auditory attention

听觉注意
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:功能连通性(FC)的度量可以阐明哪些皮质区域协同工作以完成各种行为任务。这项研究的主要目的是扩大以前发表的测量FC的模型,以包括多个受试者和几个感兴趣的区域。虽然FC在视觉和其他感觉运动任务中得到了更广泛的研究,这在试镜中没有得到很好的理解。这项研究的次要目的是研究当注意力针对不同的听觉刺激时,听觉区域如何在功能上与其他皮质区域相连。 方法。这项研究实现了一个线性动态系统(LDS)来测量多个皮层区域的结构化时滞依赖性,以便在双流听觉注意力任务中估计其FC。 结果。模型的输出显示了在不同的监听条件下功能上一致的连接区域,指示听觉注意力网络,该网络参与而不管注意力的内源性切换或被关注的不同听觉线索。 意义。本研究中实施的LDS实施了多变量自回归,以在听觉注意力任务期间推断皮层区域的FC。这项研究显示了一阶自回归函数如何可靠地从M/EEG数据中测量功能连通性。此外,该研究显示了听觉区域如何与视觉注意力文献中概述的超模式注意力网络互动。
    Objective.Measures of functional connectivity (FC) can elucidate which cortical regions work together in order to complete a variety of behavioral tasks. This study\'s primary objective was to expand a previously published model of measuring FC to include multiple subjects and several regions of interest. While FC has been more extensively investigated in vision and other sensorimotor tasks, it is not as well understood in audition. The secondary objective of this study was to investigate how auditory regions are functionally connected to other cortical regions when attention is directed to different distinct auditory stimuli.Approach.This study implements a linear dynamic system (LDS) to measure the structured time-lagged dependence across several cortical regions in order to estimate their FC during a dual-stream auditory attention task.Results.The model\'s output shows consistent functionally connected regions across different listening conditions, indicative of an auditory attention network that engages regardless of endogenous switching of attention or different auditory cues being attended.Significance.The LDS implemented in this study implements a multivariate autoregression to infer FC across cortical regions during an auditory attention task. This study shows how a first-order autoregressive function can reliably measure functional connectivity from M/EEG data. Additionally, the study shows how auditory regions engage with the supramodal attention network outlined in the visual attention literature.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人类的残余耳廓定向系统能够响应于侧向的瞬时听觉刺激而增加若干耳廓肌肉的活动。例如,已记录了耳后肌(PAM)肌电图活动对注意力捕获刺激的短暂增加。对于目前的研究,记录了10名听力正常的参与者的PAMs和上耳肌(SAMs)的表面肌电图(EMGs)。在实验过程中,横向瞬时听觉刺激,例如,一个哭泣的婴儿,一个破碎的花瓶,或参与者的名字,被介绍了。这些短暂的刺激要么是在沉默中呈现的,或者当参与者积极收听播客时。虽然同侧PAM活性增加响应于瞬时刺激,SAM显示相反的行为,即,一个简短的,同侧抑制活动。同侧SAM活动的抑制在右侧(75%)比左侧(35%)更频繁,而同侧PAM增加在两侧的患病率大致相等(左:90%,右:95%)。在主动收听任务期间,响应同侧刺激,右耳的SAM抑制明显更大,与对侧相比(p=0.002),而PAM活性显著增加(p=0.002)。总的来说,本研究提供了外源性注意过程中SAM的系统性瞬时抑制的证据.这可能意味着一个比以前假设的更复杂的系统,因为不同耳廓肌肉中同步的兴奋性和抑制性成分的存在指向了一种协调的尝试,即反射性地将耳廓定向为声音。
    The vestigial pinna-orienting system in humans is capable of increasing the activity of several auricular muscles in response to lateralized transient auditory stimuli. For example, transient increases in electromyographic activity in the posterior auricular muscle (PAM) to an attention-capturing stimulus have been documented. For the current study, surface electromyograms (EMGs) were recorded from the PAMs and superior auricular muscles (SAMs) of 10 normal-hearing participants. During the experiments, lateralized transient auditory stimuli, such as a crying baby, a shattering vase, or the participant\'s first names, were presented. These transient stimuli were either presented in silence or when participants actively listened to a podcast. Although ipsilateral PAM activity increased in response to transient