human–animal interaction

人与动物的互动
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人与动物的关系对动物福利至关重要。温和的处理增强了猪的舒适度,而粗糙的处理会导致恐惧和压力。这项研究研究了不同的人与动物关系质量如何影响36头保育猪的行为和心率变异性(线性和非线性参数)。六个多星期,猪经历了阳性(n=12),最小(n=12),或负(n=12)人类处理。然后在实验舞台上评估他们对处理程序的反应,分为四个阶段:习惯化,暴露于站着和坐着的处理程序,强迫互动。受到负面处理的猪表现出增加的恐惧相关行为,花费更少的时间与处理者联系。他们还表现出增强的应激反应,与阳性处理猪相比,LF/HF比率和L平均值更高。相反,轻轻处理的猪表现出附属物行为,接受更多的笔画,和更高的副交感神经激活,由更大的RMSSD/SDNN和SampEn值表示,表明一种更积极的情感状态。最少处理的猪表现出一些行为上的相似性,温和处理的猪,尽管生理数据表明,这种相互作用可能对温和处理的猪更有益。这些结果强调了人与动物关系对猪福利的影响,并强调了在此类评估中纳入非线性心率变异性参数的价值。
    The human-animal relationship is crucial for animal welfare. Gentle handling enhances pigs\' comfort while rough handling causes fear and stress. This study examined how different human-animal relationship qualities affect the behavior and heart rate variability (linear and non-linear parameters) of 36 nursery pigs. Over six weeks, pigs experienced positive (n = 12), minimal (n = 12), or negative (n = 12) human handling. Their responses to handlers were then assessed in an experimental arena with four phases: habituation, exposure to the handler standing and sitting, and forced interaction. Pigs subjected to negative handling exhibited increased fear-related behaviors, spending less time in contact with the handler. They also exhibited heightened stress responses, with greater LF/HF ratio and Lmean values compared with positively handled pigs. Conversely, gently handled pigs displayed affiliative behaviors, accepting more strokes, and higher parasympathetic activation, indicated by greater RMSSD/SDNN and SampEn values, suggesting a more positive affective state. Minimally handled pigs exhibited some behavioral similarities to gently handled pigs, although physiological data indicated that the interaction was likely more rewarding for the gently handled pigs. These results emphasize the impact of human-animal relationships on pig welfare and highlight the value of incorporating non-linear heart rate variability parameters in such evaluations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对人类及其物体的吸引力增加通常是在反复和积极的人类与野生动物相遇(例如在旅游环境中提供的食物)之后产生的。这种“过度吸引”的原因,这可能是由于人类和食物之间的学习联系,在野生动物中的研究仍然很少。了解人类对动物反应的影响对于防止负面影响(例如侵略)至关重要。我们向两组自由放养的棕色skuas展示了三个新颖的物体(Catharactaantarcticassp。lonnbergi)在遥远的南极,它们的栖息地显示没有或最小的人为干扰。一组(Verte)中的Skuas以前曾与人类实验者一起参加过重复的食物奖励行为和认知任务;另一组(Ratmanoff)中的skuas从未这样做过。物体由(i)一个类似天然食物的物体(塑料鱼)组成,(ii)一个人为食物对象(真正的蛋糕片),和(iii)一个人为的非食物物体(黄色手套)。Verte小组的skuas接近人类实验者,并且在新颖的物体上更快地啄食。因此,人类与食物的联系可能会导致对人类的吸引力增加,并对以前幼稚的棕色斯卡鲁进行了新奇的探索,使这个物种成为研究人类经验对野生动物行为的影响的有用模型。
    Increased attraction to humans and their objects often arises after repeated and positive human-wildlife encounters (e.g. food provided in tourist settings). The causes of this \'over-attraction\', which may result from a learned association between humans and food, are still poorly studied in wild animals. Understanding the influence of humans on animals\' responses is yet crucial to prevent negative effects (e.g. aggression). We presented three novel objects to two groups of free-ranging brown skuas (Catharacta antarctica ssp. lonnbergi) in the remote sub-Antarctic, where their habitats show no or minimal human disturbance. Skuas in one group (Verte) had previously participated in repeated food-rewarded behavioural and cognitive tasks with a human experimenter; skuas in the other group (Ratmanoff) had never done so. Objects consisted of (i) one natural-food-resembling object (plastic fish), (ii) one anthropogenic food object (real cake slice), and (iii) one anthropogenic non-food object (yellow glove). Verte group skuas approached the human experimenter and pecked significantly more and sooner at novel objects. Human-food association may have thus resulted in increased attraction to humans and novelty exploration in previously naive brown skuas, making this species a useful model for investigating the consequences of experience with humans on wildlife behaviour.