Working memory

工作记忆
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    使用以人为本的方法,我们旨在识别不同的执行功能概况,以通过潜在概况分析评估同一学校年级内个体之间的异质性。150名2年级(7-8岁)的样本,150年级6(11-12岁),并对150名10年级(15-16岁)儿童和青少年进行了11种不同的执行任务的评估,这些任务代表了三个主要的执行功能子组件(即,抑制,认知灵活性,和工作记忆),流体智能,处理速度,解决问题,阅读理解。基于抑制的不同互动模式的三种不同执行功能概况,认知灵活性,并确定了年级内和年级之间的工作记忆。此外,这些情况与阅读理解和数学成绩有不同的关系。第二,正如预期的那样,我们没有发现这些数据与社会人口统计学变量如实际年龄或性别相关.尽管如此,流体智能和处理速度与每个等级的不同曲线有不同的关系。我们还发现,执行功能概况与每种认知技能(即,流体智能和处理速度)预测阅读理解和数学成就。这些发现为制定教育预防和干预策略提供了宝贵的见解。
    Using a person-centered approach, we aimed to identify different executive functioning profiles to assess heterogeneity across individuals within the same school grade through latent profile analysis. A sample of 150 Grade 2 (7-8 years old), 150 Grade 6 (11-12 years old), and 150 Grade 10 (15-16 years old) children and adolescents were assessed on 11 different executive tasks representative of the three main executive functioning subcomponents (i.e., inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory), fluid intelligence, processing speed, problem-solving, and reading comprehension. Three different executive functioning profiles of different patterns of interactions based on inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory within and between grades were identified. Moreover, these profiles were differentially related to reading comprehension and mathematical achievement. Second, as expected, we did not find these profiles to be associated with sociodemographic variables such as chronological age or sex. Still, fluid intelligence and processing speed were differentially related to the different profiles at each grade. We also found that the executive functioning profiles interacted with each cognitive skill (i.e., fluid intelligence and processing speed) in predicting reading comprehension and math achievement. These findings provide valuable insights for developing preventive and intervention strategies in education.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    工作记忆(WM)是一种认知系统,其关键作用是暂时掌握和操纵信息。早期研究表明,言语WM通常与左半球(LH)大脑区域有关,而WM中视觉空间信息的处理更具体地取决于右半球(RH)。然而,最近的证据表明,在这些过程中,一个更复杂的网络涉及两个半球的前额叶和顶叶后皮质。不幸的是,以前的病变研究通常只检查一种模态(无论是口头的,或视觉空间)或一个半球,这限制了关于非偏侧半球参与的可能结论。使用基于连接体的病变-症状作图对大样本的左(LBD)和右(RBD)局灶性脑损伤患者,我们研究了灰质损伤和白质断开是否可以预测N-back任务中WM更新的缺陷。患者接受了两种不同模态的WM任务(口头,空间)和认知负荷(1-back,2后卫)。行为结果表明,RBD患者在WM更新方面表现出显著缺陷,无论任务模式或负载如何。这一观察得到了基于全脑体素的分析的支持,揭示了RH中WM缺陷与灰质簇之间的关联。具体来说,包括与Broca区同源的大脑区域在内的右侧额叶皮层的损伤与言语WM缺陷有关,而右下顶叶和颞叶后皮质的损伤则预示着空间WM缺陷。此外,白质分析确定了RH中严重受影响的区域,预测语言和空间WM的缺陷。我们的发现表明,在WM更新中对言语和视觉空间信息的心理操纵依赖于RH的完整性,不管具体的信息类型。
    Working Memory (WM) is a cognitive system whose crucial role is to temporarily hold and manipulate information. Early studies suggest that verbal WM is typically associated with left hemisphere (LH) brain regions, while the processing of visuospatial information in WM more specifically depends on the right hemisphere (RH). However, recent evidence suggests a more complex network involving both hemispheres\' prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in these processes. Unfortunately, previous lesion studies often examined only one modality (either verbal, or visuospatial) or one hemisphere, which limits the possible conclusions regarding non-lateralized hemispheric involvement. Using connectome-based lesion-symptom mapping on a large sample of patients with left (LBD) and right (RBD) focal brain damage, we examined whether gray matter damage and white matter disconnections predict deficits of WM updating in an N-back task. Patients were examined with two WM tasks that differed regarding modality (verbal, spatial) and cognitive load (1-back, 2-back). Behavioral outcomes indicated that RBD patients showed significant deficits in WM updating, regardless of task modality or load. This observation was supported by whole-brain voxel-based analysis, revealing associations between WM deficits and gray matter clusters in the RH. Specifically, damage to the right lateral frontal cortex including the brain region homologous to Broca\'s area was associated with verbal WM deficits, while damage to the right inferior parietal lobe and posterior temporal cortex predicted spatial WM deficits. Additionally, white matter analyses identified severely impacted tracts in the RH, predicting deficits in both verbal and spatial WM. Our findings suggest that the mental manipulation of both verbal and visuospatial information in WM updating relies on the integrity of the RH, irrespective of the specific type of information held in mind.