stimuli, the SAM displayed the opposite behavior, i.e., a brief, ipsilateral suppression of activity. This suppression of ipsilateral SAM activity was more frequent on the right (75%) than left side (35%), whereas an ipsilateral PAM increase was roughly equal in prevalence on the two sides (left: 90%, right: 95%). During the active listening task, SAM suppression on the right ear was significantly larger in response to ipsilateral stimuli, compared with contralateral ones (P = 0.002), whereas PAM activity increased significantly (P = 0.002). Overall, this study provides evidence of a systematic transient suppression of the SAM during exogenous attention. This could suggest a more complex system than previously assumed, as the presence of synchronized excitatory and inhibitory components in different auricular muscles points toward a coordinated attempt at reflexively orienting the pinna toward a sound.NEW & NOTEWORTHY This study provides evidence that two auricular muscles in humans, the posterior and superior auricular muscles (PAM, SAM), react fundamentally different to lateralized transient auditory stimuli, especially during active listening. Although the PAM reacts with a transient increase in ipsilateral activity, ongoing ipsilateral SAM activity is briefly suppressed at the same time. This indicates the presence of a more complex and nuanced pinna-orienting system, with synchronized excitatory and inhibitory components in humans, than previously suspected.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目标:主要目标是调查CHAPS评估听力损失患者认知能力和听觉处理的适用性,特别是在听觉处理领域,口头工作记忆,和听觉注意力。
    方法:该研究包括44名年龄在7至14岁之间的人,22名听力损失(N=11名男性),22人听力正常(男性N=10)。个人听觉注意力,工作记忆,在研究中评估了听觉处理技能,并使用自我报告问卷。评估采用了持续听觉注意能力测试(SAACT),工作记忆量表(WMS),过滤词测试,听觉有像地面测试(AFGT),和儿童听觉表现量表(CHAPS)。进行了分析,包括组比较,相关性考试,和接收器工作特性(ROC)评估。
    结果:CHAPS总数存在显着差异,注意,噪音,安静,和组之间的多个输入。各组间CHAPS_理想记忆和CHAPS_听觉记忆无显著差异。SAACT及其子评分的研究,WMS及其子评分,FWT,AFGT显示两组之间存在显着差异,与NH组相比,HL组中的人表现不佳。SAACT及其子得分与CHAPS_attention显着相关。AUC计算表明,SAACT和CHAPS_注意区分有或没有听力损失的人(p<0.05)。WMS_stm和WMS_total与CHAPS听觉记忆分量表相关,但是WMS_vwm没有。WMS的AUC值及其子得分在识别有或没有听力损失的儿童方面显示出显著的区别(p<0.05),而CHAPS_听觉记忆没有(AUC=0.665;p=0.060)。FWT和AFGT与CHAPS_噪声和CHAPS_多输入分量表存在显着关系。CHAPS_quiet和CHAPS_ideal子测试仅与AFGT相关。CHAPS_quite和CHAPS_ideal在识别有或没有听力损失的儿童方面没有表现出显著的判别值(p<0.05),而CHAPS_噪声,CHAPS_多个输入,FWT,AFGT做到了。
    结论:CHAPS_attention分量表可能是评估听力损失儿童听觉注意力的值得信赖的工具。然而,CHAPS_听觉记忆分量表可能不适合测试工作记忆。虽然基于性能的听觉处理测试显示出更好的辨别能力,CHAPS_噪声和CHAPS_多输入子测试仍然可以评估听力受损的听觉处理。CHAPS_quiet和CHAPS_ideal子测试可能无法评估听觉处理。
    OBJECTIVE: The primary goal was to investigate the suitability of CHAPS for assessing cognitive abilities and auditory processing in people with hearing loss (HL), specifically in the domains of auditory processing, verbal working memory, and auditory attention.
    METHODS: The study comprised 44 individuals between the ages of seven and 14, 22 with HL (N = 11 males) and 22 with normal hearing (N = 10 males). Individuals\' auditory attention, working memory, and auditory processing skills were assessed in the study, and self-report questionnaires were used. The evaluation utilized the Sustained Auditory Attention Capacity Test (SAACT), Working Memory Scale (WMS), Filtered Words Test, Auditory Figured Ground Test (AFGT), and the Children\'s Auditory Performance Scale (CHAPS). Analyses were conducted, including group comparisons, correlation examinations, and receiver operating characteristic evaluations.