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在这个实验中,我们研究了一个啮齿动物模型,该模型选择了57代以上,在母系分离期间,超声发声(USV)的发生率高或低。我们调查了这种繁殖对成年动物随后的声音输出的影响,比较不同发育阶段的声学变量。我们假设幼犬USV率的选择会影响成年USV的产生,而不会影响低频呼叫。与这个假设相反,我们发现在选定的成人行之间,USV呼叫数或其他声学变量均无差异.相反,我们发现幼犬USV选择主要影响成人的低频(人类可听到的)呼叫。此外,低频发声并不完全符合体重和基频之间的预测相关性:高线男性,虽然平均来说最重,没有产生最低的基本频率。我们的发现表明,多向性选择早期超声发声行为会导致解剖产生机制和/或神经控制的变化,从而影响低频呼叫。
    In this experiment, we studied a rodent model selected over 57 generations for high or low rates of ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) during maternal separation as pups. We investigated the influence of this breeding on the adult animals\' subsequent vocal output, comparing acoustic variables across developmental stages. We hypothesized that selection on pup USV rate would impact adult USV production without affecting lower frequency calls. Contrary to this hypothesis, we found neither number of USV calls or other acoustic variables to differ among selected adult lines. Instead, we found that pup USV selection mainly affected adults\' low-frequency (human-audible) calls. Furthermore, low-frequency vocalizations did not fully fit a predicted correlation between body weight and fundamental frequency: high line males, although the heaviest on average, did not produce the lowest fundamental frequencies. Our findings suggest that selection for early ultrasonic vocal behaviour pleiotropically results in changes in anatomical production mechanisms and/or neural control affecting low-frequency calls.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    近年来,短头犬的品种越来越受欢迎,尽管显示了构象相关疾病的高发病率和早期死亡率。有人认为,这种流行可能是通过公众对这些短嘴狗看起来特别像婴儿或“可爱”的看法来解释的。\"这里,通过分析在线出售的狗和小狗广告的单词内容,研究了短嘴品种特别可能被描述为可爱的假设。被广告的狗的年龄和品种被考虑,除了每个广告的文字是否包括“可爱”或两个相关的词:“可爱”和“甜蜜”。对整个广告样本(n=43,312)的分析表明,年轻的狗更有可能被广告为“可爱”和“可爱”,“而年龄较大的人更有可能被宣传为“甜蜜”。\"shortmuzzled,短脑品种(颅面比<0.5)更有可能被宣传为“可爱,6个月以下的短头幼犬特别可能被称为“可爱”,也是“可爱”。\"然而,品种大小对广告中的单词使用有更大和更广泛的影响,较小的狗更频繁地使用这三个词做广告:“可爱,\"\"可爱,\"和\"甜。“当仅考虑成年狗的数据时(n=11,400),并使用了持续的枪口缩短和年龄数据,发现了一些不同和更复杂的结果模式。“可爱”和“可爱”的使用与这些成年狗的枪口缩短程度无关,但“甜”更常用于口吻较长的品种的广告中。我们得出的结论是,当前的数据集为以下断言提供了部分支持:短嘴狗被描述为比长嘴狗更“可爱”,但小尺寸是一个更好的预测使用“可爱”和它的同义词。
    Brachycephalic dog breeds have become increasingly popular in recent years, despite showing a high incidence of conformation-related disorders and early mortality. It has been suggested that this popularity might be explained by public perceptions of these short-muzzled dogs as looking particularly infant-like or \"cute.\" Here, the hypothesis that short-muzzled breeds are especially likely to be described as cute was investigated by analyzing the word contents of advertisements for dogs and puppies being sold online. The ages and breeds of dogs being advertised were considered, in addition to whether the text of each advertisement included the word \"cute\" or two associated words: \"adorable\" and \"sweet.\" Analyses of the entire sample of advertisements (n = 43,312) indicated that younger dogs were more likely to be advertised as \"cute\" and \"adorable,\" while older ones were more likely to be advertised as \"sweet.\" Shortmuzzled, brachycephalic breeds (cranio-facial ratio < 0.5) were more likely to be advertised as \"cute,\" with brachycephalic puppies under 6 months of age being particularly likely to be called \"cute\" and also \"adorable.\" However, breed size had a larger and wider effect on word use in advertisements, with smaller dogs being advertised more frequently using all three words: \"cute,\" \"adorable,\" and \"sweet.