atom

原子
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    原子探针断层扫描(APT)已用于研究两种模型硼硅酸盐玻璃的微观结构,这些玻璃旨在了解五氧化二磷(P2O5)的溶解度极限。该成分存在于某些用于玻璃化的高放射性国防废物中,其中相分离可能导致许多与玻璃加工及其长期化学和结构稳定性有关的问题。初步确定了合适的聚焦离子束(FIB)制备路线和APT分析条件,在检查它们详细的微观结构之前。在3.0摩尔%P2O5掺杂玻璃中,APT数据的视觉检查和敏感统计分析均显示出均匀的微观结构,将含量提高到4.0mol%时,会引发富集磷的纳米级沉淀物的形成。这项研究证实了这些玻璃的预期不均匀性和相分离,并提供了使用APT以近原子尺度分辨率表征这些玻璃的途径。
    Atom probe tomography (APT) has been utilized to investigate the microstructure of two model borosilicate glasses designed to understand the solubility limits of phosphorous pentoxide (P2O5). This component is found in certain high-level radioactive defence wastes destined for vitrification, where phase separation can potentially lead to a number of issues relating to the processing of the glass and its long-term chemical and structural stability. The development of suitable focused ion beam (FIB)-preparation routes and APT analysis conditions were initially determined for the model glasses, before examining their detailed microstructures. In a 3.0 mol% P2O5-doped glass, both visual inspection and sensitive statistical analysis of the APT data show homogeneous microstructures, while raising the content to 4.0 mol% initiates the formation of phosphorus-enriched nanoscale precipitates. This study confirms the expected inhomogeneities and phase separation of these glasses and offers routes to characterizing these at near-atomic scale resolution using APT.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    发展性计算障碍(DD)是一种特殊的学习障碍,可阻止儿童获得足够的数字和算术能力。我们调查了DD儿童的困难是否超出了数字范围,并影响了他们感知时间的能力。一组37名患有和不患有DD的儿童/青少年接受了听觉分类任务,测量了亚秒(0.25-1s)和超秒(0.75-3s)范围内的时间感知阈值。结果表明,在两个时间尺度上,DD儿童的听觉时间知觉均受到强烈损害。即使年龄较大,损伤仍然存在,非语言推理,性别被退步了。总的来说,我们的结果表明,DD的困难可以影响数值以外的幅度,并有助于越来越多的证据表明,将计算障碍视为一种影响多种神经认知和感知系统的疾病。
    Developmental dyscalculia (DD) is a specific learning disability which prevents children from acquiring adequate numerical and arithmetical competences. We investigated whether difficulties in children with DD spread beyond the numerical domain and impact also their ability to perceive time. A group of 37 children/adolescent with and without DD were tested with an auditory categorization task measuring time perception thresholds in the sub-second (0.25-1 s) and supra-second (0.75-3 s) ranges. Results showed that auditory time perception was strongly impaired in children with DD at both time scales. The impairment remained even when age, non-verbal reasoning, and gender were regressed out. Overall, our results show that the difficulties of DD can affect magnitudes other than numerical and contribute to the increasing evidence that frames dyscalculia as a disorder affecting multiple neurocognitive and perceptual systems.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    分组是一种完善的数字感知策略,可提高速度和感觉精度。基于ATOM理论,Anobile提出了感觉运动数字系统,这表明数字和行动之间有着密切的联系。以前使用电机适应技术的研究表明,高频电机适应会导致对数字感知的低估,而低频适应会导致高估。然而,运动适应对成群的影响,以及视觉运动适应是否会产生类似的效果,仍然不清楚。在这项研究中,我们研究了运动适应后群体优势的持久性,并探讨了视觉运动适应的影响。令人惊讶的是,我们的发现表明,本体感觉运动适应削弱了群体化的优势,表明即使采用分组,运动适应也有强大的效果。此外,我们观察到双向关系,因为成群也削弱了适应效果。这些结果突显了运动适应与数字感知中的群体之间复杂的相互作用。此外,我们的研究提供了证据,表明视觉运动适应也有适应效应,但不能完全复制本体感觉运动适应对成群的影响。总之,我们的研究强调了分组作为数字感知中一种有价值的策略的重要性,并阐明了运动适应对该策略的影响。
    Groupitizing is a well-established strategy in numerosity perception that enhances speed and sensory precision. Building on the ATOM theory, Anobile proposed the sensorimotor numerosity system, which posits a strong link between number and action. Previous studies using motor adaptation technology have shown that high-frequency motor adaptation leads to underestimation of numerosity perception, while low-frequency adaptation leads to overestimation. However, the impact of motor adaptation on groupitizing, and whether visual motion adaptation produces similar effects, remain unclear. In this study, we investigate the persistence of the advantage of groupitizing after motor adaptation and explore the effects of visual motion adaptation. Surprisingly, our findings reveal that proprioceptive motor adaptation weakens