    RESULTS: There were significant differences in CHAPS total, attention, noise, quiet, and multiple inputs between groups. No significant differences were seen in CHAPS_ideal and CHAPS_auditory memory across groups. The study of SAACT and its subscores, WMS and its subscores, FWT, and AFGT revealed a significant difference between groups, caused by the poor performance of persons in the HL group compared to those in the NH group. The SAACT and its subscores correlated significantly with CHAPS_attention. The AUC calculation showed that The SAACT and CHAPS_attention distinguished persons with or without HL (p < 0.05). WMS_STM and WMS_total correlated with CHAPS auditory memory subscale; however, WMS_VWM did not. AUC values for WMS and its subscores showed significant discrimination in identifying children with or without HL (p < 0.05), whereas CHAPS_auditory memory did not (AUC = 0.665; p = 0.060). FWT and AFGT had a significant relationship with CHAPS_noise and CHAPS_multiple inputs subscales. The CHAPS_quiet and CHAPS_ideal subtests only correlated with AFGT. CHAPS_quite and CHAPS_ideal did not exhibit significant discriminative values (p < 0.05) for identifying children with or without HL, while CHAPS_noise, CHAPS_multiple inputs, FWT, and AFGT did.
    CONCLUSIONS: The CHAPS_attention subscale could be a trustworthy instrument for assessing auditory attention in children with HL. However, the CHAPS_auditory memory subscale may not be suitable for testing working memory. While performance-based auditory processing tests showed improved discrimination, the CHAPS_noise and CHAPS_multiple inputs subtests can still assess hearing-impaired auditory processing. The CHAPS_quiet and CHAPS_ideal subtests may not evaluate auditory processing.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    平衡是一项非常重要的技能,支持许多日常生活活动。已经建立了认知运动干扰(CMI)双任务范式来识别复杂的自然运动任务的认知负荷,比如跑步和骑自行车。在这里我们使用无线,智能手机记录的脑电图(EEG)和运动传感器,而参与者要么站在坚固的地面上,执行听觉怪球任务(双任务条件)或不同时执行任何任务(单任务条件)。与地面站立条件相比,我们预计P3事件相关电位(ERP)组件的振幅降低,延迟增加,以实现复杂平衡的目标声音。与单任务平衡条件相比,双任务进一步减少。Further,在执行并发听觉注意力任务时,我们预计在松弛时姿势摇摆会更大。二十个年轻人,有经验的懒人执行了一个听觉古怪的任务,默默地计算一系列频繁出现的标准音调中出现的罕见目标音调。结果显示在两种运动条件下相似的P3形貌和形态。与我们的预测相反,我们观察到P3振幅没有显着降低,松弛时的潜伏期也没有明显增加。出乎意料的是,与双任务相比,我们发现松弛时的姿势摇摆更大,没有额外的任务。Further,我们发现参与者的技能水平和P3潜伏期之间存在显著的相关性,但不在技能水平和P3振幅或姿势摇摆之间。这种结果模式表明,对技术较低的个体有干扰效应,而具有高技能水平的个人可能已经显示出促进效应。我们的研究增加了不断增长的研究领域,表明在不受控制的情况下获得的ERP,日常生活情况可以提供有意义的结果。我们认为,个人CMI对P3ERP的影响反映了平衡任务对未经训练的个人的要求,它利用有限的资源,否则可用于听觉注意力处理。在今后的工作中,同时记录的运动传感器信号的分析将有助于识别自然执行的运动任务的认知需求,不受控制的环境。
    Balancing is a very important skill, supporting many daily life activities. Cognitive-motor interference (CMI) dual-tasking paradigms have been established to identify the cognitive load of complex natural motor tasks, such as running and cycling. Here we used wireless, smartphone-recorded electroencephalography (EEG) and motion sensors while participants were either standing on firm ground or on a slackline, either performing an auditory oddball task (dual-task condition) or no task simultaneously (single-task condition). We expected a reduced amplitude and increased latency of the P3 event-related potential (ERP) component to target sounds for the complex balancing compared to the standing on ground condition, and a further decrease in the dual-task