attentional bias

注意偏见
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究的目的是阐明调节游戏刺激的注意力处理的个体差异因素,以解释现有发现的异质性。当前的研究检查了注意力控制组成部分(AC转移和AC聚焦)的个体差异是否减轻了网络游戏成瘾症状与注意力参与和游戏相关线索的脱离偏见之间的联系。
    共有75名参加过传奇联盟(LOL)2年以上的男生完成了完善的网络游戏障碍症状和注意力控制自我报告问卷。使用对远端与远端的注意力反应来测量注意力参与和脱离对游戏刺激的注意力偏见近端情绪信息(ARDPEI)任务。
    结果显示,注意控制是网络游戏成瘾症状与注意脱离偏差之间关系的重要调节因素。进一步的分析表明,仅在交流转移能力较低的人群中,网络游戏成瘾症状与注意力脱离偏见之间存在正相关关系。与我们的期望相反,AC转移也缓和了网络游戏障碍和注意力参与偏见之间的关系。网络游戏成瘾症状与注意投入偏差之间的正相关仅出现在交流转移能力水平较低的人群中。交流聚焦的个体差异并不能缓解网络游戏障碍与任何注意偏差之间的关系。
    这项研究证实,游戏成瘾的症状越严重,注意力偏见越强,特别是在具有低交流换档能力的个人中。因此,在理解网络游戏成瘾发展中注意偏差机制的本质时,有必要检查AC的子因素,并将其视为改善注意偏差的心理干预。
    UNASSIGNED: The aim of this study was to elucidate individual difference factors that modulate the attentional processing of game stimuli to explain the heterogeneity of extant findings. The current study examined whether individual differences in components of attentional control (AC-shifting and AC-focusing) moderated the link between internet gaming addiction symptom and attentional engagement to and disengagement biases from game-relevant cues.
    UNASSIGNED: A total of 75 male undergraduate students who have played League of Legend (LOL) for more than 2 years completed well-established self-report questionnaires of internet gaming disorder symptoms and attentional control. The attentional bias toward game stimuli was measured for attentional engagement and disengagement using the attentional response to the distal vs. proximal emotional information (ARDPEI) task.
    UNASSIGNED: The results revealed that attentional control was a significant moderator of the relationship between internet game addiction symptoms and attentional disengagement bias. Further analyses revealed a positive relationship between internet game addiction symptoms and attentional disengagement bias only among those with low levels of AC-shifting ability. Contrary to our expectations, AC-shifting also moderated the relationship between internet gaming disorder and attentional engagement bias. The positive relationship between internet game addiction symptoms and attentional engagement bias only appeared among those with low levels of AC-shifting ability. Individual differences in AC-focusing did not moderate the relationship between internet gaming disorder and any attentional bias.
    UNASSIGNED: This study confirmed that the greater the symptoms of game addiction, the stronger the attentional bias, especially in individuals with low AC-shifting ability. Therefore, it is necessary to examine sub-factors of AC in understanding the nature of attentional bias mechanisms in the development of internet game addiction and consider it as a psychological intervention to improve attentional bias.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:智能手机提供的注意偏差调整训练(ABMT)干预已成为缓解心理健康问题症状的远程解决方案。然而,现有文献提供了混合的结果,表明智能手机提供的干预措施的影响显着和不显著。
    目的:本系统综述和荟萃分析旨在评估智能手机提供的ABMT对注意偏差和心理健康问题症状的影响。具体来说,我们研究了不同的设计方法和管理方法,关注常见的心理健康问题,比如焦虑和抑郁,和设计元素,包括游戏化和刺激类型。
    方法:我们的搜索范围从2014年到2023年,涵盖了4个主要数据库:MEDLINE,PsycINFO,PubMed,还有Scopus.研究选择,数据提取,3名作者使用PRISMA(系统评价和荟萃分析的首选报告项目)指南独立进行批判性评估。必要时,我们将标准化平均差与95%CI进行汇总.此外,我们进行了敏感性,子组,和荟萃回归分析,以探索积极和安慰剂ABMT干预对减轻心理健康问题和注意偏差症状的调节变量。