\" When data for adult dogs only were considered (n = 11,400), and continuous muzzle shortening and age data were used, a somewhat different and more complex pattern of results were found. Use of the words \"cute\" and \"adorable\" were not associated with degree of muzzle shortening among these adult dogs, but \"sweet\" was used more often in advertisements for longer-muzzled breeds. We conclude that the present dataset provides partial support for the assertion that short-muzzled dogs are described as more \"cute\" than longer-muzzled ones, but that small size is a better predictor of the use of \"cute\" and its synonyms.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    依恋关系被广泛认为对增加亲社会倾向有影响,现有文献表明人类依恋可以增加移情过程,从而潜在地促进亲社会行为。鉴于宠物经常满足依恋数字的标准,这项研究调查了观察到的人类依恋之间的关联,同理心,亲社会态度延伸到人与动物的互动(HAI)。这项研究考察了宠物依恋之间的关系,动物移情,和亲社会对人类的态度。该研究假设动物移情调解宠物依恋和亲社会态度之间的关联。对343名菲律宾参与者进行了横断面调查,主要由具有大学教育背景的单身女性年轻人组成。参与者完成了一系列评估,包括当代伴侣动物结合量表(CCABS),动物移情量表(AES)和成人亲社会量表(PSA)。与我们的假设一致,我们的研究表明,动物移情在宠物依恋和对人类态度之间的关系中起着重要的中介作用。我们发现,更强的宠物依恋与增强的动物同理心呈正相关,随后导致亲社会态度水平的提高。我们的发现促使人们对理解人与动物关系的含义进行讨论,并为未来的研究探索提供了途径。
    Attachment relationships are widely recognized as influential in increasing prosocial tendencies, with existing literature indicating that human attachment can increase empathetic processes, thereby potentially facilitating prosocial behavior. Given that pets frequently fulfill the criteria for attachment figures, this study investigates whether the observed associations among human attachment, empathy, and prosocial attitudes extend to human-animal interactions (HAI). This study examines the relationship between pet attachment, animal empathy, and prosocial attitudes toward humans. The study hypothesizes that animal empathy mediates the association between pet attachment and prosocial attitudes. A cross-sectional survey was administered to 343 Filipino participants, predominantly consisting of single female young adults with college education backgrounds. Participants completed a battery of assessments including the Contemporary Companion Animal Bonding Scale (CCABS), the Animal Empathy Scale (AES), and the Prosocialness Scale for Adults (PSA). Aligned with our hypothesis, our study reveals that animal empathy plays a significant mediating role in the relationship between pet attachment and attitudes toward humans. We found that stronger pet attachment correlates positively with heightened animal empathy, subsequently leading to elevated levels of prosocial attitudes. Our findings prompt discussions on implications for understanding human-animal relationships and suggest avenues for future research exploration.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尽管许多伴侣动物(或“宠物”)主人报告说,他们与宠物的关系很重要,我们对动物所有权持续时间和动物生命阶段与关系质量的关系知之甚少。在1303只狗和猫主人的样本中,本研究探索了关系持续时间之间的关联,宠物生活阶段(小狗/小猫,年轻的成年人,成熟的成年人,andsenior),关系质量的四个标志:与宠物相关的自我扩张,感知到的宠物反应能力,感知宠物不敏感,和人与动物的纽带。我们发现,关系持续时间与狗和猫主人的自我扩张呈负相关和线性关系。比较不同宠物生命阶段的关系质量标记的结果表明,对于狗和猫的主人来说,与年龄较大的动物的所有者相比,年轻动物的所有者的自我扩张更高。对感知的宠物反应没有显著影响,感知宠物不敏感,或基于关系持续时间或动物生命阶段的人与动物联系。这些发现表明,与宠物和动物生命阶段的关系的持续时间对人们感觉宠物帮助他们成长的程度有影响,而人与宠物关系质量的其他标记可能因其他经验而异。