compared to the single-task balancing condition. Further, we expected greater postural sway during slacklining while performing the concurrent auditory attention task. Twenty young, experienced slackliners performed an auditory oddball task, silently counting rare target tones presented in a series of frequently occurring standard tones. Results revealed similar P3 topographies and morphologies during both movement conditions. Contrary to our predictions we observed neither significantly reduced P3 amplitudes, nor significantly increased latencies during slacklining. Unexpectedly, we found greater postural sway during slacklining with no additional task compared to dual-tasking. Further, we found a significant correlation between the participant\'s skill level and P3 latency, but not between skill level and P3 amplitude or postural sway. This pattern of results indicates an interference effect for less skilled individuals, whereas individuals with a high skill level may have shown a facilitation effect. Our study adds to the growing field of research demonstrating that ERPs obtained in uncontrolled, daily-life situations can provide meaningful results. We argue that the individual CMI effects on the P3 ERP reflects how demanding the balancing task is for untrained individuals, which draws on limited resources that are otherwise available for auditory attention processing. In future work, the analysis of concurrently recorded motion-sensor signals will help to identify the cognitive demands of motor tasks executed in natural, uncontrolled environments.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Objectives.这项研究旨在调查单独和同时暴露于职业噪声和手传递振动(HTV)对听觉和认知注意力的影响。方法。对40名暴露于噪声(A加权等效声压级为90dB)和HTV(31.5Hz时10m/s2)的建筑工人进行了实验研究,并在施工中使用振动设备的模拟工作下同时暴露于两者30分钟。然后在每个会话的两个暴露前和暴露后设置中评估每个个体的认知表现方面。结果。同时暴露(HTV噪声)和单独暴露于噪声对听觉注意力的影响大小非常接近(影响大小=0.648和0.626)。两种类型的注意力(选择性和分散注意力)的响应时间差异的最大变化与并发暴露场景以及然后暴露于HTV有关,分别。选择和分散注意力的正确反应的最高影响与同时暴露(HTV+噪声)和噪声暴露有关,分别。结论。同时暴露期间的HTV效应隐藏在听觉注意力中,和噪声有主要的影响。在不同场景下,分散注意力比选择性注意力受到的影响更大。
    Objectives. This study aimed to investigate the effects of separate and concurrent exposure to occupational noise and hand-transmitted vibration (HTV) on auditory and cognitive attention. Methods. The experimental study was conducted with 40 construction workers who were exposed to noise (A-weighted equivalent sound pressure level of 90 dB) and to HTV (10 m/s2 at 31.5 Hz), and concurrent exposure to both for 30 min under simulated work with vibrating equipment used in construction. Cognitive performance aspects were then evaluated from each individual in two pre-exposure and post-exposure settings for each session. Results. The effect sizes of concurrent exposure (HTV + noise) and separate exposure to noise on auditory attention were very close (effect size = 0.648 and 0.626). The largest changes in the difference of response time in both types of attention (selective and divided attention) were related to the concurrent exposure scenario and then exposure to HTV, respectively. The highest effects for the correct response of selective and divided attention are related to concurrent exposure (HTV + noise) and then noise exposure, respectively. Conclusion. The HTV effect during concurrent exposure is hidden in auditory attention, and noise has the main effects. The divided attention was more affected than the selective attention in the different scenarios.