    结果:我们的综述包括12篇论文,共有24503人参加,我们能够对来自11篇论文的20个不同的研究样本进行荟萃分析。积极的ABMT在减少心理健康问题的症状方面表现出-0.18(P=0.03)的效应大小(Hedgesg),而整体效果仍然显著。同样,安慰剂ABMT在减轻精神健康问题症状方面的效应大小为-0.38(P=.008)。此外,主动ABMT(对冲g-0.17;P=0.004)对减少注意偏差有显著影响,而安慰剂ABMT没有显著改变注意偏倚(Hedgesg-0.04;P=0.66)。
    结论:我们对智能手机提供的ABMT潜力的理解突出了积极和安慰剂干预在精神保健中的价值。主持人分析的见解还表明,针对特定威胁刺激定制智能手机提供的ABMT干预措施并考虑暴露持续时间对于优化其功效至关重要。这项研究强调了ABMT中个性化方法的必要性,以有效减少注意偏差和心理健康问题的症状。
    背景:PROSPEROCRD42023460749;https://www.crd.约克。AC.uk/prospro/display_record.php?RecordID=460749。
    BACKGROUND: Smartphone-delivered attentional bias modification training (ABMT) intervention has gained popularity as a remote solution for alleviating symptoms of mental health problems. However, the existing literature presents mixed results indicating both significant and insignificant effects of smartphone-delivered interventions.
    OBJECTIVE: This systematic review and meta-analysis aims to assess the impact of smartphone-delivered ABMT on attentional bias and symptoms of mental health problems. Specifically, we examined different design approaches and methods of administration, focusing on common mental health issues, such as anxiety and depression, and design elements, including gamification and stimulus types.
    METHODS: Our search spanned from 2014 to 2023 and encompassed 4 major databases: MEDLINE, PsycINFO, PubMed, and Scopus. Study selection, data extraction, and critical appraisal were performed independently by 3 authors using the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) guidelines. When necessary, we pooled the standardized mean difference with a 95% CI. In addition, we conducted sensitivity, subgroup, and meta-regression analyses to explore moderator variables of active and placebo ABMT interventions on reducing symptoms of mental health problems and attentional bias.
    RESULTS: Our review included 12 papers, involving a total of 24,503 participants, and we were able to conduct a meta-analysis on 20 different study samples from 11 papers. Active ABMT exhibited an effect size (Hedges g) of -0.18 (P=.03) in reducing symptoms of mental health problems, while the overall effect remained significant. Similarly, placebo ABMT showed an effect size of -0.38 (P=.008) in reducing symptoms of mental health problems. In addition, active ABMT (Hedges g -0.17; P=.004) had significant effects on reducing attentional bias, while placebo ABMT did not significantly alter attentional bias (Hedges g -0.04; P=.66).
    CONCLUSIONS: Our understanding of smartphone-delivered ABMT\'s potential highlights the value of both active and placebo interventions in mental health care. The insights from the moderator analysis also showed that tailoring smartphone-delivered ABMT interventions to specific threat stimuli and considering exposure duration are crucial for optimizing their efficacy. This research underscores the need for personalized approaches in ABMT to effectively reduce attentional bias and symptoms of mental health problems.