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    几十年来,认知失败问卷(CFQ)已被广泛用作日常生活中主观认知功能的衡量标准。然而,关于它如何与执行职能任务的客观绩效相关的证据是混合的。这些混合结果的一个可能原因是CFQ与执行功能的某些方面具有选择性关系,而不是其他方面。这里,因此,我们根据一个有影响力的执行职能转换框架对任务进行分类,更新,抑制,我们还考虑了持续关注响应任务(SART)作为一个类别,因为它是自定义设计来衡量认知失败。我们综合了大量现有证据,并对这四个类别中的CFQ得分与执行功能任务的客观表现之间的关系进行了四次贝叶斯荟萃分析。结果表明,CFQ评分与SART的客观表现相关(18个效应大小,μ=-.19,BF10=18.03,即有关系的证据比没有关系的证据多18.03倍),更新工作记忆(49个效果大小,μ=-.06,BF10=17.80),和抑制任务(41种效果大小,μ=-.07,BF10=15.40),而没有关于转换的明确证据(34个效应大小,μ=-.06,BF10=.50,即两倍以上的证据表明没有关系)。这表明主观认知功能可以预测至少某些执行功能任务的客观表现。我们讨论了限制最大可观察相关性的方法论和理论因素,并考虑了主观度量与任务绩效提供的相对见解。
    The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) has been widely used as a measure of subjective cognitive function in everyday life for decades. However, the evidence on how it relates to objective performance on executive function tasks is mixed. One possible reason for these mixed results is that the CFQ has selective relationships with some aspects of executive function and not others. Here, therefore, we classified tasks according to an influential framework of executive functions-switching, updating, inhibition, and we also considered the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART) as a category because it was custom designed to gauge cognitive failures. We synthesized a large body of available evidence and performed four Bayesian meta-analyses on the relationship between CFQ scores and objective performance on executive function tasks in these four categories. Results suggested that CFQ scores were associated with objective performance on SART (18 effect sizes, μ = -.19, BF10 = 18.03, i.e., 18.03 times more evidence of a relationship versus no relationship), updating working memory (49 effect sizes, μ = -.06, BF10 = 17.80), and inhibition tasks (41 effect sizes, μ = -.07, BF10 = 15.40), whereas there was not definitive evidence regarding switching (34 effect sizes, μ = -.06, BF10 = .50, i.e., two times greater evidence for no relationship). This suggests that subjective cognitive function can predict objective performance on at least some executive function tasks. We discuss methodological and theoretical factors that constrain the maximum observable correlation and consider the relative insights that subjective measures versus task performance provide.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对唐氏综合症(DS)患者的日常执行功能的研究主要集中在儿童和青少年上。当前的研究检查了DS成年人的日常执行功能。27名DS成年人(年龄在22.00至42.00岁之间)使用执行功能行为评级清单-成人告密者版本(BRIEF-A)进行了测试。调查结果显示,DS成年人在全球行政综合指数(GEC)中表现出更大的损害,工作记忆,计划/组织,相对于规范样本中通常发育中的成年人的任务监测和元认知指数(MI)。DS成年人的所有日常执行功能能力均存在很大差异。调查结果强调了对DS个人的日常执行职能进行全面评估的重要性,以确定问题领域并计划个性化的支持计划。
    Research on everyday executive functions in individuals with Down Syndrome (DS) has focused mostly on children and adolescents. The current study examined everyday executive functions in DS adults. 27 DS adults (aged 22.00 to 42.00 years) were tested using the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Informant Version (BRIEF-A). Findings revealed that DS adults exhibited greater impairments in Global Executive Composite (GEC), Working Memory, Plan/Organise, Task-Monitor and the Metacognition Index (MI) relative to typically developing adults in the normative sample. Substantial variabilities were found in all everyday executive function abilities among DS adults. Findings highlight the importance of making comprehensive assessments of everyday executive functions in DS individuals in order to identify problem areas and plan individualised support programmes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:尽管咖啡消费在世界范围内普遍存在,最近的一项研究表明,咖啡因的急性摄入会对习惯性咖啡因消费者的n-back任务的工作记忆(WM)表现产生负面影响。然而,缺乏双盲,使用空间跨度任务评估咖啡因的空间WM(SWM)效应的安慰剂对照研究。因此,本研究旨在检查急性咖啡因给药的效果(200毫克,PO)关于习惯性咖啡因消费者的SWM和口头WM(VWM)。
    方法:咖啡因对工作记忆(WM)的影响是通过在延迟依赖的条件下(0、4、8和6s),双盲,安慰剂对照研究,包括18名健康参与者。这些数据来自我们之前发表的研究。获得的总分和达到的最高分数是研究的主要结果变量。
    结果:咖啡因对SWM有重大影响(获得的最大值,p=0.013;总分,p=0.007)以与延迟无关的方式。然而,咖啡因对VWM没有显著的主要影响(获得的最大值p=0.82,总评分p=0.56)。
    结论:总体而言,本研究发现与咖啡因改善认知能力的普遍看法相矛盾,并提示在习惯性饮用咖啡/茶的人群中,急性摄入咖啡因可能会损害SWM.