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在缺乏信息的情况下,做出基于证据的政策决策具有挑战性,特别是在决定公共资助的健康保险计划的提供者支付率时。因此,本研究的目的是评估印度三级医疗机构中人工耳蜗植入手术的成本.我们还研究了患者人工耳蜗植入手术的自付(OOP)费用。从患者和医疗系统的角度来看,我们评估了人工耳蜗植入手术的财务成本.使用自下而上的定价模型来评估医疗保健系统为人工耳蜗植入程序承担的成本。在一年的时间里,收集了为听力损失提供人工耳蜗植入服务所需的所有资源(包括资本和持续资源)的信息。120名接受人工耳蜗植入手术的听力损失患者透露了他们的自付(OOP)费用,其中包括直接医疗和非医疗费用。预计2018-2019预算年度的所有费用。单位卫生系统花费了151卢比(2美元),578卢比(7.34美元)和37,449卢比(478美元)耳部检查,听力学评估,人工耳蜗植入手术,分别。耳鼻咽喉科病房的每个床位日,住院费用202卢比(2.6美元),或1211卢比(15.5美元)。人工耳蜗手术的估计平均自付费用为682,230卢比(8710美元)。我们的研究可用于建立印度公共资助保险计划的一揽子费率,规划公共部门听力保健服务的增长,并对各种听力护理模式进行成本效益评估。
    在线版本包含补充材料,可在10.1007/s12070-023-04389-7获得。
    Making evidence-based policy decisions is challenging when there is a lack of information, especially when deciding provider payment rates for publicly funded health insurance plans. Therefore, the goal of this study was to estimate the cost of a cochlear implant operation in a tertiary care setting in India. We also looked at the patients\' out-of-pocket (OOP) expenses for the cochlear implant surgery. From the perspectives of the patients and the healthcare systems, we assessed the financial costs of the cochlear implantation procedure. A bottom-up pricing model was used to assess the cost that the healthcare system would bear for a cochlear implant procedure. Information on all the resources (both capital and ongoing) required to offer cochlear implantation services for hearing loss was gathered over the course of a year. 120 individuals with hearing loss who had cochlear implantation surgery disclosed their out-of-pocket (OOP) costs, which included both direct medical and non-medical expenses. All costs for the budgetary year 2018-2019 were anticipated. The unit health system spent ₹ 151($2), ₹ 578($7.34) and ₹ 37,449($478) on ear exams, audiological evaluations, and cochlear implant surgeries, respectively. Per bed-day in the otolaryngology ward, hospitalization cost ₹ 202($2.6), or ₹ 1211($15.5). The estimated average out-of-pocket cost for a cochlear implant operation was ₹ 682,230($8710). Our research can be used to establish package rates for publicly funded insurance plans in India, plan the growth of public sector hearing care services, and do cost-effectiveness assessments on various hearing care models.
    UNASSIGNED: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s12070-023-04389-7.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    了解努力倾听的潜在机制可以帮助减少社交退缩的情况并减轻疲劳,尤其是老年人。然而,短暂的努力和长期的疲劳之间的关系可能比原先认为的更复杂。这里,我们操纵了货币奖励的存在/不存在,以检查动机和情绪状态在控制听力感知努力和疲劳变化中的作用。在一项在线研究中,185名参与者被随机分配到“奖励”组(n=91)或“无奖励”组(n=94),并完成了一项二分聆听任务,以及一系列评估随时间变化的问卷感知努力,心情,和疲劳。奖励组的总体努力评分较高,然而,该组的疲劳评分显示,随着时间的推移,呈线性增加.中介分析显示,奖励通过感知的情绪状态对疲劳等级有间接影响;奖励会引起更积极的情绪状态,这与减少的疲劳有关。这些结果表明:(1)如果认为努力是值得的,则被评为“努力”的听力条件可能会减少疲劳,(2)对一个人的情绪状态的改变代表了一种潜在的机制,通过这种机制,在没有回报的听力情况下可能会引起疲劳。