    BACKGROUND: PROSPERO CRD42023460749; https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?RecordID=460749.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Vogt等人。(2011)研究了目标相关性在注意力中的作用。具体来说,他们诱发了厌恶的情绪状态,并对目标相关的刺激表现出注意偏差(AB)(即,清洁度图片)使用点探针任务。在两个实验中,我们测试了(a)替代解释和(b)点探针任务的重要方法特征的作用。由于这种影响可以被解释为情感反调节(即,与清洁相关的图片引起了人们的注意,因为它们在负面厌恶状态下是正面的),我们添加了阳性刺激来测试处于厌恶状态的AB是否延伸到这些刺激。在实验1中,我们使用了定位点探针任务。也就是说,参与者必须对目标位置进行分类.可以说,这项任务混淆了注意力过程和反应启动过程。在实验2中,我们使用了辨别点探针任务,即,参与者必须对与位置正交变化的目标特征进行分类,从而消除了混淆。在实验1中,我们没有复制情绪状态对AB清洁刺激的影响,而在实验2中,我们做到了。阳性刺激的平均AB得分不受情绪状态的影响。得出两个结论:第一,实验2的结果支持Vogt及其同事的动机陈述。第二,由于两个理论原因,结果都支持使用歧视任务(即,效果可以更清楚地解释为基于注意过程)和经验原因(即,定位任务没有复制预期的模式)。
    Vogt et al. (2011) investigated the role of goal-relevance in attention. Specifically, they induced the emotional state of disgust and showed an attentional bias (AB) to goal-related stimuli (i.e., cleanliness pictures) using the dot-probe task. In two experiments, we tested (a) an alternative interpretation and (b) the role of an important methodological feature of the dot-probe task. Since the effect can be interpreted alternatively as affective counter-regulation (i.e., cleanliness-related pictures attracted attention because they are positive in the negative disgust state), we added positive stimuli to test whether the AB in the disgust state extends to these stimuli. In Experiment 1, we used the location dot-probe task. That is, participants had to categorize the location of the target. It can be argued that this task confounds attentional processes with response priming processes. In Experiment 2, we used a discrimination dot-probe task, i.e., participants had to categorize a target feature that varied orthogonally to location, thus eliminating the confound. In Experiment 1, we did not replicate the effect of emotional state on AB for cleanliness stimuli, whereas in Experiment 2 we did. Mean AB scores for positive stimuli were not affected by emotional state. Two conclusions were drawn: First, the result of Experiment 2 supports the motivational account of Vogt and colleagues. Second, the results support the use of the discrimination task for both theoretical reasons (i.e., effects can be more clearly interpreted as based on attentional processes) and empirical reasons (i.e., the location task did not replicate the expected pattern).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近的证据表明,观察者能够学习跨试验的规律性,如对目标的更快反应所表明的,目标的位置是由先前试验中目标的位置预测的。本研究调查了是否对这两个目标都有反应,就像迄今为止的研究一样,是学习发生所需要的。参与者搜索一个形状的单例目标,并对里面的线做出回应。关于目标位置,有两个跨试验预测规律:如果目标出现在给定试验的一个特定位置,它将出现在下一次审判的另一个特定地点。与以前的实验不同,对于这些规律性对中的一个,只需要对该对中的第一个或第二个目标做出反应.实验1表明,跨试验学习仅在需要对一对目标的两个目标做出响应时才发生。如果必须保留对一对中的一个目标的响应,没有学习发生。实验2表明,学习的缺失不能归因于由于无需响应而导致的结转抑制。在了解了跨试验的突发事件后,即使必须保留对这对第一目标的反应,学习仍然存在。我们的研究结果表明,对于这两个试验的(任意)简单按键响应的执行是需要进行跨试验统计学习的,而仅参加目标地点不会导致任何学习。
    Recent evidence shows that observers are able to learn across-trial regularities as indicated by faster responses to targets whose location was predicted by the target\'s location on the preceding trial. The present study investigated whether responding to both targets of the pair, as was the case in studies thus far, was needed for learning to occur. Participants searched for a shape singleton target and responded to the line inside. There were two across-trial predicting-predicted regularities regarding target locations: if the target appeared at one specific location on a given trial, it would appear at another specific location on the next trial. Unlike previous experiments, for one of these regularity pairs a response was only needed on either the first or the second target in the pair. Experiment 1 showed that across-trial learning only occurred when responding was required to both targets of a pair. If the response to one target of a pair had to be withheld, no learning occurred. Experiment 2 showed that the absence of learning cannot be attributed to carry-over inhibition resulting from not having to respond. After learning across-trial contingencies, learning remained in place even when the response to the first target of the pair had to be withheld. Our findings show that the execution of the (arbitrary) simple key-press response for both trials of the pair was needed for across-trial statistical learning to occur, whereas solely attending target locations did not result in any learning.