framing

成帧
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:招募临床试验参与者面临挑战。参与的主要障碍包括心理因素(例如,恐惧和不信任)和后勤限制(例如,交通运输,成本,和调度)。临床试验信息传递的战略设计可以帮助克服这些障碍。虽然战略沟通可以通过各种渠道进行(例如,招聘广告),在社交媒体时代,互联网上的医疗保健提供者被认为是向美国成年人传达临床试验信息的关键来源。
    目的:本研究旨在研究沟通来源如何(即,医生和同行)和TikTok视频的消息框架(即,心理和后勤框架)影响临床试验相关的态度,感知,以及在综合行为模型指导下的报名行为。
    方法:这项研究使用了2(来源:医生与同伴)×2(框架:心理与后勤)参与者之间的因素设计网络实验,针对美国从未参加过的成年人临床试验(即,新人)。Qualtrics小组用于招募和补偿研究受访者(n=561)。参与者观看了与医生或同龄人的简短视频,使用心理或后勤框架。主要结果指标包括感知来源可信度,自我效能感,对临床试验参与的态度,行为意图,和注册行为。结构方程模型用于分析信息因素对结果变量的直接和间接影响。来源(医生=1;同伴=0)和框架(心理=1;后勤=0)被虚拟编码。
    结果:以医生为特征的消息导致了更大的感知来源可信度(β=.31,P<.001),导致更大的自我效能感(95%CI0.13-0.30),这反过来又增强了行为意向(95%CI0.12-0.29)和临床试验签署行为(95%CI0.02-0.04)。后勤屏障框架信息导致更大的自我效能感(β=-.09,P=.02),导致更高的参与临床试验的意向(95%CI-0.38至-0.03)和改善的签署行为(95%CI-0.06至-0.004)。后勤屏障框架信息也与临床试验的可能性增加直接相关(β=-.08,P=.03)。该模型占临床试验签约行为方差的21%。在这项研究中,态度没有显著影响行为意向(β=.08,P=.14),在参与临床试验的态度上,心理和后勤屏障框架信息没有显着差异(β=-.04,P=.09)。
    结论:这些发现促进了我们对人们如何处理短视频中流行信息特征的理解,并为传播者提供了实用的指导。我们鼓励医疗专业人员考虑短视频网站(例如,TikTok和Instagram卷轴)作为讨论临床试验和参与机会的有效工具。具体来说,建议让医生讨论减少后勤障碍的努力。我们对实际行为作为结果的测量是对这项研究的罕见且值得注意的贡献。
    BACKGROUND: Recruiting participants for clinical trials poses challenges. Major barriers to participation include psychological factors (eg, fear and mistrust) and logistical constraints (eg, transportation, cost, and scheduling). The strategic design of clinical trial messaging can help overcome these barriers. While strategic communication can be done through various channels (eg, recruitment advertisements), health care providers on the internet have been found to be key sources for communicating clinical trial information to US adults in the social media era.
    OBJECTIVE: This study aims to examine how communication source (ie, medical doctors and peers) and message framing of TikTok videos (ie, psychological and logistical framing) influence clinical trial-related attitudes, perceptions, and sign-up behaviors under the guidance of the integrated behavioral model.
    METHODS: This study used a 2 (source: doctor vs peer) × 2 (framing: psychological vs logistical) between-participant factorial design web-based experiment targeting adults in the United States who had never participated in clinical trials (ie, newcomers). A Qualtrics panel was used to recruit and compensate the study respondents (n=561). Participants viewed short-form videos with doctors or peers, using psychological or logistical framing. The main outcome measures included perceived source credibility, self-efficacy, attitude toward clinical trial participation, behavioral intention, and sign-up behavior. Structural equation modeling was used to analyze the direct and indirect effects of message factors on the outcome variables. Source (doctor=1; peer=0) and framing (psychological=1; logistical=0) were dummy-coded.