    背景:CT-2018-CTN-02561(治疗用品管理临床试验注册中心)和ACTRN12618001292268(澳大利亚新西兰临床试验注册中心)。
    BACKGROUND: Although coffee consumption is widespread worldwide, a recent study showed that acute intake of caffeine negatively affects working memory (WM) performance on n-back tasks among habitual caffeine consumers. However, there is a scarcity of double-blind, placebo-controlled studies that assess the spatial WM (SWM) effects of caffeine using spatial span tasks. Therefore, the present study aimed examine the effects of acute caffeine administration (200 mg, PO) on SWM and verbal WM (VWM) among habitual caffeine consumers.
    METHODS: The effects of caffeine on working memory (WM) was evaluated through the administration of backward digit span and spatial span tasks under a delay-dependent condition (0, 4, 8, and 6 s) in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study involving 18 healthy participants. This data is derived from our previous published study. The total scores obtained and the maximum scores achieved were the primary outcome variables of the study.
    RESULTS: Caffeine had a significant impact on SWM (maximum obtained, p = 0.013; for total scored, p = 0.007) in a delay-independent manner. However, there were no significant main effects of caffeine on VWM (p = 0.82 for maximum obtained, p = 0.56 for total scored).
    CONCLUSIONS: Overall, the present findings contradict the commonly held belief that caffeine improves cognitive performance and suggest that acute administration of caffeine may impair SWM in habitual coffee/tea drinkers.
    BACKGROUND: CT-2018-CTN-02561 (Therapeutic Goods Administration Clinical Trial Registry) and ACTRN12618001292268 (The Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)中的工作记忆与皮质功能网络连接(CFNC)密切相关,比如额叶之间的异常连接,temporal,枕骨皮质和其他大脑区域。低强度经颅超声刺激(TUS)具有无创性优点,高空间分辨率,和高穿透深度,可以改善多动症的记忆行为。然而,它如何调节ADHD中的CFNC以及改善ADHD中工作记忆行为的CFNC机制尚不清楚。在这项研究中,我们观察到ADHD大鼠的工作记忆障碍,在工作记忆任务期间,建立CFNC的变化与行为状态之间的对应关系。具体来说,我们注意到ADHD大鼠在执行工作记忆任务时CFNC的信息传递和处理能力异常.这些异常表现在特定领域的网络集成能力,以及CFNC的信息流和功能分化。此外,我们的发现表明TUS通过调节信息传递有效增强ADHD大鼠的工作记忆能力,processing,和集成能力,同时调整CFNC的信息流和功能差异化。此外,我们解释了CFNC机制,通过该机制TUS改善ADHD中的工作记忆。总之,这些发现表明CFNCs在ADHD的工作记忆行为中很重要。
    Working memory in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is closely related to cortical functional network connectivity (CFNC), such as abnormal connections between the frontal, temporal, occipital cortices and with other brain regions. Low-intensity transcranial ultrasound stimulation (TUS) has the advantages of non-invasiveness, high spatial resolution, and high penetration depth and can improve ADHD memory behavior. However, how it modulates CFNC in ADHD and the CFNC mechanism that improves working memory behavior in ADHD remain unclear. In this study, we observed working memory impairment in ADHD rats, establishing a corresponding relationship between changes in CFNCs and the behavioral state during the working memory task. Specifically, we noted abnormalities in the information transmission and processing capabilities of CFNC in ADHD rats while performing working memory tasks. These abnormalities manifested in the network integration ability of specific areas, as well as the information flow and functional differentiation of CFNC. Furthermore, our findings indicate that TUS effectively enhances the working memory ability of ADHD rats by modulating information transmission, processing, and integration capabilities, along with adjusting the information flow and functional differentiation of CFNC. Additionally, we explain the CFNC mechanism through which TUS improves working memory in ADHD. In summary, these findings suggest that CFNCs are important in working memory behaviors in ADHD.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在研究工作记忆(WM)时,“槽模型”和“资源模型”是用于描述信息保留如何发生的两个主要理论。插槽模型表明,WM容量由一定数量的可用于信息存储的预定义插槽组成。该理论解释了在信息召回期间存在二进制条件,其中信息要么完全保留在插槽中,要么被遗忘。资源模型具有基于分辨率的方法,建议能够在WM容量中的多个项目之间分配的连续资源。最近出现了混合动力模型,这表明WM可能不会严格遵循一个模型。因此,为了理解WM评估中使用最广泛的两种范式之间的关系,我们在两个不同的心理物理学任务中实施了相关评估,具有顺序条显示的模拟回忆范例和具有棋盘刺激的延迟匹配样本(DMS)任务。我们的研究揭示了WM在DMS任务中的表现与召回错误之间的显著相关性,精度,以及顺序范式中错误的来源。总的来说,研究结果强调了在理解WM过程中考虑这两项任务的重要性,因为他们揭示了理论和用于评估WM能力的任务中的重叠元素,从而阐明了插槽和资源模型之间的争论。
