face age

面部年龄
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人们可以代表时间刺激(例如,描绘过去和未来事件的图片)作为沿三个主轴(水平,矢状,和垂直)。例如,过去和未来的事件通常被代表,从个人的角度来看,被放在他们的后面和前面,分别。这里,我们报告说,这种3D表示也可以出现在不同年龄的面部刺激。在三个实验中,参与者对中心目标面部进行了分类,代表一个处于不同年龄阶段的人,比40岁的参考面孔年轻或年长。手动响应提供了两个沿水平轴放置的键(实验1),矢状轴(实验2),和垂直轴(实验3)。结果表明,年轻的面孔代表在空间的左/后/顶侧,而较旧的面孔代表在空间的右/前/底侧。此外,在所有的实验中,延迟随着目标人脸和参考人脸年龄之间的绝对差异而减少(即,距离效应)。总的来说,这项工作表明,时间的空间表示包括人脸的社会特征。
    People can represent temporal stimuli (e.g., pictures depicting past and future events) as spatially connoted dimensions arranged along the three main axes (horizontal, sagittal, and vertical). For example, past and future events are generally represented, from the perspective of the individuals, as being placed behind and in front of them, respectively. Here, we report that such a 3D representation can also emerge for facial stimuli of different ages. In three experiments, participants classified a central target face, representing an individual at different age stages, as younger or older than the reference face of 40 years. Manual responses were provided with two keys placed along the horizontal axis (Experiment 1), the sagittal axis (Experiment 2), and the vertical axis (Experiment 3). The results indicated that the younger faces were represented on the left/back/top side of the space, whereas the older faces were represented on the right/forward/bottom side of the space. Furthermore, in all experiments, the latencies decreased with the absolute difference between the age of the target face and that of the reference face (i.e., a distance effect). Overall, this work suggests that the spatial representation of time includes social features of the human face.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在生命的第一年结束时,婴儿的辨别能力调谐到经常经历的面部群体。对有效区分频繁采用的探索策略知之甚少,熟悉的面孔类型。本眼动追踪研究检查了10个月大和4个月大的单身人士在学习成人时产生的视觉注视的分布(即,熟悉)和孩子(即,不熟悉)白脸。在婴儿控制的视觉习惯任务中对婴儿进行了测试,其中习惯后偏好衡量成功的歧视。结果证实了早期的证据,没有兄弟姐妹的经验,10个月的孩子只歧视成人面孔。习惯过程中凝视运动的分析表明,婴儿的注视集中在刺激的上部。成人面部的嘴巴采样时间比儿童面部长,而儿童眼睛的采样时间比成人眼睛更长,更频繁。10个月时,但不是在4个月的时候,整个面部扫描行为的全球测量也根据面部年龄而变化,因为扫描路径的时空分布显示,成人面孔与儿童面孔的参与者之间和参与者之间的相似性更大。参考感性缩小文献讨论结果,以及适合年龄的发育任务对婴儿面部处理能力的影响。
    By the end of the first year of life, infants\' discrimination abilities tune to frequently experienced face groups. Little is known about the exploration strategies adopted to efficiently discriminate frequent, familiar face types. The present eye-tracking study examined the distribution of visual fixations produced by 10-month-old and 4-month-old singletons while learning adult (i.e., familiar) and child (i.e., unfamiliar) White faces. Infants were tested in an infant-controlled visual habituation task, in which post-habituation preference measured successful discrimination. Results confirmed earlier evidence that, without sibling experience, 10-month-olds discriminate only among adult faces. Analyses of gaze movements during habituation showed that infants\' fixations were centered in the upper part of the stimuli. The mouth was sampled longer in adult faces than in child faces, while the child eyes were sampled longer and more frequently than the adult eyes. At 10 months, but not at 4 months, global measures of scanning behavior on the whole face also varied according to face age, as the spatiotemporal distribution of scan paths showed larger within- and between-participants similarity for adult faces than for child faces. Results are discussed with reference to the perceptual narrowing literature, and the influence of age-appropriate developmental tasks on infants\' face processing abilities.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    了解人类如何将儿童与成人区分开来,在法医和认知心理学的许多领域都具有有用的意义。然而,在这两个学科中,我们如何从面孔中提取年龄的研究令人惊讶地不足。这里,我们使用一种新颖的数据驱动的实验技术来客观地测量人类观察者用来对儿童和成人面部进行分类的面部特征。依靠35000多次试验,我们使用了一种反向相关技术,使我们能够揭示已知在面部感知位置中重要的特定特征,空间频率(SF),和取向-与准确的儿童和成人歧视有关。这表明人类观察者依靠鼻骨和眉毛区域的证据来进行准确的成人分类,而他们依靠眼睛和下巴区域来准确地对儿童面孔进行分类。对于定向结构,只有垂直方向的面部信息与面部成人分类相关,而水平和的特征,在较小程度上倾斜的方向,更多的是儿童脸的诊断。最后,我们发现SF诊断显示出面部年龄分类的U形模式,低SF和高SF中的信息可以诊断儿童面孔,和中期SFs诊断成人面孔。通过面部分类的面部特征的首次表征,我们表明,在一般的面部感知的心理物理学研究中发现的重要信息(即,眼睛区域,水平线,和中级SFs)对于面部年龄分类的实际背景至关重要,并提出了数据驱动的程序,通过这些程序可以针对现实世界的挑战实施面部年龄分类培训。