    RESULTS: Doctor-featured messages led to greater perceived source credibility (β=.31, P<.001), leading to greater self-efficacy (95% CI 0.13-0.30), which in turn enhanced behavioral intention (95% CI 0.12-0.29) and clinical trial sign-up behavior (95% CI 0.02-0.04). Logistical barrier-framed messages led to greater self-efficacy (β=-.09, P=.02), resulting in higher intention to participate in clinical trials (95% CI -0.38 to -0.03) and improved sign-up behavior (95% CI -0.06 to -0.004). Logistical barrier-framed messages were also directly associated with an increased likelihood of signing up for a clinical trial (β=-.08, P=.03). The model accounted for 21% of the variance in clinical trial sign-up behavior. Attitude did not significantly affect behavioral intention in this study (β=.08, P=.14), and psychological and logistical barrier-framed messages did not significantly differ in attitudes toward clinical trial participation (β=-.04, P=.09).
    CONCLUSIONS: These findings advance our understanding of how people process popular message characteristics in short-form videos and lend practical guidance for communicators. We encourage medical professionals to consider short-form video sites (eg, TikTok and Instagram Reels) as effective tools for discussing clinical trials and participation opportunities. Specifically, featuring doctors discussing efforts to reduce logistical barriers is recommended. Our measuring of actual behavior as an outcome is a rare and noteworthy contribution to this research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    预测和估计有时被限定为确定。这个认知标记在言语概率表达的尺度上占据了优越的位置,反映概率接近1。但是这样的陈述很少被比作简单的,没有提到确定性的无保留意见。我们在9项研究(N=2784)中检查了明确声称是确定的陈述是否被认为是(1)更(或更少)可信,(2)更(或更少)精确,和(3)更多(或更少)强烈基于证据,与平原相比,无标记的声明性声明。我们发现,与标准量表的假设形成明显对比,“确定”通常被认为不太值得信赖,不太可靠,并且持有的信心低于未标记的声明。平原,声明性语句被进一步假定为更精确,而确定性意味着更极端的结果是可能的。当确定亨利犯了四个错误时,很明显,他犯了不少于四个,但他可能犯了五个或更多的错误。因此,确定性可以指示上界未定义的区间的下界,因此,确定性陈述比没有提到确定性的估计更加模糊。至少确定性的解释会影响风险选择问题中期权的解释,其中“200条生命将被拯救”被大多数人认为正好是200条,而“可以肯定的是200条生命将被拯救”,可能意味着200-600条生命。我们还发现,可信度受确定性类型的影响,具有非个人确定性(“它是确定的”),被认为比个人确定性(“我是确定的”)更准确和有说服力,尤其是对未来事件的预测。此外,提及确定性可以表明,说话者的估计是基于主观判断和猜测,而不是客观证据。这些发现对沟通有影响。当省略确定性主张时,估计可能会显得更加合意。为了传达确定性,最好不要提及一个人是确定的。
    Predictions and estimates are sometimes qualified as certain. This epistemic marker occupies a privileged position at the top of scales of verbal probability expressions, reflecting probabilities close to 1. But such statements have rarely been compared to plain, unqualified statements in which certainty is not mentioned. We examined in nine studies (N = 2784) whether statements explicitly claimed to be certain are perceived as (1) more (or less) credible, (2) more (or less) precise, and (3) more (or less) strongly based upon evidence, compared to plain, unmarked declarative statements. We find, in apparent contrast with assumptions made by the standard scales, that \"certain\" are often judged to be less trustworthy, less reliable, and held with lower confidence than unmarked statements. Plain, declarative statements are further assumed to be more precise, while certainty implies that more extreme outcomes are possible. When it is certain that Henry made four errors, it is clear he did not commit less than four, but he might have committed five errors or more. Thus certainty can indicate lower bounds of an interval whose upper bounds are not defined, and certainty statements are consequently more ambiguous than estimates that do not mention certainty. At least-interpretations of certainty affect the interpretation of options in risky choice problems, where \"200 lives will be saved\" was deemed by a majority to mean exactly 200, while \"it is certain that 200 will be saved\", could mean 200-600 lives. We also find that credibility is affected by type of certainty, with impersonal certainty (\"it is certain\") perceived to be more accurate and persuasive than personal certainty (\"I am certain\"), especially in predictions of future events. Moreover, mentions of certainty can reveal that that a speaker\'s estimate is based on subjective judgments and guesswork rather than upon objective evidence. These findings have implications for communication. Estimates can appear more consensual when claims of certainty are omitted. To convey certainty it may be better not to mention that one is certain.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    不平等往往被片面地认为是不利的,一种被实证研究和批判性奖学金认定为有问题的做法,因为它使特权不可见,塑造了对不平等的看法和反应。重要的是,不平等可以意味着积极的差异(例如,促销)或负面结果(例如,骚扰)。借鉴(组)比较中涉及的认知过程以及正面和负面内容的处理,我们预测,结果的效价缓和了对劣势的偏好(与优势)框架。我们对有关性别不平等的社交媒体帖子进行了编码(研究1,n=1,402),并让参与者参与了一项在线实验(研究2,n=164),以积极和消极的结果描述了性别和性取向不平等。确认假设,人们整体使用缺点帧更多,但更有可能在积极(与消极相比)的结果中使用优势框架来解决不平等。我们讨论了对不平等框架研究的理论意义以及对特权意识干预的实际意义。
