏者)的这种关联,他们在乐器上的音符映射不同。结果表明,仅对于受过正规教育的音乐家,音符与反应执行空间之间存在明显的关联。此外,这种联系似乎不受演奏的特定乐器的影响,因为钢琴和横向长笛演奏者都表现出相同的效果方向(左键按压优势为低音符,反之亦然)。
    The Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) suggests the existence of an association between number magnitude and response location, with faster left key-press responses to small numbers and faster right key-press responses to large numbers. We investigated whether a similar association exists between musical notes on the stave and the space of response execution, involving amateur and expert musicians (Experiment 1). Moreover, in Experiment 2 we further investigated such association in two groups of expert musicians (piano and transverse flute players) who differ in the note mapping on their instruments. Results indicate a clear association between musical notes and the space of response execution only for musicians with formal education. Furthermore, this association seems not to be influenced by the specific instrument played, as both piano and transverse flute players showed the same effect direction (left key-press advantage for low notes, and vice versa).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    乐谱阅读和单词阅读有很多共同点,从他们的历史渊源到他们的认知基础和神经相关。在腹侧枕颞叶皮层(VOT),用于单词阅读的所谓视觉单词形式区域的专业化与其与遥远语言区域的特权结构连通性有关。在这里,我们研究了解剖连接如何与VOT中专门用于音乐符号或单词的区域的分离有关。在一群专业音乐家和非音乐家中,我们使用概率纤维束成像结合任务相关功能MRI来识别单独定义的单词和音乐选择性左VOT区域的连接.尽管他们非常接近,这些区域的结构连通性显著不同,不管音乐专业。与单词选择区域相比,音乐选择区域与后外侧颞区的连接程度更高,which,相反,与前腹侧颞叶皮层的连接明显更多。此外,音乐专业知识对音乐区域的连通性有双重影响。首先,音乐家的音乐范围明显大于非音乐家,与Perisylvian音乐相关区域的连通性略高。第二,音乐和词束之间的空间相似性在音乐家中显著增加,与音乐家的语言和音乐功能激活的重叠增加一致,与非音乐家相比。这些结果支持这样的观点,对于音乐,对于文字,非常特殊的解剖学连接会影响不同VOT区域的专业化,反过来,这些联系被单词或音乐阅读的专业知识选择性地增强。
    Musical score reading and word reading have much in common, from their historical origins to their cognitive foundations and neural correlates. In the ventral occipitotemporal cortex (VOT), the specialization of the so-called Visual Word Form Area for word reading has been linked to its privileged structural connectivity to distant language regions. Here we investigated how anatomical connectivity relates to the segregation of regions specialized for musical notation or words in the VOT. In a cohort of professional musicians and non-musicians, we used probabilistic tractography combined with task-related functional MRI to identify the connections of individually defined word- and music-selective left VOT regions. Despite their close proximity, these regions differed significantly in their structural connectivity, irrespective of musical expertise. The music-selective region was significantly more connected to posterior lateral temporal regions than the word-selective region, which, conversely, was significantly more connected to anterior ventral temporal cortex. Furthermore, musical expertise had a double impact on the connectivity of the music region. First, music tracts were significantly larger in musicians than in non-musicians, associated with marginally higher connectivity to perisylvian music-related areas. Second, the spatial similarity between music and word tracts was significantly increased in musicians, consistently with the increased overlap of language and music functional activations in musicians, as compared to non-musicians. These results support the view that, for music as for words, very specific anatomical connections influence the specialization of distinct VOT areas, and that reciprocally those connections are selectively enhanced by the expertise for word or music reading.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    音乐和数学需要抽象思维和使用符号符号。关于从音乐训练到数学成就的转移存在争议。当前的研究检查了两个综合干预计划的效果,这些计划代表了整体方法与声学方法,关于分数知识。三班四年级学生参加了12节零分课程:一节课参加了“MusiMath”整体课程(n=30),重点是旋律中的节奏。另一个班参加了“学术音乐”声学课程(Courey等人。,EducStudMath81:251,2012)(n=25),仅使用节奏。第三班接受常规分数课程(比较组,n=22)。这两个音乐节目的学生都学会了通过鼓掌和击鼓来编写音符并执行有节奏的模式,这是他们分数课程的一部分。他们致力于添加音符以产生数字(分数),并创建了加法/减法问题与音符。音乐程序使用了4/4时间签名,一半,四分之一和八分音符。在数学课上,学生们学习了音乐持续时间和12,14,18分之间的类比,但也练习了12,14,18以外的分数。以前评估过音乐和数学,紧接着,干预后3个月和6个月。干预前后的分析表明,与对照组相比,只有“MusiMath”组显示出更多的转移到干预训练和未训练的分数。“学术音乐”小组显示出训练分数的趋势。尽管两个音乐组在训练分数干预后3个月和6个月优于对照组,只有“MusiMath”组在未训练分数中表现出更大的增益。受过训练的分数比未经训练的分数更明显。本文的视频摘要可以在https://youtu查看。是/uJ_KWWDO624。