    Knowing how humans differentiate children from adults has useful implications in many areas of both forensic and cognitive psychology. Yet, how we extract age from faces has been surprisingly underexplored in both disciplines. Here, we used a novel data-driven experimental technique to objectively measure the facial features human observers use to categorise child and adult faces. Relying on more than 35,000 trials, we used a reverse correlation technique that enabled us to reveal how specific features which are known to be important in face-perception - position, spatial-frequency (SF), and orientation - are associated with accurate child and adult discrimination. This showed that human observers relied on evidence in the nasal bone and eyebrow area for accurate adult categorisation, while they relied on the eye and jawline area to accurately categorise child faces. For orientation structure, only facial information of vertical orientation was linked to face-adult categorisation, while features of horizontal and, to a lesser extent oblique orientations, were more diagnostic of a child face. Finally, we found that SF diagnosticity showed a U-shaped pattern for face-age categorisation, with information in low and high SFs being diagnostic of child faces, and mid SFs being diagnostic of adult faces. Through this first characterisation of the facial features of face-age categorisation, we show that important information found in psychophysical studies of face-perception in general (i.e., the eye area, horizontals, and mid-level SFs) is crucial to the practical context of face-age categorisation, and present data-driven procedures through which face-age classification training could be implemented for real-world challenges.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Inattentional blindness (IB) refers to the nature of an individual being unaware of an unexpected stimulus when focusing on an attentional task. Investigation into IB provides an innovative approach for the research of attentional bias that is connected with an individual\'s immediate attentional capture. This study explored the effect of emotional valence and age of faces on the IB rates of children and adults. We employed a between-subject experimental design with a 2 (age of participants: child, adult) × 2 (age of face: child face, adult face) × 2 (emotional valence: positive, negative) method and used the classic IB task of cross judgment. The participants included 79 adults (54.63 ± 8.192 years old) and 78 children (10.62 ± 2.32 years old). The results showed that: (1) The IB rates of the two groups differed significantly. Children performed better than adults in detecting the faces appearing unexpectedly. (2) The effect of emotional valence only emerged on adult participants\' IB. Adults were significantly more likely to detect the positive-emotional faces than the negative-emotional ones. (3) The age of a face was not a significant factor to predict participants\' IB. Neither children nor adults showed a preference for the faces of a specific age (e.g., their own age). These findings revealed the distinct characteristics of attentional capture of children and adults when confronting unexpected facial stimuli.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Multiple facial cues such as facial expression and face gender simultaneously influence facial trustworthiness judgement in adults. The current work was to examine the effect of multiple facial cues on trustworthiness judgement across age groups. Eight-, 10-year-olds, and adults detect trustworthiness from happy and neutral adult faces (female and male faces) in Experiment 1. Experiment 2 included both adult and child faces wearing happy, angry, and neutral expressions. Nine-, 11-, 13-year-olds, and adults had to rate facial trustworthiness with a 7-point Likert scale. The results of Experiments 1 and 2 revealed that facial expression and face gender independently affected facial trustworthiness judgement in children aged 10 and below but simultaneously affected judgement in children aged 11 and above, adolescents, and adults. There was no own-age bias in children and adults. The results showed that children younger than 10 could not process multiple facial cues in the same manner as in older children and adults when judging trustworthiness. The current findings provide evidence for the stable-feature account, but not for the own-age bias account or the expertise account.