    Inequality is often one-sidedly framed as disadvantage, a practice identified as problematic by empirical research and critical scholarship, as it renders privilege invisible and shapes perceptions of and reactions to inequality. Importantly, inequality can mean differences in positive (e.g., promotions) or negative outcomes (e.g., harassment). Drawing on cognitive processes involved in (group) comparisons and the processing of positive and negative content, we predict that the valence of outcomes moderates the preference for disadvantage (vs. advantage) frames. We coded social media posts on gender inequality (Study 1, n = 1,402) and had participants in an online experiment (Study 2, n = 164) describe gender and sexual orientation inequality in positive and negative outcomes. Confirming hypotheses, people overall used disadvantage frames more, but were more likely to use advantage frames for inequality in positive (compared with negative) outcomes. We discuss theoretical implications for inequality framing research and practical implications for privilege awareness interventions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    推动可以是促进疫苗接种的有效策略。然而,有必要更好地识别产生最强影响的轻推的特征,以及它们如何与个人的态度互动。在这里,我们依次测试三个微推特征的有效性(框架,轻推类型,和呈现方式)以及参与者对新冠肺炎疫苗接种的态度的作用,疫苗接种决策中的社会团结和威权主义。在研究1-4中,参与者被提示操纵目标特征(例如,正/负框架,轻推类型)和测量疫苗接种意愿和相关变量与对照轻推进行了比较。研究5使用了单个组合的轻推,反映了先前研究中成功轻推的组合。所有研究的结果表明,轻推具有不可靠的效果,而疫苗态度与所有疫苗意愿指标更可靠地联系在一起。这些结果表明,态度在有效采用疫苗接种中起着更可靠的作用。
    Nudges can be an effective strategy to promote vaccination. However, it is necessary to better identify the characteristics of nudges that produce the strongest effects and how they interact with individuals\' attitudes. Here we sequentially test the effectiveness of three nudge characteristics (framing, nudge type, and presentation modality) and the role of participants\' attitudes toward Covid-19 vaccination, social solidarity and authoritarianism in vaccination decisions. In studies 1-4, participants were presented with a nudge manipulating a target characteristic (e.g. positive/negative framing, nudge type) and measuring willingness to vaccinate and related variables compared a control nudge. Study 5 used a single combined nudge reflecting the combination of successful nudges in previous studies. Results over all studies show that nudging has unreliable effects while vaccine attitudes are more reliably linked to all measures of vaccines willingness. These results suggest that attitudes play a more reliable role on effective adoption of vaccinations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:国家食品环境政策有助于减少与饮食相关的非传染性疾病。然而,它们在荷兰的执行仍然很低。据推测,媒体可以在引发政策关注度激增方面发挥关键作用,从而塑造政治行动。这项研究的目的是通过分析各种行为者使用的论点,研究2000-2022年间荷兰报纸文章中关于食品政策的论述。
    方法:在NexisUni中进行了系统搜索,以识别2000-2022年间在七家荷兰国家报纸上发表的涵盖国家级荷兰食品环境政策的报纸文章。承保政策分为六个领域,包括食品成分,标签,促销,价格,提供和零售,并进入政策周期的四个阶段;政策制定,决策,实施,和评价。灰色文献检索用于确定2000-2022年期间实施的食品政策。描述性统计数据被用来总结政策随着时间的推移,政策类型和政策阶段。对报纸文章的随机子样本进行了解释性内容分析,以确定演员,粮食政策的观点和论点。
    结果:我们确定了896篇相关报纸文章。报纸对食品政策的报道最初很低,但在2018/2021/2022年达到顶峰。通过灰色文献检索,我们确定了6项2000-2022年实施或调整的食品政策。大多数报纸文章都报道了食品定价政策,并在政策制定阶段进行了讨论。学者(主要是支持)是最多的,食品行业(主要是反对)是引用最少的参与者。支持性论点强调了健康后果,健康不平等和集体责任,而相反的论点集中在不必要的政府干预和政策无效上。
    结论:涉及食品政策的荷兰报纸文章代表了各种参与者和论点,个人对食物选择的责任和集体责任在争论中起着核心作用。这些见解可以作为进一步研究为什么使用某些论点及其对政策关注和执行的影响的基础。
    BACKGROUND: National food environment policies can contribute to the reduction of diet-related non-communicable diseases. Yet, their implementation in the Netherlands remains low. It has been hypothesized that the media can play a pivotal role in inducing spikes in policy attention, thereby shaping political action. The aim of this study was to examine the discourse on food policies in Dutch newspaper articles between 2000-2022, by analyzing arguments used by various actors.