the advantage of groupitizing, indicating a robust effect of motor adaptation even when groupitizing is employed. Moreover, we observe a bidirectional relationship, as groupitizing also weakens the adaptation effect. These results highlight the complex interplay between motor adaptation and groupitizing in numerosity perception. Furthermore, our study provides evidence that visual motion adaptation also has an adaptation effect, but does not fully replicate the effects of proprioceptive motor adaptation on groupitizing. In conclusion, our research underscores the importance of groupitizing as a valuable strategy in numerosity perception, and sheds light on the influence of motion adaptation on this strategy.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当参与者决定所呈现的音调是响亮的还是柔和的时,与使用底侧响应键相比,他们对带有顶侧响应键的响亮音调的反应更快,反之亦然。此效果与完善的水平空间-数字响应码关联(SNARC)效果相当,并且通常被称为响度的空间-音乐响应码关联(SMARC)效果。响度的SMARC效应通常通过空间表示的假设或极性对应原理来解释。至关重要的是,当响度与任务无关时,两种理论在SMARC效应的预测上都不同。因此,我们调查了在音色辨别任务中是否仍然发生SMARC效应:参与者(N=36)听到了单个音调,并用垂直排列的响应键对其音色进行了分类。此外,音调的响度级别有六个级别不同。在空间表示的情况下,在极性对应原理的情况下,SMARC效应仍应发生,效果应该不存在。结果表明,SMARC效应仍然存在,并且上侧和下侧响应之间的差异是响度水平的线性函数,表明响度的连续空间表示。
    When participants decide whether a presented tone is loud or soft they react faster to loud tones with a top-sided response key in comparison to a bottom-sided response key and vice versa for soft tones. This effect is comparable to the well-established horizontal Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) effect and is often referred to as Spatial-Musical Association of Response Codes (SMARC) effect for loudness. The SMARC effect for loudness is typically explained by the assumption of a spatial representation or by the polarity correspondence principle. Crucially, both theories differ in the prediction of the SMARC effect when loudness is task-irrelevant. Therefore, we investigated whether the SMARC effect still occurs in a timbre discrimination task: Participants (N = 36) heard a single tone and classified its timbre with vertically arranged response keys. Additionally, the tone\'s loudness level varied in six levels. In case of a spatial representation, the SMARC effect should still occur while in case of polarity corresponding principle, the effect should be absent. Results showed that the SMARC effect was still present and that the differences between top-sided and bottom-sided responses were a linear function of loudness level indicating a continuous spatial representation of loudness.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    心理学缺乏数学形式化的原语。因此,概念和定量研究缺乏将它们置于自然科学公司的本体论基础。本文通过描述最小的心理结构来解决这个问题,用量子理论的代数表示。结构被划分为情感和颜色的类别,被誉为基本的心理现象。这是通过量子理论量子比特状态空间来实现的,在意义和数学上都与情感和色彩体验同构。特别是,颜色通过HSL-RGB颜色固体和Bloch球体之间的几何亲和力映射到量子位状态,在物理学中广泛使用。由此产生的对应关系与最近的主观经验模型相一致,生成统一的球形情感和颜色图。这种结构被识别为自然思维的语义原子-一种带有情感色彩的个人含义的单位,参与二元决策的基本行为。该模型有助于找到惰性和生命自然的统一本体论,桥接以前断开的研究领域。特别是,它能够实现基于理论的情感协调,decision,和控制论科学,需要达到新的实际影响水平。
    Psychology suffers from the absence of mathematically-formalized primitives. As a result, conceptual and quantitative studies lack an ontological basis that would situate them in the company of natural sciences. The article addresses this problem by describing a minimal psychic structure, expressed in the algebra of quantum theory. The structure is demarcated into categories of emotion and color, renowned as elementary psychological phenomena. This is achieved by means of quantum-theoretic qubit state space, isomorphic to emotion and color experiences both in meaning and math. In particular, colors are mapped to the qubit states through geometric affinity between the HSL-RGB color solids and the Bloch sphere, widely used in physics. The resulting correspondence aligns with the recent model of subjective experience, producing a unified spherical map of emotions and colors. This structure is identified as a semantic atom of natural thinking-a unit of affectively-colored personal meaning, involved in elementary acts of a binary decision. The model contributes to finding a unified ontology of both inert and living Nature, bridging previously disconnected fields of research. In particular, it enables theory-based coordination of emotion, decision, and cybernetic sciences, needed to achieve new levels of practical impact.