    Although many companion animal (or \"pet\") owners report that their relationships with their pets are important, we know little about how animal ownership duration and animal life stage are related to relationship quality. In a sample of 1303 dog and cat owners, the present research explored the associations between relationship duration, pet life stage (puppy/kitten, young adult, mature adult, and senior), and four markers of relationship quality: pet-related self-expansion, perceived pet responsiveness, perceived pet insensitivity, and human-animal bond. We found that relationship duration was negatively and linearly associated with self-expansion for both dog and cat owners. Results comparing relationship quality markers across pet life stages revealed that for both dog and cat owners, self-expansion was higher for owners of younger animals compared to owners of older animals. There were no significant effects for perceived pet responsiveness, perceived pet insensitivity, or human-animal bond based on relationship duration or animal life stage. These findings indicate that the duration of the relationship with one\'s pet and animal life stage have implications for how much people feel a pet helps them grow, whereas other markers of human-pet relationship quality likely vary based on other experiences.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    几种类型的富集可用于改善动物福利。这篇综述总结了有关机械刷使用的文献,触觉乳房刺激,音乐,和视觉刺激作为奶牛的富集方法。机械刷和乳房的触觉刺激已被证明对牛奶产量和整体行为库具有积极影响,增强自然行为。古典音乐降低了压力水平,并且同样与牛奶产量增加有关。建议在低于70dB的频率下缓慢或中等速度(70至100bpm)具有这种积极作用。关于其他类型浓缩影响的证据,比如通过镜子的视觉刺激,图片,和彩色灯,或者使用嗅觉刺激,是模棱两可的,需要进一步研究。
    Several types of enrichment can be used to improve animal welfare. This review summarizes the literature on the use of mechanical brushes, tactile udder stimulation, music, and visual stimuli as enrichment methods for dairy cows. Mechanical brushes and tactile stimulation of the udder have been shown to have a positive effect on milk yield and overall behavioral repertoire, enhancing natural behavior. Classical music reduces stress levels and has similarly been associated with increased milk yield. A slow or moderate tempo (70 to 100 bpm) at frequencies below 70 dB is recommended to have this positive effect. Evidence on the impacts of other types of enrichment, such as visual stimulation through mirrors, pictures, and color lights, or the use of olfactory stimuli, is equivocal and requires further study.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    被诊断患有创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)的退伍军人的伴侣面临各种挑战的风险,尚不清楚精神科服务犬是否会破坏其睡眠或提供类似的益处,这在有限的退伍军人文献中可以看到。作为一项更大的临床试验的一部分,检查精神科服务犬对患有PTSD的退伍军人及其家人的功效,这项研究的重点是退伍军人伴侣的睡眠模式(n=88),纳入主观(临床验证的自我报告调查)和客观睡眠测量(活动记录)。在随访时,使用线性回归分析与组(干预与对照组)相关的差异,控制基线评分。结果显示,主观调查(p=0.15;p=0.75)和客观活动记录(p=0.06-0.98)在组间没有显着差异。这表明精神科服务犬没有破坏性,它们也没有为伴侣睡眠提供任何好处。合作伙伴的睡眠模式与基线时的国家规范相当,并在后续行动中保持在这样的水平。最终,使用主观和客观的措施,我们没有发现精神科服务犬对退伍军人伴侣睡眠的影响。
    Partners of veterans diagnosed with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are at risk of a variety of challenges, and it is unknown whether psychiatric service dogs are disruptive to their sleep or provide similar benefits that are seen in the limited literature on veterans. As part of a larger clinical trial examining the efficacy of psychiatric service dogs for veterans with PTSD and their families, this study focused on sleep patterns of veterans\' partners (n = 88), incorporating both subjective (clinically validated self-report surveys) and objective sleep measures (actigraphy). Linear regression was used to analyze differences in relation to group (intervention versus control) at follow up, controlling for baseline score. Results revealed no significant differences between groups for both the subjective surveys (p = 0.15; p = 0.75) and the objective actigraphy measures (p = 0.06-0.98). This suggests that psychiatric service dogs are not disruptive, nor do they provide any benefits to partner sleep. Partners had sleep patterns on par with national norms at baseline and remained at such levels at follow up. Ultimately, using both subjective and objective measures, we found no impact of psychiatric service dogs on the sleep of veterans\' partners.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    近几十年来,基于自然的解决方案(NBS)已经在科学研究中传播开来,它们越来越多地部署在城市战略规划中。虽然城市中非人类动物的数量在增长,仍然缺乏对作为潜在NBS的人-动物相互作用的优势的具体反映。本文旨在概述城市中动物的现状,并探讨在这种情况下动物的作用及其与人类的相互作用。这些主题对于卢卡(意大利)的欧洲项目IN-HABIT至关重要,旨在编纂关于人与动物之间关系的综合政策;其产出将在其他城市转移和复制。本文最后强调了不同利益相关者参与公私伙伴关系的必要性,以实施旨在评估人与动物关系及其积极影响的行动。这项研究提出了一种观点,即动物NBS与提高城市生活质量的相关性,对于市民和生活在城市的动物来说,并引入制定综合动物城市政策的机会,以提高城市中人与动物的互动。