    Music and mathematics require abstract thinking and using symbolic notations. Controversy exists regarding transfer from musical training to math achievements. The current study examined the effect of two integrated intervention programs representing holistic versus acoustic approaches, on fraction knowledge. Three classes of fourth graders attended 12 lessons on fractions: One class attended the \'MusiMath\' holistic program (n = 30) focusing on rhythm within the melody. Another class attended the \'Academic Music\' acoustic program (Courey et al., Educ Stud Math 81:251, 2012) (n = 25) which uses rhythm only. The third class received regular fraction lessons (comparison group, n = 22). Students in both music programs learned to write musical notes and perform rhythmic patterns through clapping and drumming as part of their fraction lessons. They worked toward adding musical notes to produce a number (fraction), and created addition/subtraction problems with musical notes. The music programs used a 4/4 time signature with whole, half, quarter and eighth notes. In the math lessons, the students learned the analogy between musical durations and 12,14,18 fractions, but also practiced fractions other than 12,14,18 . Music and math were assessed before, immediately following, and 3- and 6-months post-intervention. Pre- to post-intervention analyses indicated that only the \'MusiMath\' group showed greater transfer to intervention-trained and untrained fractions than the comparison group. The \'Academic Music\' group showed a trend on trained fractions. Although both music groups outperformed the comparison group 3- and 6-months post-intervention on trained fractions, only the \'MusiMath\' group demonstrated greater gains in untrained fractions. Gains were more evident in trained than in untrained fractions. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/uJ_KWWDO624.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    The acquisition of literacy has a profound impact on the functional specialization and lateralization of the visual cortex. Due to the overall lateralization of the language network, specialization for printed words develops in the left occipitotemporal cortex, allegedly inducing a secondary shift of visual face processing to the right, in literate as compared to illiterate subjects. Applying the same logic to the acquisition of high-level musical literacy, we predicted that, in musicians as compared to non-musicians, occipitotemporal activations should show a leftward shift for music reading, and an additional rightward push for face perception. To test these predictions, professional musicians and non-musicians viewed pictures of musical notation, faces, words, tools and houses in the MRI, and laterality was assessed in the ventral stream combining ROI and voxel-based approaches. The results supported both predictions, and allowed to locate the leftward shift to the inferior temporal gyrus and the rightward shift to the fusiform cortex. Moreover, these laterality shifts generalized to categories other than music and faces. Finally, correlation measures across subjects did not support a causal link between the leftward and rightward shifts. Thus the acquisition of an additional perceptual expertise extensively modifies the laterality pattern in the visual system.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:能够识别情绪的表达对于有效的临床沟通研究至关重要。然而,传统的语言编码系统通常不能表示非词汇或语音表达的情绪(即,不是通过语言本身,而是通过声音音调,速度/节奏/节奏,和体积)。
    方法:在自然医院环境中使用姑息治疗咨询的录音,两位经验丰富的音乐学者采用了西方乐谱,以及数字音频程序的图形实现(钢琴卷可视化),在视觉上代表患者情绪激动的“窒息”时刻的对话声音特征。
    结果:西方乐谱显示了音高和速度变化与患者情绪相对应的方式:强度急剧上升,然后逐渐消失。钢琴卷可视化是一个有用的补充。
    结论:使用音乐符号来说明医院环境中的姑息治疗对话可以使研究人员在其他情况下将其视为直觉或主观的情感表达的几个方面可见。各种形式和格式的音乐符号技术和声音可视化技术应被视为临床通信研究中传统编码工具的富有成果和补充替代品。
    结论:音乐符号为研究人员和学习者提供了机会,让他们“看到”交流在临床接触中的演变,特别是在人际交流的词汇和语音特征相互一致和不一致的地方。
    OBJECTIVE: Being able to identify expressions of emotion is crucial to effective clinical communication research. However, traditional linguistic coding systems often cannot represent emotions that are expressed nonlexically or phonologically (i.e., not through words themselves but through vocal pitch, speed/rhythm/tempo, and volume).
    METHODS: Using audio recording of a palliative care consultation in the natural hospital setting, two experienced music scholars employed Western musical notation, as well as the graphic realization of a digital audio program (Piano roll visualization), to visually represent the sonic features of conversation where a patient has an emotional \"choke\" moment.
    RESULTS: Western musical notation showed the ways that changes in pitch and rate correspond to the patient\'s emotion: rising sharply in intensity before slowly fading away. Piano roll visualization is a helpful supplement.
    CONCLUSIONS: Using musical notation to illustrate palliative care conversations in the hospital setting can render visible for analysis several aspects of emotional expression that researchers otherwise experience as intuitive or subjective. Various forms and formats of musical notation techniques and sonic visualization technologies should be considered as fruitful and complementary alternatives to traditional coding tools in clinical communications research.
    CONCLUSIONS: Musical notation offers opportunity for both researchers and learners to \"see\" how communication evolves in clinical encounters, particularly where the lexical and phonological features of interpersonal communication are concordant and discordant with one another.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

公众号