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:注意过程受刺激特征和个体因素(如情绪或个人经历)的影响。研究表明,在童年和青春期有同伴受害史的个体中,可能会出现对社会相关刺激的注意力偏见。基于此,本研究旨在检查有同伴受害经历的参与者在行为和神经生理水平上对情绪面孔的注意过程。
    方法:在60名成人参与者的样本中,回顾性报道的儿童和青少年中同伴受害的严重程度不同,点探针任务是愤怒的,恶心,悲伤,和快乐的面部表情。除了行为反应,生理反应(即,事件相关电位)进行分析。
    结果:对平均P100和P200振幅的分析显示,在同伴受害程度较高的个体中,P200振幅发生了改变。较高水平的关系同伴受害与响应面部表情的P200振幅增加有关,特别是愤怒和厌恶的面部表情。分层回归分析显示,没有证据表明同伴受害经历对不同情绪的反应时间或P100振幅有影响。
    结论:皮质研究结果表明,同伴受害程度较高的个体在面对负面情绪社会刺激时,会调动更多的注意力资源。童年和青春期的同伴受害经历似乎会影响成年后的皮质过程。
    BACKGROUND: Attentional processes are influenced by both stimulus characteristics and individual factors such as mood or personal experience. Research has suggested that attentional biases to socially relevant stimuli may occur in individuals with a history of peer victimization in childhood and adolescence. Based on this, the present study aimed to examine attentional processes in response to emotional faces at both the behavioral and neurophysiological levels in participants with experiences of peer victimization.
    METHODS: In a sample of 60 adult participants with varying severity of retrospectively reported peer victimization in childhood and adolescence, the dot-probe task was administered with angry, disgusted, sad, and happy facial expressions. In addition to behavioral responses, physiological responses (i.e., event-related potentials) were analyzed.
    RESULTS: Analyses of mean P100 and P200 amplitudes revealed altered P200 amplitudes in individuals with higher degrees of peer victimization. Higher levels of relational peer victimization were associated with increased P200 amplitudes in response to facial expressions, particularly angry and disgusted facial expressions. Hierarchical regression analyses showed no evidence for an influence of peer victimization experiences on reaction times or P100 amplitudes in response to the different emotions.
    CONCLUSIONS: Cortical findings suggest that individuals with higher levels of peer victimization mobilize more attentional resources when confronted with negative emotional social stimuli. Peer victimization experiences in childhood and adolescence appear to influence cortical processes into adulthood.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    与社交焦虑较低的人相比,社交焦虑较高的人对自我相关信息和外在威胁刺激的注意力偏见更大。一些研究调查了社交焦虑高的人的自我相关信息与外部社交线索之间的注意偏差,但没有得出一致的结论。这项研究使用了改良的点探针任务,并在15名社交焦虑高的人和20名社交焦虑低的人中收集了对三种自我对方面孔的时间和脑电图反应。与外部社交线索相比,两组对自我相关信息的反应都更快。其他人的情绪效力缓和了注意力偏见。与社交焦虑较低的人相比,社交焦虑较高的人在所有面部对中表现出较大的P1幅度和较小的N170幅度(主要在右半球),在自我中立和其他愤怒的面部对中表现出较大的P2幅度。这些发现表明,有社交焦虑的人优先考虑将注意力资源分配给自我相关的信息,与其他愤怒的面孔对分配影响最大。与社交焦虑较低的人相比,社交焦虑较高的人对所有三种面孔都表现出更高的注意力警惕和更少的结构编码。与低社交焦虑的人相比,他们在威胁线索上投入了更多的注意力资源。
    Individuals with high social anxiety show a more considerable attentional bias toward self-relevant information and outwardly threatening stimuli than do those with low social anxiety. Some studies have investigated the attentional bias between self-relevant information and external social cues in people with high social anxiety but have not reached consistent conclusions. This study used a modified dot-probe task and collected temporal and electroencephalogram responses to three self-other face pairs among 15 people with high social anxiety and 20 people with low social anxiety. Both groups responded more quickly to self-relevant information than external social cues. Others\' emotional potencies moderated the attentional bias. Individuals with high social anxiety exhibited larger P1 amplitudes