    Knowledge of the underlying mechanisms of effortful listening could help to reduce cases of social withdrawal and mitigate fatigue, especially in older adults. However, the relationship between transient effort and longer term fatigue is likely to be more complex than originally thought. Here, we manipulated the presence/absence of monetary reward to examine the role of motivation and mood state in governing changes in perceived effort and fatigue from listening. In an online study, 185 participants were randomly assigned to either a \"reward\" (n = 91) or \"no-reward\" (n = 94) group and completed a dichotic listening task along with a series of questionnaires assessing changes over time in perceived effort, mood, and fatigue. Effort ratings were higher overall in the reward group, yet fatigue ratings in that group showed a shallower linear increase over time. Mediation analysis revealed an indirect effect of reward on fatigue ratings via perceived mood state; reward induced a more positive mood state which was associated with reduced fatigue. These results suggest that: (1) listening conditions rated as more \"effortful\" may be less fatiguing if the effort is deemed worthwhile, and (2) alterations to one\'s mood state represent a potential mechanism by which fatigue may be elicited during unrewarding listening situations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    保持沟通能力促进健康老龄化。为此,年龄典型的感觉敏锐度丧失可能部分由个体保留的注意力神经过滤补偿。这种代偿性脑行为联系纵向稳定吗?它可以预测个体听力行为的变化吗?我们在这里表明,个体听力行为和神经过滤能力在很大程度上遵循独立的发育轨迹,对N=105个衰老个体(39-82年)的脑电图和行为数据进行建模。首先,尽管预期听力阈值衍生的感觉敏锐度会下降,听力任务表现在2年以上被证明是稳定的。第二,神经滤波和行为仅在每个单独的测量时间点(T1,T2)内相关.纵向,然而,我们的结果提高了对注意力引导的神经过滤指标作为听力行为个体轨迹的预测因子的谨慎:无论是T1时的神经过滤还是其2年的变化都不能预测个体2年的行为变化,在建模策略的组合下。
    人类是群居动物。与其他人交流对我们的社会福祉至关重要,与他人建立牢固的联系与更健康的衰老有关。对于大多数人类来说,演讲是交际的一个组成部分,但是在日常的社交环境中,言语理解可能是具有挑战性的:想象一下,在拥挤的餐馆里尝试跟随对话,或者在繁忙的火车站破译公告。对于老年人来说,嘈杂的环境特别难以导航,因为与年龄相关的听力损失会影响检测和区分语音的能力。一些老年人比其他人更好地应对这个问题,但原因是,以及倾听的成功如何改变一生,知之甚少。将语音与其他声音分离的机制之一取决于大脑对听觉信号应用“神经滤波器”。大脑通过调整大脑中处理声音的部分神经元的活动来做到这一点,听觉皮层,感兴趣的语音信号的波动。这种神经“语音跟踪”可以帮助大脑更好地编码一个人正在收听的语音信号。TuneandObleser想知道个人实施此过滤策略的准确性是否代表了听力成功的标志。Further,研究人员想回答,在衰老的听众之间观察到的神经过滤强度的差异是否可以预测他们的听力能力将如何发展,并确定这些神经变化是否与人们行为的变化有关。为了回答这些问题,TuneandObleser两次使用了从一组健康的中年和老年听众中收集的数据,相隔两年。然后,他们使用这些数据建立了数学模型,以研究大脑和行为中个体之间的差异如何相互关联。研究人员发现,在两个时间点上,神经滤波能力更强的个体更善于区分言语和听力.然而,在第一个时间点测量的神经滤波强度不能很好地预测两年后个体的听力.的确,大脑和行为水平的变化彼此独立发生。Tune和Obleser的发现将与神经科学家相关,以及心理学家和听力学家,他们的目标是了解个人在听力成功方面的差异。结果表明,由注意力引导的神经过滤是个人注意力状态的重要读出。然而,结果还警告不要仅仅基于神经因素来解释听力表现,考虑到倾听行为和神经过滤遵循独立的轨迹。
    Preserved communication abilities promote healthy ageing. To this end, the age-typical loss of sensory acuity might in part be compensated for by an individual\'s preserved attentional neural filtering. Is such a compensatory brain-behaviour link longitudinally stable? Can it predict individual change in listening behaviour? We here show that individual listening behaviour and neural filtering ability follow largely independent developmental trajectories modelling electroencephalographic and behavioural data of N = 105 ageing individuals (39-82 y). First, despite the expected decline in hearing-threshold-derived sensory acuity, listening-task performance proved stable over 2 y. Second, neural filtering and behaviour were correlated only within each separate measurement timepoint (T1, T2). Longitudinally, however, our results raise caution on attention-guided neural filtering metrics as predictors of individual trajectories in listening behaviour: neither neural filtering at T1 nor its 2-year change could predict individual 2-year behavioural change, under a combination of modelling strategies.