    When studying the working memory (WM), the \'slot model\' and the \'resource model\' are two main theories used to describe how information retention occurs. The slot model shows that WM capacity consists of a certain number of predefined slots available for information storage. This theory explains that there is a binary condition during information recall in which information is either wholly maintained within a slot or forgotten. The resource model has a resolution-based approach, suggesting a continuous resource able to be distributed among a number of items in WM capacity. Recently hybrid models have been introduced, suggesting that WM may not strictly conform to only one model. Accordingly, to understand the relationship between two of the most widely used paradigms in WM evaluation, we implemented a correlational assessment in two different psychophysics tasks, an analog recall paradigm with sequential bar presentation and a delayed match-to-sample (DMS) task with checkerboard stimuli. Our study revealed significant correlations between WM performance in the DMS task and recall error, precision, and sources of errors in the sequential paradigm. Overall, the findings emphasize the importance of considering both tasks in understanding WM processes, as they shed light on the debate between the slot and resource models by revealing overlapping elements in both theories and the tasks used to evaluate WM capacity.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尽管许多关于言语工作记忆的研究已经调查了语音循环的能力和排练作为维持记忆痕迹的一个核心过程的有效性,从长期记忆中重建记忆痕迹,称为redintegration,研究不那么彻底。这尤其适用于有特殊教育学习需求和轻度和边缘性智力障碍(MBID)的学生群体。在之前的研究中,我们发现redintegration的有效性和词汇量之间存在不同的发展关系,反直觉地表明,MBID的学生倾向于表现出更高词汇量的较低效率的redintegration。然而,图片命名任务中的差异项功能(DIF)可能会导致结果偏差。因此,当前的研究是对词汇测量中DIF的这种相互作用控制的重新分析。为此,图片命名任务(k=95)的项目通过Rasch模型进行分析,排除k=29个有偏见的项目。得到的校正词汇得分用于预测redintegration有效性,比较有和没有MBID的学生。互动仍然很重要,支持最初的发现,即MBID的学生具有不同的发展模式,并且在重建工作记忆中的痕迹时,不太能够充分利用不断增长的词汇。讨论了该结果对理解MBID和进一步研究方向的意义。
    While numerous studies on verbal working memory have investigated the capacity of the phonological loop and the effectiveness of rehearsal as one core process for maintaining the memory trace, the reconstruction of the memory trace from long-term memory, called redintegration, has been studied less thoroughly. This holds particularly for the population of students with special educational learning needs and mild and borderline intellectual disabilities (MBID). In a previous study, we found a differential developmental relation between the effectiveness of redintegration and vocabulary size, counter-intuitively suggesting that students with MBID tend to show less effective redintegration with higher vocabulary size. However, differential item functioning (DIF) in the picture naming task may have biased the result. Therefore, the current study is a re-analysis of this interaction controlling for DIF in the vocabulary measure. To this end, the items of the picture naming task (k = 95) were analyzed through a Rasch model, and k = 29 biased items were excluded. The resulting corrected vocabulary score was used to predict the redintegration effectiveness, comparing students with and without MBID. The interaction remains significant, supporting the original finding that students with MBID have a differential developmental pattern and are less able to make adequate use of a growing vocabulary when reconstructing traces in their working memory. Implications of this result for the understanding of MBID and further research directions are discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:工作记忆(WM)障碍是中风后的常见现象;但是,其康复管理研究较少。本系统综述和荟萃分析旨在定量综合分析计算机化认知训练(CCT)和经颅直流电刺激(tDCS)对卒中后个体WM跨度的影响。
    方法:在PubMed,Embase,Scopus,CochraneLibrary专注于随机对照试验,测试CCT和tDCS对治疗后卒中患者与未治疗对照组的影响。诸如数字跨度向前/向后和视觉跨度向前测试之类的神经心理学工具定义了WM跨度的结果。在使用Cochrane偏差风险工具提取研究特征和质量评估后,我们使用标准化均值差异进行了荟萃分析和荟萃回归.