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    面部吸引力会影响以前见过的面部记忆。对于年轻人和老年人,这种效果已被证明是不同的。两项平行研究检查了面部年龄和感知者年龄对面部吸引力与面部记忆之间关系的调节作用。研究1包括29名年轻参与者和31名年长参与者;研究2包括25名年轻参与者和24名年长参与者。在两项研究中,参与者完成了附带的面部编码和令人惊讶的新旧识别测试,这些测试具有不同的面部吸引力。面部吸引力影响了年轻而不是年长的面部的记忆。此外,年轻而不是年长的感知者表现出面部吸引力对年轻面孔记忆的线性影响,而年轻和年长的感知者对年轻面孔的记忆力都表现出二次效应。这些发现通过证明面部吸引力对面部记忆的影响是感知者年龄和面部年龄的函数,从而扩展了先前的工作。讨论了可以解释面部吸引力和面部记忆之间关联的这种适度和感知的因素(例如,社会目标和面部相似性/独特性的年龄差异)。
    Face attractiveness can influence memory for previously seen faces. This effect has been shown to differ for young and older perceivers. Two parallel studies examined the moderation of both the age of the face and the age of the perceiver on the relationship between facial attractiveness and face memory. Study 1 comprised 29 young and 31 older participants; Study 2 comprised 25 young and 24 older participants. In both studies, participants completed an incidental face encoding and a surprise old/new recognition test with young and older faces that varied in face attractiveness. Face attractiveness affected memory for young but not older faces. In addition, young but not older perceivers showed a linear effect of facial attractiveness on memory for young faces, while both young and older perceivers showed a quadratic effect on memory for young faces. These findings extend previous work by demonstrating that the effect of facial attractiveness on face memory is a function of both the age of the perceiver and the age of the face. Factors that could account for such moderations of face and perceiver age on the associations between face attractiveness and face memory are discussed (e.g. age differences in social goals and face similarity/distinctiveness).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Adults\' face processing may be specialized for the dimensions of young adult faces. For example, young and older adults exhibit increased accuracy in normality judgments and greater agreement in attractiveness ratings for young versus older adult faces. The present study was designed to examine whether there is a similar young adult face bias in facial age estimates. In Experiment 1, we created a face age continuum by morphing an averaged young adult face with an averaged older adult face in 5% increments, for a total of 21 faces ranging from 0 to 100% old. Young and older adults estimated facial age for three stimulus age categories [young (morphs 0-30%), middle-aged (morphs 35-65%), and older adult (morphs 70-100%)]. Both age groups showed the least differentiation in age estimates for young adult faces, despite showing greater consensus across participants in estimates for young faces. In Experiment 2, young and older adults made age estimates for individual young and older adult identities. Both age groups were more accurate and showed greater consensus in age estimates for young faces. Collectively, these results provide evidence for a bias in processing young adult faces beyond that which is often observed in recognition and normality/attractiveness judgment tasks.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们检查了9个月和12个月大的年龄不同的面孔的类别形成,以及暴露于婴儿面孔对这种能力的影响。婴儿熟悉成人或婴儿的面孔,然后用来自熟悉类别的新颖范例与来自新颖类别的新颖范例配对进行测试(实验1)。这两个年龄组都形成了成人和婴儿面孔的离散类别,但是在日常生活中接触婴儿的面孔并不能调节表现。对儿童和婴儿的面孔进行了相同的任务(实验2)。而9个月大的孩子在熟悉儿童面孔后更喜欢婴儿面孔,但不是熟悉婴儿面孔后的儿童面孔,12个月大的孩子形成了离散的儿童和婴儿面孔类别。此外,当婴儿熟悉12个月大婴儿的婴儿面孔时,更多的婴儿面孔暴露与更高的新类别偏好评分相关,但不是9个月大的孩子.9个月大的不对称性并不能反映出婴儿对儿童面孔的自发偏好(实验3)。这些发现表明,9个月和12个月的孩子可以形成基于年龄的面孔类别。12个月大的孩子能够形成单独的儿童和婴儿类别,这表明他们对面部年龄有更多的排他性代表,可能会受到先前婴儿面孔经验的影响。