    METHODS: A systematic search in Nexis Uni was used to identify newspaper articles that covered national-level Dutch food environment policies published in seven Dutch national newspapers between 2000-2022. Covered policies were classified into six domains including food composition, labeling, promotion, prices, provision and retail and into the four stages of the policy cycle; policy formulation, decision-making, implementation, and evaluation. A grey literature search was used to identify food policies implemented during 2000-2022. Descriptive statistics were used to summarize coverage of policies over time, policy type and policy stage. An interpretive content analysis was performed on a random subsample of the newspaper articles to determine the actors, viewpoints and arguments of the food policies.
    RESULTS: We identified 896 relevant newspaper articles. The coverage of food policies in newspapers was initially low but peaked in 2018/2021/2022. Through grey literature search we identified 6 food policies which were implemented or adjusted between 2000-2022. The majority of the newspaper articles reported on food pricing policies and were discussed in the policy formulation stage. Academics (mainly supportive) were the most and food industry (mostly opposing) the least cited actors. Supportive arguments highlighted health consequences, health inequalities and collective responsibility, whereas opposing arguments focused on unwanted governmental interference and ineffectiveness of policies.
    CONCLUSIONS: Dutch newspaper articles covering food policies represented a variety of actors and arguments, with individual versus collective responsibility for food choices playing a central role in the arguments. These insights may serve as a basis for further research into why certain arguments are used and their effect on policy attention and implementation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在青少年心理健康危机中,负面叙事传播具有统一的后果。支持性沟通涉及重新组织沟通,以找到不引起偏见的新叙述。这种叙述必须强调代理并突出优势,潜力,和年轻人的共同经历。很明显,支持积极的发展和福祉是“我们”的努力。在这种传播策略中,儿科专业人员可以解决年轻人的问题,看护者,其他医疗保健专业人员,和社区。框架科学帮助我们塑造一种更具支持性和生产力的话语。
    In the adolescent mental health crisis, negative narrative communication has unitended consequences. Supportive communication involves reframing communications to find a new narrative that does not evoke biases. This narrative must emphasize agency and highlight the strengths, potential, and common experiences of young people. It is clear that supporting positive development and well-being is an \"us\" endeavor. There is a place in this communication strategy for pediatric professionals to address young people, caregivers, other health care professionals, and the community. The science of framing helps us shape a more supportive and productive discourse.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    机器人技术可能会导致农业发生根本性的变化。但是公众对农业机器人的了解和看法是什么?最近对其他农业技术的经验,例如植物基因工程,表明公众的看法可以影响创新的速度和方向,所以理解感知和它们是如何形成的是很重要的。这里,我们使用来自在线调查(n=2269)的代表性数据来分析公众对德国作物种植机器人的态度-这个国家有时会怀疑新的农业技术。虽然不到一半的调查参与者知道机器人在农业中的使用,一般态度大多是积极的,兴趣水平很高。框架实验表明,所提供的信息类型会影响态度。有关可能的环境效益的信息比有关可能的粮食安全和劳动力市场影响的信息更能增加积极的看法。这些见解可以帮助设计沟通策略,以促进农业中的技术接受和可持续创新。