    结果:搜索产生了4142篇文章,其中9个(N=461)符合纳入标准。在CCT的情况下,我们发现数字跨度后向测试(Z=2.65,P=0.008;95%CI[0.10,0.67])和视觉跨度前向测试性能(Z=3.05,P=0.002;95%CI[0.15,0.69])显着改善,而对于tDCS,我们找不到足够数量的研究进行分析.此外,meta回归中未发现显著的调节因素.
    结论:结论:CCT似乎是增强冲程后WM跨度性能的合适选择。然而,由于研究数量有限,需要进一步的研究来调查tDCS的效果.
    背景:根据PRISMA(系统评论和荟萃分析的首选报告)标准和PROSPERO注册方案(ID:CRD42023387182)进行荟萃分析。
    BACKGROUND: Working memory (WM) impairment is a common phenomenon after stroke; however, its management in rehabilitation is less researched. This systematic review and meta-analysis aimed to provide a quantitative synthesis of the impact of computerised cognitive training (CCT) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on WM span in post-stroke individuals.
    METHODS: The literature search in PubMed, Embase, Scopus, and Cochrane Library focused on randomized controlled trials testing the effect of CCT and tDCS on treated stroke patients as compared to untreated controls. Neuropsychological instruments such as Digit Span Forward/Backward and Visual Span Forward Tests defined the outcome of WM span. After extracting study characteristics and quality assessment using the Cochrane Risk of Bias Tool, we conducted a meta-analysis and meta-regression using standardised mean differences.
    RESULTS: The search yielded 4142 articles, nine of which (N = 461) fulfilled the inclusion criteria. In the case of CCT, we found significant improvement in Digit Span Backward Test (Z = 2.65, P = 0.008; 95% CI [0.10, 0.67]) and Visual Span Forward Test performance (Z = 3.05, P = 0.002; 95% CI [0.15, 0.69]), while for tDCS, we could not find a sufficient number of studies for the analysis. Furthermore, no significant moderating factor was found in the meta-regression.
    CONCLUSIONS: In conclusion, CCT appears to be a suitable choice to enhance WM span performance after stroke. However, further research is needed to investigate the effect of tDCS due to the limited number of studies.
    BACKGROUND: The meta-analysis was conducted according to PRISMA (Preferred Reporting of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) standards with a PROSPERO registration protocol (ID: CRD42023387182).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究分析了主要执行功能(EF)成分水平的差异(如抑制、工作记忆,和认知灵活性)在俄罗斯和日本学龄前儿童中。该研究涉及102名5-6.9岁的儿童:51名来自俄罗斯的儿童和51名来自日本的儿童。102名儿童中,48名是男孩,54名女孩。研究发现,俄罗斯儿童的认知灵活性水平高于日本儿童,抑制水平低于日本儿童。男孩EF比较的结果表明,俄罗斯男孩的认知和身体抑制水平低于日本男孩。还表明,俄罗斯女孩的认知灵活性明显高于日本女孩,而认知抑制则低于日本女孩。从所研究的两个国家可能存在的文化差异的角度讨论了所获得的结果,这表现在成年人对儿童的期望中。
    This study analyzed differences in level of main executive function (EF) components (such as inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility) among Russian and Japanese preschoolers. The study involved 102 children of 5-6.9 years old: 51 child from Russia and 51 child from Japan. Out of 102 children 48 were boys and 54 girls. It was found that the cognitive flexibility level in Russian children is higher and inhibition level is lower than in Japanese children. The results of the boys\' EF comparison showed that boys from Russia have lower cognitive and physical inhibition levels than boys from Japan. Also it was shown that cognitive flexibility in Russian girls is significantly higher and cognitive inhibition is lower than in Japanese girls. The results obtained are discussed from the point of view of possible cultural differences in the two countries studied, which are manifested in the expectations of adults from children.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号