and smaller N170 amplitudes (mainly in the right hemisphere) for all face pairs and larger P2 amplitudes for the self-neutral and other-angry face pairs than those with low social anxiety. These findings suggest that people with social anxiety prioritize allocating attentional resources to self-relevant information, with others\' angry faces having the greatest influence on the allocation. Individuals with high social anxiety showed more attentional vigilance and less structural coding for all three face pairs compared to those with low social anxiety. They devoted more attentional resources to threat cues than low social anxiety individuals.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自杀的认知模型提出,有偏见的认知过程有助于自杀的想法和行为,初步证据表明,对自杀一致线索的注意偏差可能会预测超出既定临床风险因素的自杀行为。迄今为止,更少的工作探讨了解释偏差在自杀风险的发展和预测中的潜在作用。本研究评估了心理测量特性,包括可靠性,并发有效性,和预测效度,词句协会范式(WSAP)的新颖改编,以评估自杀-自杀解释偏见。在239名精神病住院患者的样本中,包括近期没有自杀意念或终生尝试的患者(n=35),那些最近自杀未遂的人(n=29),以及最近有自杀意念的人(n=174),参与者完成了改良后的WSAP以及自我报告的自杀意念和随后6个月的尝试.尽管WSAP表现出良好的可靠性,同时有效性和前瞻性有效性的证据(就自我报告的自杀意念和尝试而言)有限.具体来说,临床组在解释偏差的任何度量上彼此没有差异,在控制烦躁不安的解释偏倚时,自杀特异性解释支持也不能预测并发或未来的自杀意念.然而,自杀特异性解释偏差与终生自杀未遂的历史独特相关.未来的工作应进一步阐明自杀特异性解释偏差与临床结果之间关系的程度和特异性。
    The cognitive model of suicide proposes that biased cognitive processes contribute to suicidal thoughts and behaviors, and initial evidence suggests that attentional bias to suicide-congruent cues may predict suicidal behavior beyond established clinical risk factors. To date, much less work has explored the potential role of interpretation biases in the development and prediction of suicide risk. The present study assessed the psychometric properties, including reliability, concurrent validity, and predictive validity, of a novel adaptation of the Word Sentence Association Paradigm (WSAP) to assess suicide-suicide interpretation biases. In a sample of 239 psychiatric inpatients, including patients with no recent suicidal ideation or lifetime attempts (n = 35), those with recent suicide attempts (n = 29), and those with recent suicidal ideation (n = 174), participants completed the modified WSAP as well as self-reported suicidal ideation and attempts for the subsequent 6 months. Although the WSAP demonstrated good reliability, evidence of concurrent and prospective validity (in terms of self-reported suicidal ideation and attempts) was limited. Specifically, the clinical groups did not differ from one another on any measure of interpretation bias, nor did suicide-specific interpretation endorsements predict concurrent or future suicidal ideation when controlling for dysphoric interpretation bias. However, suicide-specific interpretation biases were uniquely associated with a history of a lifetime suicide attempt. Future work should further clarify the extent and specificity of the relationship between suicide-specific interpretation biases and clinical outcomes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:我们小组最近的一项研究发现,在清醒和醉酒状态下,女性在卵泡后期和早期对酒精相关线索的注意偏差更大,这表明女性在这一阶段过度饮酒的风险更大。注意偏差作为月经周期阶段的函数的变化引发了关于相对一致性的潜在性别差异的问题,随着时间的推移,女性和男性表现出对酒精的注意偏差。
    目的:本研究通过比较从早期到晚期卵泡期女性注意偏差与同期男性注意偏差的变化,来检验酒精注意偏差的性别差异。
    方法:25名21-32岁的男性和女性参加了一项安慰剂对照研究,研究了酒精奖励特性的性别差异。参与者在0.6g/kg酒精和安慰剂后的两个疗程中完成了对酒精相关线索的注意偏差测量。考试间隔一周,女性参与者与卵泡早期和晚期相吻合。
    结果:在两种剂量下,男性在整个疗程中始终表现出对酒精相关线索的注意偏差。相比之下,女性仅在卵泡后期表现出注意偏差,比男性观察到的幅度更大,在两种剂量下都持续存在。
    结论:这些发现强调了性和月经周期阶段在使饮酒者对酒精相关线索的奖励特性敏感方面的潜在作用。男人喝酒的动机可能保持相对一致,而女性在卵泡后期可能最有动力。
    BACKGROUND: A recent study by our group found that women displayed greater attentional bias to alcohol-related cues during the late versus early follicular phase in both sober and intoxicated states, suggesting a greater risk of excessive drinking among women during this phase. Changes in attentional bias as a function of menstrual cycle phase raise questions about potential sex differences in the relative consistency by which women and men display attentional bias to alcohol over time.