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    幅度理论(ATOM)表明,大脑中的广义幅度系统处理诸如空间之类的幅度,时间,和数字。许多行为和神经认知研究为ATOM理论提供了支持。然而,常见的震级处理的证据主要来自于在视觉上呈现数值和时间信息的研究。我们目前对这种跨维度幅度相互作用的理解仅限于视觉模态。然而,尚不清楚ATOM框架是否考虑了跨模态震级信息的整合。为了检查数值幅度对音调时间处理的跨模态影响,我们使用时间平分任务进行了三个实验。我们在持续时间判断任务的同时提供了视觉域中的数值幅度信息和听觉中的时间信息(实验1),在持续时间判断任务之前(实验2),在持续时间判断任务之前,但数值大小也与任务相关(实验3)。结果表明,仅当数值幅度与任务相关且在进行时间判断时可用时,视觉域中呈现的数字信息才会影响音调的时间处理(实验1和3)。然而,当与持续时间信息在时间上分离时,数字信息不会干扰时间信息(实验2)。研究结果表明,跨模态设置中视觉数字对时间处理的影响可能不是来自共同的幅度系统,而是来自注意力和记忆等一般认知机制。
    A theory of magnitude (ATOM) suggests that a generalized magnitude system in the brain processes magnitudes such as space, time, and numbers. Numerous behavioral and neurocognitive studies have provided support to ATOM theory. However, the evidence for common magnitude processing primarily comes from the studies in which numerical and temporal information are presented visually. Our current understanding of such cross-dimensional magnitude interactions is limited to visual modality only. However, it is still unclear whether the ATOM-framework accounts for the integration of cross-modal magnitude information. To examine the cross-modal influence of numerical magnitude on temporal processing of the tone, we conducted three experiments using a temporal bisection task. We presented the numerical magnitude information in the visual domain and the temporal information in the auditory either simultaneously with duration judgment task (Experiment-1), before duration judgment task (Experiment-2), and before duration judgment task but with numerical magnitude also being task-relevant (Experiment-3). The results suggest that the numerical information presented in the visual domain affects temporal processing of the tone only when the numerical magnitudes were task-relevant and available while making a temporal judgment (Experiments-1 and 3). However, numerical information did not interfere with temporal information when presented temporally separated from the duration information (Experiments-2). The findings indicate that the influence of visual numbers on temporal processing in cross-modal settings may not arise from the common magnitude system but instead from general cognitive mechanisms like attention and memory.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    越来越多的证据表明,动作和感知在持续时间和数量等幅度的处理中相互作用。持续的体育锻炼(例如跑步或骑自行车)会增加活动期间视觉刺激的明显持续时间。然而,运动对数字感知的影响尚未得到研究。这里,我们要求参与者通过比较静息时显示的标准刺激与跑步时显示的标准刺激的持续时间或数字来做出时间或数字判断.结果支持以前的报告,表明身体活动显着延长了感知的持续时间;然而,它对感知的数字没有影响。此外,感知持续时间的扭曲在跑步会话后不久消失了,使得诸如心率之类的生理因素不太可能成为时间失真的基础。一起来看,这些结果表明,运动系统对时间感知的域选择性影响,而不是对幅度的一般影响。
    There is increasing evidence that action and perception interact in the processing of magnitudes such as duration and numerosity. Sustained physical exercise (such as running or cycling) increases the apparent duration of visual stimuli presented during the activity. However, the effect of exercise on numerosity perception has not yet been investigated. Here, we asked participants to make either a temporal or a numerical judgment by comparing the duration or numerosity of standard stimuli displayed at rest with those presented while running. The results support previous reports in showing that physical activity significantly expands perceived duration; however, it had no effect on perceived numerosity. Furthermore, the distortions of the perceived durations vanished soon after the running session, making it unlikely that physiological factors such as heart rate underlie the temporal distortion. Taken together, these results suggest a domain-selective influence of the motor system on the perception of time, rather than a general effect on magnitude.