    Humans are social animals. Communicating with other humans is vital for our social wellbeing, and having strong connections with others has been associated with healthier aging. For most humans, speech is an integral part of communication, but speech comprehension can be challenging in everyday social settings: imagine trying to follow a conversation in a crowded restaurant or decipher an announcement in a busy train station. Noisy environments are particularly difficult to navigate for older individuals, since age-related hearing loss can impact the ability to detect and distinguish speech sounds. Some aging individuals cope better than others with this problem, but the reason why, and how listening success can change over a lifetime, is poorly understood. One of the mechanisms involved in the segregation of speech from other sounds depends on the brain applying a ‘neural filter’ to auditory signals. The brain does this by aligning the activity of neurons in a part of the brain that deals with sounds, the auditory cortex, with fluctuations in the speech signal of interest. This neural ‘speech tracking’ can help the brain better encode the speech signals that a person is listening to. Tune and Obleser wanted to know whether the accuracy with which individuals can implement this filtering strategy represents a marker of listening success. Further, the researchers wanted to answer whether differences in the strength of the neural filtering observed between aging listeners could predict how their listening ability would develop, and determine whether these neural changes were connected with changes in people’s behaviours. To answer these questions, Tune and Obleser used data collected from a group of healthy middle-aged and older listeners twice, two years apart. They then built mathematical models using these data to investigate how differences between individuals in the brain and in behaviours relate to each other. The researchers found that, across both timepoints, individuals with stronger neural filtering were better at distinguishing speech and listening. However, neural filtering strength measured at the first timepoint was not a good predictor of how well individuals would be able to listen two years later. Indeed, changes at the brain and the behavioural level occurred independently of each other. Tune and Obleser’s findings will be relevant to neuroscientists, as well as to psychologists and audiologists whose goal is to understand differences between individuals in terms of listening success. The results suggest that neural filtering guided by attention to speech is an important readout of an individual’s attention state. However, the results also caution against explaining listening performance based solely on neural factors, given that listening behaviours and neural filtering follow independent trajectories.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    精神分裂症通常发生在青年时期,精神病风险综合征(PRS)发生在精神病发作之前。评估PRS个体的神经心理异常可以帮助早期识别和积极干预精神疾病。听觉P300振幅缺陷是PRS中注意力处理异常的重要表现,但目前尚不清楚PRS个体对节律性复合音调刺激的注意力处理是否存在异常,以及这些刺激诱导的P300振幅是否对PRS个体具有特异性,并与他们的临床结果相关。
    总共,226名参与者,包括122例PRS患者,51名情绪障碍(ED)患者,对53名健康对照(HC)进行了评估。在复合音调古怪任务期间记录基线脑电图。由两个频率(20-Hz,40-Hz)进行了测量。几乎所有PRS患者都随访了12个月,并重新分为四组:PRS转换,有症状的PRS,PRS-情绪障碍,和PRS完全缓解。比较了临床结果组之间基线ERPs的差异。
    无论刺激频率如何,PRS患者的平均P300振幅显著高于ED组(p=0.003,d=0.48)和HC组(p=0.002,d=0.44).PRS转换组的平均P300振幅明显高于PRS完全缓解组(p=0.016,d=0.72)和HC组(p=0.001,d=0.76),PRS症状组的平均P300振幅明显高于HC组(p=0.006,d=0.48)。无论群体如何(PRS,ED,HC)或PRS临床结果组,20Hz音调刺激引起的平均P300振幅显着高于40Hz刺激引起的平均P300振幅(ps<0.001,2=0.074-0.082)。PRS的平均反应时间明显快于ED(p=0.01,d=0.38),参与者对20Hz目标刺激的平均反应时间明显快于对40Hz目标刺激的平均反应时间(p<0.001,d=0.21)。
    有节奏的复合音调刺激引起的听觉P300振幅是PRS的特定电生理表现,复合音调刺激诱导的听觉P300振幅有望作为PRS临床结果的推定预后生物标志物,包括转换为精神病和临床完全缓解。
    UNASSIGNED: Schizophrenia often occurs in youth, and psychosis risk syndrome (PRS) occurs before the onset of psychosis. Assessing the neuropsychological abnormalities of PRS individuals can help in early identification and active intervention of mental illness. Auditory P300 amplitude defect is an important manifestation of attention processing abnormality in PRS, but it is still unclear whether there are abnormalities in the attention processing of rhythmic compound tone stimuli in PRS individuals, and whether the P300 amplitude induced by these stimuli is specific to PRS individuals and related to their clinical outcomes.
    UNASSIGNED: In total, 226 participants, including 122 patients with PRS, 51 patients with emotional disorders (ED), and 53 healthy controls (HC) were assessed. Baseline electroencephalography was recorded during the compound tone oddball task. The event-related potentials (ERPs) induced by rhythmic compound tone stimuli of two frequencies (20-Hz, 40-Hz) were measured. Almost all patients with PRS were followed up for 12 months and reclassified into four groups: PRS-conversion, PRS-symptomatic, PRS-emotional disorder, and PRS-complete remission. The differences in baseline ERPs were compared among the clinical outcome groups.