    OBJECTIVE: The present study tested sex differences in attentional bias to alcohol by comparing the change in women\'s attentional bias from early to late follicular phase to that observed in men over the same period.
    METHODS: Twenty-five men and 25 women aged 21-32 participated in a placebo-controlled study examining sex differences in the rewarding properties of alcohol. Participants completed measures of attentional bias to alcohol-related cues during two sessions following both 0.6 g/kg alcohol and placebo. Test sessions occurred one week apart, and for female participants coincided with the early and late follicular phases.
    RESULTS: Men consistently displayed attentional bias to alcohol-related cues across sessions under both doses. By contrast, women showed attentional bias only during the late follicular phase, at a magnitude greater than that observed in men, and persistent under both doses.
    CONCLUSIONS: These findings highlight the potential role of sex and menstrual cycle phase in sensitizing drinkers to rewarding properties of alcohol-related cues. Men\'s motivation to drink may remain relatively consistent, whereas women may be most motivated during the late follicular phase.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:本研究旨在基于脑电图(EEG)功能连接(FC)研究失眠障碍(ID)患者的睡眠辅助音乐与睡眠相关注意偏差之间的关系。评估音乐在帮助睡眠方面的有效性。
    方法:这项研究包括30名参与者,包括15名ID患者和15名健康对照(HC)。选择了六种音乐来帮助睡眠,并利用基于睡眠相关注意偏差的点探针任务来收集行为和脑电图数据。使用反应时间和EEGFC测量警惕偏差和脱离偏差。探索音乐干预前后睡眠相关注意偏差的差异,以评估睡眠辅助效果并确定EEG生物标志物。
    结果:与HC相比,ID患者在枕中央叶和颞叶额叶之间的EEGFC睡眠相关注意偏差降低。在六种音乐中,国际标准睡眠辅助和摇篮曲对减少ID组的警惕偏差有更大的影响。此外,国际标准睡眠援助和自然声音在减少ID组中脱离偏倚方面更有效.这项研究还检查了音乐干预前后ID患者的静息状态EEGFC。结果表明,FC在时间上,额叶,枕叶在音乐干预前后有显著差异,特别是使用国际标准睡眠辅助,摇篮曲,和阿尔法声波。然而,值得注意的是,这三种类型的音乐在EEGFC中没有显示出相似之处,与EEGFC的睡眠相关注意偏差的结果相反。
    结论:这项研究发现,与静息状态EEGFC相比,EEGFC的睡眠相关注意偏差具有更明显的特征。结果表明,EEGFC的睡眠相关注意偏差可能是评估音乐干预的睡眠辅助效果的潜在生物标志物。在六种睡眠辅助音乐中,国际标准睡眠辅助对ID患者最有效。这些发现可以促进针对ID患者的个性化治疗的开发。
    背景:中国临床试验注册,http://www。chictr.org.cn,ID:ChiCTR2400081608。
    OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to investigate the relationship between sleep-aiding music and sleep-related attentional bias based on electroencephalography (EEG) functional connectivity (FC) in patients with insomnia disorder (ID), to evaluate the effectiveness of music in aiding sleep.