    We examined category formation for faces differing in age in 9- and 12-month-olds, and the influence of exposure to infant faces on such ability. Infants were familiarized with adult or infant faces, and then tested with a novel exemplar from the familiarized category paired with a novel exemplar from a novel category (Experiment 1). Both age groups formed discrete categories of adult and infant faces, but exposure to infant faces in everyday life did not modulate performance. The same task was conducted with child versus infant faces (Experiment 2). Whereas 9-month-olds preferred infant faces after familiarization with child faces, but not child faces after familiarization with infant faces, 12-month-olds formed discrete categories of child and infant faces. Moreover, more exposure to infant faces correlated with higher novel category preference scores when infants were familiarized with infant faces in 12-month-olds, but not 9-month-olds. The 9-month-old asymmetry did not reflect spontaneous preference for infant over child faces (Experiment 3). These findings indicate that 9- and 12-month-olds can form age-based categories of faces. The ability of 12-month-olds to form separate child and infant categories suggests that they have a more exclusive representation of face age, one that may be influenced by prior experience with infant faces.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    It is well-established that our recognition ability is enhanced for faces belonging to familiar categories, such as own-race faces and own-age faces. Recent evidence suggests that, for race, the recognition bias is also accompanied by different visual scanning strategies for own- compared to other-race faces. Here, we tested the hypothesis that these differences in visual scanning patterns extend also to the comparison between own and other-age faces and contribute to the own-age recognition advantage. Participants (young adults with limited experience with infants) were tested in an old/new recognition memory task where they encoded and subsequently recognized a series of adult and infant faces while their eye movements were recorded. Consistent with findings on the other-race bias, we found evidence of an own-age bias in recognition which was accompanied by differential scanning patterns, and consequently differential encoding strategies, for own-compared to other-age faces. Gaze patterns for own-age faces involved a more dynamic sampling of the internal features and longer viewing time on the eye region compared to the other regions of the face. This latter strategy was extensively employed during learning (vs. recognition) and was positively correlated to discriminability. These results suggest that deeply encoding the eye region is functional for recognition and that the own-age bias is evident not only in differential recognition performance, but also in the employment of different sampling strategies found to be effective for accurate recognition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    The correct interpretation of emotional expressions is crucial for social life. However, emotions in old relative to young faces are recognized less well. One reason for this may be decreased signal clarity of older faces due to morphological changes, such as wrinkles and folds, obscuring facial displays of emotions. Across three experiments, the present research investigates how misattributions of emotions to elderly faces impair emotion discrimination. In a preliminary task, neutral expressions were perceived as more expressive in old than in young faces by human raters (Experiment 1A) and an automatic system for emotion recognition (Experiment 1B). Consequently, task difficulty was higher for old faces relative to young faces in a visual search task (Experiment 2). Specifically, participants detected old faces expressing negative emotions less accurately and slower among neutral faces of their peers than young faces among neutral faces of their peers. Thus, we argue that age-related changes in facial features are the most plausible explanation for the differences in emotion perception between young and old faces. These findings are of relevance for the social interchange with the elderly, especially when multiple older individuals are present.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号