    In recent decades, nature-based solutions (NBSs) have spread in scientific research, and they are increasingly deployed in cities\' strategic planning. While the number of nonhuman animals in cities is growing, a specific reflection on the advantages of human-animal interactions as potential NBSs is still lacking. This article aims to provide an overview of the current situation of animals in cities and to explore the roles of animals and their interactions with humans in such a context. These topics are crucial to the European project IN-HABIT in Lucca (Italy), which aims to codify an integrated policy on the relationship between people and animals; its outputs will then be transferred and replicated in other cities. This article concludes by highlighting the need for the involvement of different stakeholders in public-private-people partnerships to implement actions that aim to valorize human-animal relationships and their positive effects. This study presents a perspective on the relevance of animal NBSs to increase the quality of life in cities, both for citizens and for animals living in cities, and to also introduce the opportunity to develop an integrated animal urban policy able to valorize human-animal interactions in cities.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    狗与人类的行为同步越来越具有科学兴趣。然而,研究缺乏对这种社会认知能力的神经认知基础的全面探索。从人类行为同步的潜在机制中得出相似之处,特别是运动共振和镜像神经元的募集,我们假设狗与人类的行为同步是由类似的机制支撑的,即种间运动共振。在文献综述的基础上,我们认为狗具有运动共振的先决条件,我们认为种间行为同步依赖于人类和犬类镜像神经元的激活。此外,种间行为研究强调了运动共振的某些特征,包括运动传染及其社会调节因素。虽然这些发现强烈表明了种间运动共振的潜在存在,直接证明尚待建立。因此,我们的分析为未来的研究铺平了道路,以确认种间运动共振的存在是种间行为同步的神经认知基础。解开这种行为调整背后的神经认知机制对于理解狗与人类的进化动力学以及改善狗与人之间相互作用的日常管理具有深远的意义。
    Dogs\' behavioural synchronisation with humans is of growing scientific interest. However, studies lack a comprehensive exploration of the neurocognitive foundations of this social cognitive ability. Drawing parallels from the mechanisms underlying behavioural synchronisation in humans, specifically motor resonance and the recruitment of mirror neurons, we hypothesise that dogs\' behavioural synchronisation with humans is underpinned by a similar mechanism, namely interspecific motor resonance. Based on a literature review, we argue that dogs possess the prerequisites for motor resonance, and we suggest that interspecific behavioural synchronisation relies on the activation of both human and canine mirror neurons. Furthermore, interspecific behavioural studies highlight certain characteristics of motor resonance, including motor contagion and its social modulators. While these findings strongly suggest the potential existence of interspecific motor resonance, direct proof remains to be established. Our analysis thus paves the way for future research to confirm the existence of interspecific motor resonance as the neurocognitive foundation for interspecific behavioural synchronisation. Unravelling the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying this behavioural adjustment holds profound implications for understanding the evolutionary dynamics of dogs alongside humans and improving the day-to-day management of dog-human interactions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号