    METHODS: This study included 30 participants, comprising 15 patients with ID and 15 healthy controls (HCs). Six types of music were selected for sleep aid, and a dot-probe task based on sleep-related attentional bias was utilized to collect behavioral and EEG data. Vigilance bias and disengagement bias were measured using reaction time and EEG FC. Differences in sleep-related attentional bias before and after the intervention of music were explored to evaluate the sleep-aiding effects and identify EEG biomarkers.
    RESULTS: Compared with HCs, patients with ID showed decreased sleep-related attentional bias of EEG FC between occipital-central and temporal-frontal lobes. Among the six types of music, International Standard Sleep Aid and Lullaby had a greater impact on decreasing vigilance bias in the ID group. Additionally, the International Standard Sleep Aid and Nature Sound were more effective in decreasing disengagement bias in the ID group. This study also examined the resting-state EEG FC of patients with ID before and after the intervention of music. The results showed that the FC in the temporal, frontal, and occipital lobes significantly differed before and after the intervention of music, especially with the use of International Standard Sleep Aid, Lullaby, and Alpha Sound Wave. However, it is worth noting that these three types of music showed no similarities in EEG FC, in contrast to the result of sleep-related attentional bias of EEG FC.
    CONCLUSIONS: This study found that the sleep-related attentional bias of EEG FC has more distinct characteristics when compared to resting-state EEG FC. The results suggest that the sleep-related attentional bias of EEG FC could be a potential biomarker for assessing the sleep-aiding effect of music interventions. International Standard Sleep Aid was the most effective for patients with ID among six types of sleep-aiding music. These findings could facilitate the development of personalized therapies for patients with ID.
    BACKGROUND: Chinese Clinical Trial Register, http://www.chictr.org.cn, ID: ChiCTR2400081608.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    正念与认知过程的益处有关,包括注意。然而,正念之间的确切关系,注意力的组成部分,奖励环境的作用尚未完全阐明,这是相关的,尤其是在成瘾的背景下。在目前的研究中,我们特别评估了性格正念和自愿(自上而下)之间的平衡之间的关系,和刺激驱动(自下而上)的注意力。此外,我们探讨了这种关系是否由额叶大脑活动的不对称介导,接近趋势的指标,并且随着奖励环境的变化而变化。
    总共,95名参与者(30名男性,65名女性),平均年龄25.87岁(SD=7.38)。使用EEG记录静息状态的电生理活动,并对参与者进行了倾向正念评估,并执行视觉空间提示(VSC)任务,在中性和可口的食物(奖励)环境中索引自愿和刺激驱动的注意力。在内生VSC任务中,一个中央提示信号的可能位置的后续目标。有效性效应表示有效提示相对于无效提示的成本在响应时间方面的益处。
    倾向正念与有效性效应降低有关,合理地反映了自愿关注减少和刺激驱动关注增加的结合,不管条件如何。性格正念和视觉空间注意力之间的关系不能用额叶大脑活动的不对称性来解释。
    UNASSIGNED: Mindfulness has been associated with benefits on cognitive processes, including attention. However, the exact relationship between mindfulness, components of attention, and the role of reward context has not yet been fully elucidated, which is relevant, especially in the context of addiction. In the current study, we specifically evaluated the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and the balance between voluntary (top-down), and stimulus-driven (bottom-up) attention. In addition, we explored whether the relationship was mediated by asymmetry of frontal brain activity, an index of approach tendencies, and varies as a function of reward context.
    UNASSIGNED: In total, 95 participants (30 male, 65 female) with a mean age of 25.87 (SD = 7.38) participated. Resting-state electrophysiological activity was recorded using EEG, and participants were assessed on dispositional mindfulness, and performed the visuospatial cueing (VSC) task, which indexed voluntary- and stimulus-driven attention in a neutral and palatable food (reward) context. In the endogenous VSC task, a central cue signals the likely location of a subsequent target. The validity effect represents the benefit of valid cueing relative to the costs of invalid cueing in terms of response time.
    UNASSIGNED: Dispositional mindfulness was associated with a reduced validity effect, plausibly reflecting a combination of reduced voluntary attention and increased stimulus-driven attention, irrespective of condition. The relationship between dispositional mindfulness and visuospatial attention could not be explained by asymmetry of frontal brain activity.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号