    UNASSIGNED: Regardless of the stimulation frequency, the average P300 amplitude were significantly higher in patients with PRS than in those with ED (p = 0.003, d = 0.48) and in HC (p = 0.002, d = 0.44) group. The average P300 amplitude of PRS-conversion group was significantly higher than that of the PRS-complete remission (p = 0.016, d = 0.72) and HC group (p = 0.001, d = 0.76), and the average P300 amplitude of PRS-symptomatic group was significantly higher than that of the HC group (p = 0.006, d = 0.48). Regardless of the groups (PRS, ED, HC) or the PRS clinical outcome groups, the average P300 amplitude induced by 20-Hz tone stimulation was significantly higher than that induced by 40-Hz stimulation (ps < 0.001, Ƞ2 = 0.074-0.082). The average reaction times of PRS was significantly faster than that of ED (p = 0.01, d = 0.38), and the average reaction times of the participants to 20-Hz target stimulation was significantly faster than that to 40-Hz target stimulation (p < 0.001, d = 0.21).
    UNASSIGNED: The auditory P300 amplitude induced by rhythmic compound tone stimuli is a specific electrophysiological manifestation of PRS, and the auditory P300 amplitude induced by compound tone stimuli shows promise as a putative prognostic biomarker for PRS clinical outcomes, including conversion to psychosis and clinical complete remission.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在要求苛刻的工作情况下(例如,在手术期间),复杂音景的处理随着时间的推移而变化,可能是医务人员的负担。在这里我们学习,使用移动脑电图(EEG),人类在执行复杂的视听运动任务(3D俄罗斯方块)时如何处理与工作场所相关的音景。具体来说,我们想知道注意力焦点如何改变整个音景的处理。
    参与者玩3D俄罗斯方块游戏,他们必须用双手控制掉落的方块。同时,参与者听着复杂的声景,类似于手术室中的东西(即,机械的声音,人们在后台说话,警报声音,和说明)。在这个主题内设计中,参与者必须对指示做出反应(例如,\“将下一块放在左上角\”)并根据实验条件发出声音,来自固定位置的特定警报声音或来自不同位置的蜂鸣声。对警报的关注反映了狭隘的注意焦点,因为它很容易被发现,大部分的音景可以被忽略。对蜂鸣声的关注反映了广泛的关注焦点,因为它要求参与者监视多个不同的声音流。
    结果表明,在具有复杂听觉音景的此动态任务期间,N1和P3事件相关的潜在响应的鲁棒性。此外,我们使用时间响应函数来研究听觉对整个声景的处理。这项工作是朝着使用移动EEG在手术室中研究与工作场所相关的声音处理迈出的一步。
    UNASSIGNED: In demanding work situations (e.g., during a surgery), the processing of complex soundscapes varies over time and can be a burden for medical personnel. Here we study, using mobile electroencephalography (EEG), how humans process workplace-related soundscapes while performing a complex audio-visual-motor task (3D Tetris). Specifically, we wanted to know how the attentional focus changes the processing of the soundscape as a whole.
    UNASSIGNED: Participants played a game of 3D Tetris in which they had to use both hands to control falling blocks. At the same time, participants listened to a complex soundscape, similar to what is found in an operating room (i.e., the sound of machinery, people talking in the background, alarm sounds, and instructions). In this within-subject design, participants had to react to instructions (e.g., \"place the next block in the upper left corner\") and to sounds depending on the experimental condition, either to a specific alarm sound originating from a fixed location or to a beep sound that originated from varying locations. Attention to the alarm reflected a narrow attentional focus, as it was easy to detect and most of the soundscape could be ignored. Attention to the beep reflected a wide attentional focus, as it required the participants to monitor multiple different sound streams.
    UNASSIGNED: Results show the robustness of the N1 and P3 event related potential response during this dynamic task with a complex auditory soundscape. Furthermore, we used temporal response functions to study auditory processing to the whole soundscape. This work is a step toward studying workplace-related sound processing in the operating room using mobile EEG.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号