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Ozone is the third most important anthropogenic greenhouse gas after carbon dioxide and methane but has a larger uncertainty in its radiative forcing, in part because of uncertainty in the source characteristics of ozone precursors, nitrogen oxides, and volatile organic carbon that directly affect ozone formation chemistry. Tropospheric ozone also negatively affects human and ecosystem health. Biomass burning (BB) and urban emissions are significant but uncertain sources of ozone precursors. Here, we report global-scale, in situ airborne measurements of ozone and precursor source tracers from the NASA Atmospheric Tomography mission. Measurements from the remote troposphere showed that tropospheric ozone is regularly enhanced above background in polluted air masses in all regions of the globe. Ozone enhancements in air with high BB and urban emission tracers (2.1 to 23.8 ppbv [parts per billion by volume]) were generally similar to those in BB-influenced air (2.2 to 21.0 ppbv) but larger than those in urban-influenced air (-7.7 to 6.9 ppbv). Ozone attributed to BB was 2 to 10 times higher than that from urban sources in the Southern Hemisphere and the tropical Atlantic and roughly equal to that from urban sources in the Northern Hemisphere and the tropical Pacific. Three independent global chemical transport models systematically underpredict the observed influence of BB on tropospheric ozone. Potential reasons include uncertainties in modeled BB injection heights and emission inventories, export efficiency of BB emissions to the free troposphere, and chemical mechanisms of ozone production in smoke. Accurately accounting for intermittent but large and widespread BB emissions is required to understand the global tropospheric ozone burden.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在原子和多粒子物理学中,格林函数通常作为传播者出现,以正式表示基础哈密顿量的完整光谱(积分)。然而,虽然这些函数对于描述许多二阶和高阶扰动过程非常关键,它们几乎没有被考虑和分类为复杂的原子。这里,我们展示了如何对少数和多电子原子和离子进行相对论(多电子)格林函数的近似和系统改进。这些函数的表示基于虚拟激励的类别,或者所谓的激励方案,关于给定的绑定状态引用配置,并通过对所涉及的所有原子状态应用多配置Dirac-Hartree-Fock扩展。这些近似格林函数的第一个实现已经在Jac的框架中实现,耶拿原子计算器,并将促进各种多光子和/或多电子(发射)过程的研究。
    In atomic and many-particle physics, Green functions often occur as propagators to formally represent the (integration over the) complete spectrum of the underlying Hamiltonian. However, while these functions are very crucial to describing many second- and higher-order perturbation processes, they have hardly been considered and classified for complex atoms. Here, we show how relativistic (many-electron) Green functions can be approximated and systematically improved for few- and many-electron atoms and ions. The representation of these functions is based on classes of virtual excitations, or so-called excitation schemes, with regard to given bound-state reference configurations, and by applying a multi-configuration Dirac-Hartree-Fock expansion of all atomic states involved. A first implementation of these approximate Green functions has been realized in the framework of Jac, the Jena Atomic Calculator, and will facilitate the study of various multi-photon and/or multiple electron (emission) processes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    多组分反应(MCR)在锅中具有固有的优势,原子,和阶梯经济(PASE)。这种重要的绿色合成方法由于效率高而受到越来越多的关注,最小的浪费,节约资源,和简单的程序。本文介绍了以下六种类型的5-compoment反应(5CRs)的最新发展:(I)五种不同的分子ABCDE;伪5CRs,包括(II)2ABCD,(三)2A+2B+C,(四)3A+B+C,(五)3A+2B,和(VI)4AB.5CRs具有五个以上的反应中心也包括在内。
    Multicomponent reactions (MCRs) have inherent advantages in pot, atom, and step economy (PASE). This important green synthetic approach has gained increasing attention due to high efficiency, minimal waste, saving resources, and straightforward procedures. Presented in this review article are the recent development on 5-compoment reactions (5CRs) of the following six types: (I) five different molecules A + B + C + D + E; pseudo-5CRs including (II) 2A + B + C + D, (III) 2A + 2B + C, (IV) 3A + B + C, (V) 3A + 2B, and (VI) 4A + B. 5CRs with more than five-reaction centers are also included.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号