    Robot technologies could lead to radical changes in farming. But what does the public know and think about agricultural robots? Recent experience with other agricultural technologies-such as plant genetic engineering-shows that public perceptions can influence the pace and direction of innovation, so understanding perceptions and how they are formed is important. Here, we use representative data from an online survey (n = 2269) to analyze public attitudes towards crop farming robots in Germany-a country where new farming technologies are sometimes seen with skepticism. While less than half of the survey participants are aware of the use of robots in agriculture, general attitudes are mostly positive and the level of interest is high. A framing experiment suggests that the type of information provided influences attitudes. Information about possible environmental benefits increases positive perceptions more than information about possible food security and labor market effects. These insights can help design communication strategies to promote technology acceptance and sustainable innovation in agriculture.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:自杀是全球死亡的主要原因。新闻报道准则旨在遏制不安全报道的影响;然而,在新闻报道中自杀的框架可能因情况和死者的性别等重要特征而有所不同。
    目的:本研究旨在研究新闻媒体对自杀报道使用污名化或荣耀化的语言进行陷害的程度,以及性别和自杀情况在这种陷害方面的差异。
    方法:我们分析了200篇有关自杀的新闻文章,并应用经过验证的自杀污名量表来识别污名化和荣耀化的语言。我们用2个广泛使用的指标来评估语言相似性,余弦相似性和互信息得分,使用基于机器学习的大型语言模型。
    结果:男性自杀的新闻报道比女性自杀的报道更类似于污名化(P<.001)和美化(P=.005)语言。考虑到自杀的情况,互信息得分表明,在使用污名化或美化语言的性别差异最明显的文章归因于法律(0.155),关系(0.268),或心理健康问题(0.251)为原因。
    结论:语言差异,按性别,在报告自杀时使用污名化或美化语言可能会加剧自杀差异。
    BACKGROUND: Suicide is a leading cause of death worldwide. Journalistic reporting guidelines were created to curb the impact of unsafe reporting; however, how suicide is framed in news reports may differ by important characteristics such as the circumstances and the decedent\'s gender.
    OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to examine the degree to which news media reports of suicides are framed using stigmatized or glorified language and differences in such framing by gender and circumstance of suicide.
    METHODS: We analyzed 200 news articles regarding suicides and applied the validated Stigma of Suicide Scale to identify stigmatized and glorified language. We assessed linguistic similarity with 2 widely used metrics, cosine similarity and mutual information scores, using a machine learning-based large language model.
    RESULTS: News reports of male suicides were framed more similarly to stigmatizing (P<.001) and glorifying (P=.005) language than reports of female suicides. Considering the circumstances of suicide, mutual information scores indicated that differences in the use of stigmatizing or glorifying language by gender were most pronounced for articles attributing legal (0.155), relationship (0.268), or mental health problems (0.251) as the cause.
    CONCLUSIONS: Linguistic differences, by gender, in stigmatizing or glorifying language when reporting suicide may exacerbate suicide disparities.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    引起同情,并向决策者和政府传达紧迫性,研究人员和项目开发人员提出了一种脆弱性和风险的叙述,以描述父母被诊断患有精神疾病时家庭的经历。在西方医学社会文化背景下发展起来的,这一框架将重点放在预防和早期干预服务响应的必要性上,同时也无意中“打扰”这些家庭并将“问题”个性化。这个框架产生了一些意想不到的后果,通过赤字饱和的镜头看到这些家庭,错过了力量,将家庭成员的结果彼此分开。本文提出了有关此框架的持续适合性的问题,并建议需要重新构想一个新的框架。
    To elicit compassion and communicate urgency to policy makers and governments, researchers and program developers have promoted a narrative of vulnerability and risk to frame the experience of families when parents have been diagnosed with mental illness. Developed within a western medicalised socio-cultural context, this frame has provided a focus on the need for prevention and early intervention in service responses while also unintentionally \'othering\' these families and individualizing the \'problem\'. This frame has had some unintended consequences of seeing these families through a deficit-saturated lens that misses strengths and separates family members\' outcomes from each other. This paper raises questions about the continued fit of this frame and suggests a need to reimagine a new one.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号